Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T11:15:51.497Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impulsivity and cognitive distortions in pathological gamblers attending the UK National Problem Gambling Clinic: a preliminary report

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 June 2011

R. Michalczuk
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK
H. Bowden-Jones
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, Imperial College, London, UK
A. Verdejo-Garcia
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Granada, Spain
L. Clark*
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr L. Clark, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Downing St, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Pathological gambling (PG) is a form of behavioural addiction that has been associated with elevated impulsivity and also cognitive distortions in the processing of chance, probability and skill. We sought to assess the relationship between the level of cognitive distortions and state and trait measures of impulsivity in treatment-seeking pathological gamblers.

Method

Thirty pathological gamblers attending the National Problem Gambling Clinic, the first National Health Service clinic for gambling problems in the UK, were compared with 30 healthy controls in a case-control design. Cognitive distortions were assessed using the Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS). Trait impulsivity was assessed using the UPPS-P, which includes scales of urgency, the tendency to be impulsive in positive or negative mood states. Delay discounting rates were taken as a state measure of impulsive choice.

Results

Pathological gamblers had elevated impulsivity on several UPPS-P subscales but effect sizes were largest (Cohen's d>1.4) for positive and negative urgency. The pathological gamblers also displayed higher levels of gambling distortions, and elevated preference for immediate rewards, compared to controls. Within the pathological gamblers, there was a strong relationship between the preference for immediate rewards and the level of cognitive distortions (R2=0.41).

Conclusions

Impulsive choice in the gamblers was correlated with the level of gambling distortions, and we hypothesize that an impulsive decision-making style may increase the acceptance of erroneous beliefs during gambling play.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011 The online version of this article is published within an Open Access environment subject to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence <http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/>. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.

Introduction

Gambling is a recreational activity that becomes disordered in approximately 0.9% of the British population using DSM criteria (Wardle et al. Reference Wardle, Moody, Spence, Orford, Volberg, Jotangia, Griffiths, Hussey and Dobbie2010). There is extensive clinical and pathophysiological overlap between pathological gambling (PG) and the substance use disorders (Potenza, Reference Potenza2006; Frascella et al. Reference Frascella, Potenza, Brown and Childress2010), prompting a probable reclassification of PG among the addictions in the forthcoming DSM-V (Mitzner et al. Reference Mitzner, Whelan and Meyers2010; Bowden-Jones & Clark, in press). PG is thought to arise through a combination of biological, social and psychological risk factors (Blaszczynski & Nower, Reference Blaszczynski and Nower2002; Sharpe, Reference Sharpe2002). One of the defining features of gamblers' cognition is the tendency to overestimate the chances of winning, due to variety of cognitive distortions in the processing of chance, skill and probability (Ladouceur & Walker, Reference Ladouceur, Walker and Salkovskis1996; Clark, Reference Clark2010). Indeed, it is unclear whether these distortions have an obvious parallel in substance use disorders (Xian et al. Reference Xian, Shah, Phillips, Scherrer, Volberg and Eisen2008). Using psychometric measures of gambling distortions, such as the Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS; Raylu & Oei, Reference Raylu and Oei2004a) or the Gambling Beliefs Questionnaire (GBQ; Steenbergh et al. Reference Steenbergh, Meyers, May and Whelan2002), several studies have reported elevated levels of distorted cognitions in individuals with disordered gambling compared to those without gambling problems (Miller & Currie, Reference Miller and Currie2008; Emond & Marmurek, Reference Emond and Marmurek2010; Myrseth et al. Reference Myrseth, Brunborg and Eidem2010). These cognitions are attenuated by treatment (Breen et al. Reference Breen, Kruedelbach and Walker2001), and higher baseline scores predicted poorer outcome in a Gambler's Anonymous programme (Oei & Gordon, Reference Oei and Gordon2008).

Studies using personality measures and neurocognitive tests have also highlighted changes in impulsivity in PG. Scores on the Barratt Impulsivity Scale and the Eysenck Impulsivity Venturesomeness Empathy (IVE) scale reliably increased in case-control studies (Blaszczynski et al. Reference Blaszczynski, Steel and McConaghy1997; Petry, Reference Petry2001b; Nower et al. Reference Nower, Derevensky and Gupta2004; Lawrence et al. Reference Lawrence, Luty, Bogdan, Sahakian and Clark2009a), and prospective studies have confirmed that high impulsivity during adolescence predicts later gambling problems (Vitaro et al. Reference Vitaro, Arseneault and Tremblay1999; Slutske et al. Reference Slutske, Caspi, Moffitt and Poulton2005). Rather than being a unidimensional construct, impulsivity is being increasingly viewed as a constellation of traits, including a lack of planning or forethought, reduced perseverance and the seeking of novel or intense sensory experiences (Evenden, Reference Evenden1999; Verdejo-Garcia et al. Reference Verdejo-Garcia, Lawrence and Clark2008). Recent research has also identified a fourth impulsivity component, labelled ‘urgency’: the tendency to engage in impulsive acts during intense mood states. This construct was initially identified by Whiteside & Lynam (Reference Whiteside and Lynam2001) using a composite of the Barratt and Eysenck scales and the Zuckerman Sensation Seeking Scale, and subsequent work by Cyders, Smith and colleagues (Cyders & Smith, Reference Cyders and Smith2008b) has distinguished positive and negative aspects of urgency. The urgency construct is profoundly relevant to gambling behaviour, as it is known that many gamblers are motivated to gamble to alleviate states of depression, stress or boredom (Jacobs, Reference Jacobs1986), but simultaneously, positive mood (perhaps associated with hypomania or dramatic wins) may also prompt gambling sprees (Cummins et al. Reference Cummins, Nadorff and Kelly2009; Lloyd et al. Reference Lloyd, Doll, Hawton, Dutton, Geddes, Goodwin and Rogers2010). Although positive and negative urgency are moderately correlated and both are related to a range of risky behaviours (Cyders & Smith, Reference Cyders and Smith2008b), there are some differential effects. Positive urgency predicted longitudinal increases in gambling behaviour across university (Cyders & Smith, Reference Cyders and Smith2008a), and changes in risk taking and alcohol consumption following positive mood induction (Cyders et al. Reference Cyders, Zapolski, Combs, Settles, Fillmore and Smith2010), whereas negative urgency is associated with bulimic symptoms (Anestis et al. Reference Anestis, Selby, Fink and Joiner2007) and tobacco cravings (Billieux et al. Reference Billieux, Van Der Linden and Ceschi2007).

Impulsivity can also be assessed in the laboratory using a variety of neurocognitive tasks. The delay discounting paradigm assesses impulsive choice by presenting a series of decisions between a smaller reward available soon (or immediately), and larger rewards available after a longer delay (Bickel & Marsch, Reference Bickel and Marsch2001; Reynolds, Reference Reynolds2006). Indifference points obtained from varying one of the decision parameters (e.g. the delay to the larger reward) can be fitted to a hyperbolic discounting curve, allowing derivation of a parameter k that indicates the steepness of the discounting function. Impulsive subjects show enhanced preference for the smaller immediate rewards and steeper discounting of delayed outcomes (i.e. higher k values), and several studies have described such a tendency in treatment-seeking and community groups with disordered gambling (Petry, Reference Petry2001a; Alessi & Petry, Reference Alessi and Petry2003; Dixon et al. Reference Dixon, Marley and Jacobs2003; Ledgerwood et al. Reference Ledgerwood, Alessi, Phoenix and Petry2009).

Although impulsivity and gambling-related cognitive distortions have been described in pathological gamblers, the links between these constructs has received minimal attention. In an undergraduate sample comprising a range of gambling problems, the Eysenck Impulsivity score predicted higher scores on the GBQ (MacKillop et al. Reference MacKillop, Anderson, Castelda, Mattson and Donovick2006), and a similar relationship was reported between Barratt scores and a measure of cognitive distortions that apply across many forms of psychopathology, such as personalization of negative events and all-or-none thinking (Mobini et al. Reference Mobini, Grant, Kass and Yeomans2007). We reasoned that an impulsive style of decision making may increase a gambler's tendency to accept erroneous beliefs about gambling over more ‘rational’ alternative interpretations that reflect more accurately the nature of chance. As such, the primary focus of the present study was to assess the degree of coupling between gambling-related cognitive distortions and state and trait indices of impulsivity, in participants with PG. Gamblers were recruited through the National Problem Gambling Clinic in London, which is the first (and only) National Health Service treatment facility for gambling in the UK. Given the limited treatment facilities for gambling prior to the opening of the clinic in 2008, there are few data available on the clinical characteristics and preferred forms of gambling of treatment-seeking gamblers in the UK, and it is vital to study gambling at a national level given the pronounced cultural and legislative heterogeneity in gambling practices across countries (Raylu & Oei, Reference Raylu and Oei2004b). A secondary objective was therefore to replicate prior findings, primarily from Australian and North American studies, of increased levels of gambling distortions, self-reported impulsivity, and discounting of delayed rewards in PG.

Method

Participants

Pathological gamblers (n=30) were recruited from the National Problem Gambling Clinic (28 males, two females; mean age=40.1 years, s.d.=12.3, range 21–60) and compared with community-recruited healthy controls (28 males, two females; mean age=35.8 years, s.d.=12.2, range 19–60). The protocol was approved by the Cambridge Local Research Ethics Committee (09/H0305/77) and all volunteers provided written informed consent. Participants were reimbursed for their time and travel expenses. The two groups did not differ in age (t=1.33, p=0.187), years of education (t=1.42, p=0.160) or errors committed on the National Adult Reading Test (NART; Nelson & Willison, Reference Nelson and Willison1991), which provides an estimate of verbal IQ (t=1.31, p=0.190). Inclusion criteria for the PG group were: DSM-IV diagnostic criteria for PG, and age 18–60 years. PG was confirmed using the Massachusetts Gambling Screen (MAGS; Shaffer et al. Reference Shaffer, Labrie, Scanlan and Cummings1994), which indexes the DSM-IV criteria, in conjunction with a score of ⩾8 on the Problem Gambling Severity Index (PGSI; Ferris & Wynne, Reference Ferris and Wynne2001). Exclusion criteria for both groups were: history of neurological illness, previous psychiatric hospitalization, current pharmacotherapy and significant physical illness. Among the controls, 27 had a PGSI score of 0 and had never gambled; two scored 1 on the PGSI and one scored 2.

The PG participants were recruited as a convenience sample, and comprised three subgroups in terms of their treatment profile: 15 gamblers (50%) were tested prior to receiving any treatment, seven (23%) were currently receiving treatment and eight (27%) had recently completed a 10-session course of cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT). In the PG group, the presence of other current and lifetime diagnoses of mental illness was assessed by a semi-structured interview using the ICD-10, in conjunction with the computerized version of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (e-MINI; Medical Outcome Systems, USA) (Sheehan et al. Reference Sheehan, Lecrubier, Sheehan, Amorim, Janavs, Weiller, Hergueta, Baker and Dunbar1998). Any discrepancies between these measures were resolved through discussion with the lead psychiatrist (H.B.J.) and further review of the medical records. Ten cases met current or lifetime diagnoses of major depressive disorder (six current, four lifetime). Two cases recorded current generalized anxiety disorder. For substance use disorders, one case met lifetime cannabis dependence, one case lifetime alcohol dependence, and two cases current alcohol dependence. The healthy controls recorded no lifetime or current mental health problems on the e-MINI screen.

The various forms of gambling that were played were assessed with a modified version of item 1 from the South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS; Lesieur & Blume, Reference Lesieur and Blume1987); in addition, the PG participants were asked which form they considered most problematic for them. Sixty per cent of the PG group considered fixed odds betting terminals (FOBTs) to represent their problematic form of gambling. The other preferred games were sports betting (16%), internet poker or blackjack (7%), slot machines (10%), and casino games (7%). Convergent data were obtained from games played once a week or more (SOGS item 1) (individual participants may endorse more than one form, so the total does not sum to 100%): gaming machines (59%), betting on horses (52%), lottery (41%), sports betting (38%), card games (24%), casino games (17%), bingo (7%), bowling/pool (7%) or stock market (3%). In examining the relationship status of the participants, the modal status in the PG group was single (50%), 30% were in a steady relationship, 13% were married and 7% were divorced. Among controls, the modal status was married (47%), with 23% single, 17% in a steady relationship, 10% divorced and 3% widowed. Both groups were predominately of British nationality (PG: 97%; controls: 88%). Employment status was similar in both groups; in the PG group, 61% were employed full time, 7% students, 10% self-employed, 3% retired and 17% unemployed. In the controls, 43% were employed full time, 17% students, 3% self-employed, 17% unemployed and 13% in part-time employment. The PG group rated the largest amount of money gambled in a single day, on a four-point scale: £10–£100 (6%), £100–£1000 (17%), £1000–£10 000 (60%), over £10 000 (17%). Current level of debt ranged from £0 to £80 000: 23% had no debt due to bail-out; in the gamblers with current debt, this debt ranged from £600 to £80 000 (mean £25 557).

Procedure

All participants attended a single test session in a quiet laboratory or treatment room, where they completed a cognitive assessment and questionnaire measures of clinical and personality measures.

Kirby Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ; Kirby et al. Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999)

The MCQ is a measure of delay discounting comprising 27 hypothetical choices between a smaller reward available immediately versus a larger reward available at some point in the future (e.g. ‘Would you prefer £15 today or £35 in 13 days?’). The larger reward varies across three levels of magnitude: small (£25–£35), medium (£50–£60) and large (£75–£80), deriving three k values that were averaged for the overall discounting rate. Given that the k parameter assumes an underlying hyperbolic discounting curve, an area under the curve (AUC) value was also extracted for each participant, as a parameter that is neutral to the underlying discounting function (Myerson et al. Reference Myerson, Green and Warusawitharana2001).

UPPS-P Impulsive Behaviour Scale (Cyders et al. Reference Cyders, Smith, Spillane, Fischer, Annus and Peterson2007)

This is a 59-item self-report questionnaire using a Likert scale from 1 (I agree strongly) to 4 (I disagree strongly) to assess five impulsivity subscales: Negative Urgency (e.g. ‘Sometimes when I feel bad, I can't seem to stop what I am doing even though it is making me feel worse’); Positive Urgency (e.g. ‘When overjoyed, I feel like I can't stop myself from going overboard’); (lack of) Planning (e.g. ‘I usually make up my mind through careful reasoning’ – negative loading); (lack of) Perseverance (e.g. ‘I finish what I start’ – negative loading); and Sensation Seeking (e.g. ‘I would enjoy the sensation of skiing very fast down a high mountain slope’).

GRCS (Raylu & Oei, 2004a)

The GRCS a 23-item self-report questionnaire using a seven-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) to assess five subscales: Predictive Control (e.g. ‘Losses when gambling are bound to be followed by a series of wins’); Illusion of Control (e.g. ‘I have specific rituals and behaviours that increase my chances of winning’); Interpretive Bias (e.g. ‘Relating my winnings to my skill and ability makes me continue gambling’); Gambling Expectancies (e.g. ‘Gambling makes things seem better’); and Inability to Stop (e.g. ‘I'm not strong enough to stop gambling’).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were implemented in SPSS version 17 (SPSS Inc., USA). A natural log transformation was applied to the discounting k values, to improve suitability for parametric analysis. Visual inspection of the discounting data led to exclusion of three participants' scores on this measure (one PG, two controls), who uniformly selected the larger reward across all delay (i.e. the pre-specified delays and amounts on the MCQ failed to isolate indifference points for these participants). Group comparisons on the GRCS and UPPS-P were performed by entering the component subscales into a multivariate ANOVA. A mixed-model ANOVA was used to compare k discounting values, with magnitude of the larger reward as a within-subjects factor. Within the PG group, Pearson's coefficients were used to assess univariate relationships between the UPPS-P, GRCS and Kirby MCQ variables, and a stepwise multiple linear regression assessed possible additive contributions of the UPPS-P and Kirby MCQ variables to predicting the level of cognitive distortions (with GRCS as the dependent variable).

Results

The PG group displayed a higher level of gambling-related cognitions (F 5,54=13.1, p<0.001), with significant differences on all of the GRCS subscales (see Table 1). On the UPPS-P, the PG group displayed elevated impulsivity (F 5,54=10.0, p<0.001), with significant differences on all subscales with the exception of Sensation Seeking (see Table 1). Effect sizes (Cohen's d) revealed stronger effects for the two urgency subscales (d=1.46–1.77) in comparison to the narrow impulsivity subscales (d=0.61–0.78).

Table 1. Mean scores (s.d.) on the GRCS and UPPS-P Impulsivity Scale in the participants with pathological gambling (PG) and healthy controls

GRCS, Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale; s.d., standard deviation.

ANOVA degrees of freedom 1, 59.

* p<0.05, ** p<0.005.

Choice behaviour on the delay discounting scale was analysed using a mixed-model ANOVA of group (PG, controls) by magnitude (small, medium, large). The PG group displayed higher k values than healthy controls (main effect of group: F 1,55=8.02, p=0.006), indicating elevated impulsive choice in the PG group (see Fig. 1). All participants showed higher k values (steeper discounting) for smaller, compared to larger, delayed rewards (F 1,55=23.2, p<0.001). This is consistent with a typical ‘magnitude effect’ (e.g. Chapman, Reference Chapman1996), and suggests that the PG participants are using similar cognitive mechanisms for making these inter-temporal decisions to the healthy controls. There was no interaction of group by magnitude (F 1,55=0.894, p=0.412). In addition, the PG and healthy controls differed on the AUC measure for the discounting function (t 55=2.3, p=0.025), a parameter that does not assume any underlying mathematical function. The AUC values are displayed in Table 2, alongside the untransformed k scores for comparison with previous studies with the MCQ.

Fig. 1. Delay discounting (ln k) on the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ), in patients with pathological gambling (PG) and healthy controls (HC). Errors bars indicate standard error of the mean.

Table 2. Untransformed delay discounting measures on the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ), in the participants with pathological gambling (PG) and healthy controls

AUC, Area under the curve.

Values given as mean (standard deviation).

We examined univariate associations between delay discounting (average k), UPPS-P impulsivity and GRCS scores in the PG group. In keeping with previous work (e.g. Kirby et al. Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999; Mobini et al. Reference Mobini, Grant, Kass and Yeomans2007; Koff & Lucas, Reference Koff and Lucas2011), moderate associations were observed between the delay discounting rates and self-reported impulsivity (UPPS-P), with the highest coefficients observed on Lack of Planning (r=0.376, p=0.044) and Lack of Perseverance (r=0.382, p=0.041). A strong correlation was observed between impulsive choice on the delay discounting scale and the level of gambling cognitions (see Fig. 2), explaining 41% of the variance in the total GRCS score. Each of the GRCS subscale correlations was statistically significant (see Table 3). In light of a previous study showing that gambling distortions are attenuated by psychological treatment (Breen et al. Reference Breen, Kruedelbach and Walker2001), we observed no significant difference in the GRCS scores in the three PG subgroups at different stages of treatment: pre-treatment group, mean GRCS=74.6 (s.d.=20.9); during treatment group, mean GRCS=71.4 (s.d.=37.2); post-treatment group, mean GRCS=59.5 (s.d.=30.6, F 2,27=0.78, p=0.469). Notably, the correlation between the discount rate and the GRCS total remained highly significant after controlling for stage of treatment (partial coefficient r=0.612, p=0.001).

Fig. 2. Delay discounting (ln k scores, averaged across the three magnitude levels) is correlated significantly with total score on the Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS), in the pathological gamblers.

Table 3. Univariate correlations (Pearson's r) between the delay discounting (Kirby k; n=29), Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS) (n=30) and UPPS-P (n=30) in the pathological gamblers

* p<0.05, ** p<0.005.

UPPS-P impulsivity was less clearly associated with the level of cognitive distortions in the PG group: Negative and Positive Urgency were both associated with Illusion of Control, and Positive Urgency was additionally correlated with the Gambling Expectancies and Inability to Stop subscales, and the GRCS total (see Table 3). To test whether the UPPS-P urgency scales added significantly to the discounting rates in the prediction of gambling distortions, a stepwise multiple regression model was run with the total GRCS score as the dependent variable, and the delay discounting rate, Positive Urgency and Negative Urgency as predictors. The k score was the only variable retained in the model as a significant predictor of the level of cognitive distortions (R 2=0.415, overall F 1,27=19.1, p<0.001, kβ=0.64). (The same conclusion was drawn using a hierarchical regression with the discounting rate entered at the first step and the Urgency scores entered at the second step: there was no significant change in the R 2 between the two steps; overall R 2=0.481.) We also explored the relationships between delay discounting (average k) and the GRCS score and UPPS-P subscales in the healthy controls. The total GRCS scores displayed moderate variability in the controls (minimum=23, maximum=60) and, as in the PG, impulsive choice on the delay discounting measure was predictive of the level of gambling distortions (r=0.431, p=0.022). The correlations between delay discounting and the UPPS-P subscales were not significant in the controls, although the coefficients for Negative (r=0.20) and Positive (r=0.34) Urgency, and Lack of Planning (r=0.21), were comparable to the strength of relationships observed in previous studies (Kirby et al. Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999; Mobini et al. Reference Mobini, Grant, Kass and Yeomans2007; Koff & Lucas, Reference Koff and Lucas2011).

Discussion

The present study is the first UK study to describe clinical and psychological data in treatment-seeking PG. A convenience sample of 30 gamblers recruited from the National Problem Gambling Clinic were predominantly male, well-educated (48% had entered higher education), and typically in full-time employment (61%). As expected, these gamblers participated in a range of games, with the preferred form of gambling being FOBTs in some 60% of the sample. These gaming machines (now known as category B2 gaming machines) are characterized by high stakes, high jackpots, and a rapid rate of play, and are a relatively new arrival on the UK gambling landscape. Although the national prevalence of FOBT play was low (4%) in the 2010 British Gambling Prevalence Survey (Wardle et al. Reference Wardle, Moody, Spence, Orford, Volberg, Jotangia, Griffiths, Hussey and Dobbie2010), FOBT play was the third most common form of gambling among the problem gamblers recruited in that survey (in 9%). Our data further highlight the attractiveness of these games in a treatment-seeking group with PG.

The major finding in the present dataset was of a relationship between impulsive choice on the delay discounting task and the level of gambling-related cognitive distortions. This was a particularly strong effect in the PG group, where discounting scores explained 41% of the variability in gambling distortions, and it was also observed at a significant level (19% shared variance) in the healthy controls. Group differences in impulsivity, and inaccurate beliefs about skill, chance and probability, are widely recognized in the literature on problem gambling, but the relationship between these putative aetiological mechanisms has received little attention. Our findings support a previous study in student gamblers (MacKillop et al. Reference MacKillop, Anderson, Castelda, Mattson and Donovick2006), where the GBQ was correlated moderately (r=0.40) with Eysenck Impulsivity scores. We extend these findings into a treatment-seeking PG sample, and demonstrate that a state measure of impulsive choice is the stronger predictor of gambling distortions than self-reported impulsivity. This close linkage illustrates that gamblers with more ‘myopic’ (i.e. focused on the present) decision making and a reduced capacity to defer gratification are more susceptible to the diverse range of complex distortions that occur during play, such as beliefs in superstitions and rituals (GRCS Illusion of Control), the failure to appreciate independence of turns (GRCS Predictive Control), and expectancies that gambling will be exciting and/or relieve negative affect (GRCS Gambling Expectancies) (Toneatto et al. Reference Toneatto, Blitz-Miller, Calderwood, Dragonetti and Tsanos1997; Raylu & Oei, Reference Raylu and Oei2004a). Indeed, delay discounting was significantly associated with all five of the GRCS subscale scores in the PG group. Many of these distortions can also be elicited in healthy, non-problem gamblers (Clark, Reference Clark2010), and we saw that discounting rates were also predictive of GRCS scores in the healthy controls. We have reported previously that individual differences on the GRCS predicted neural responses to gambling ‘near-misses’ in healthy non-gamblers (Clark et al. Reference Clark, Lawrence, Astley-Jones and Gray2009).

We were able to replicate several group differences reported previously in Australian and North American studies. First, the PG group reported more cognitive distortions on the GRCS, with significant group differences on each of the five subscales (Raylu & Oei, Reference Raylu and Oei2004a; Emond & Marmurek, Reference Emond and Marmurek2010). Prospective data do not yet exist to arbitrate whether these cognitions predate the onset of problem gambling, or occur as a consequence of long-term gambling. Second, the PG group displayed elevated impulsivity on the UPPS-P questionnaire. Significant differences were observed on the two subscales of ‘narrow’ impulsivity (Lack of Premeditation and Lack of Perseverance) and also on the two Urgency subscales, but the effect sizes for the Urgency subscales were considerably larger. This is the first study to differentiate Positive Urgency and Negative Urgency in treatment-seeking PG, and our findings clearly emphasize the relevance of these constructs to PG. By inference, impulsive acts in problem gamblers may predominantly arise through an interaction with current affective state, perhaps through impaired emotion regulation mechanisms (Billieux et al. Reference Billieux, Gay, Rochat and Van Der Linden2010; Cyders et al. Reference Cyders, Zapolski, Combs, Settles, Fillmore and Smith2010). Although Positive and Negative Urgency scales were correlated in the present data, it remains to be seen whether positive or negative mood states represent distinct pathways to risk taking in individual gamblers (Blaszczynski & Nower, Reference Blaszczynski and Nower2002). We observed no group differences in sensation seeking, consistent with some (Blaszczynski et al. Reference Blaszczynski, Wilson and McConaghy1986; Parke et al. Reference Parke, Griffiths and Irwing2004; Ledgerwood et al. Reference Ledgerwood, Alessi, Phoenix and Petry2009) but not all (Cunningham-Williams et al. Reference Cunningham-Williams, Grucza, Cottler, Womack, Books, Przybeck, Spitznagel and Cloninger2005) previous studies in PG. This may reflect the exclusively male sample (Nower et al. Reference Nower, Derevensky and Gupta2004), or it is possible that sensation seeking can be decomposed further into subfactors, such as boredom proneness, some of which may be associated with PG (Fortune & Goodie, Reference Fortune and Goodie2010). Alternatively, sensation seeking may dispose recreational engagement with gambling rather than the transition to disordered gambling, and may therefore have less relevance to adult treatment-seeking groups (cf. van Leeuwen et al. Reference van Leeuwen, Creemers, Verhulst, Ormel and Huizink2010).

Finally, there were significant differences in delay discounting between the two groups, in addition to the individual differences seen on this measure. Both groups showed the standard ‘magnitude effect’ on the discounting task, such that large rewards of a relatively higher value (£75–£85) were discounted less than large rewards of a relatively smaller value (£25–£35). This is a consistent finding in the behavioural economics literature (Chapman, Reference Chapman1996; Green & Myerson, Reference Green and Myerson2004), and, as in the original study using this measure in heroin addicts (Kirby et al. Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999), the magnitude effect did not interact with group status: participants with PG showed similar sensitivity to reward magnitude to the controls. Steeper discounting on the Kirby MCQ procedure replicates previous work in PG using other variants of the delay discounting paradigm (Petry, Reference Petry2001a; Alessi & Petry, Reference Alessi and Petry2003; Dixon et al. Reference Dixon, Marley and Jacobs2003; MacKillop et al. Reference MacKillop, Anderson, Castelda, Mattson and Donovick2006; Ledgerwood et al. Reference Ledgerwood, Alessi, Phoenix and Petry2009), and patients with Parkinson's disease with medication-induced PG also show steeper delay discounting than controls with Parkinson's disease on the MCQ (Housden et al. Reference Housden, O'Sullivan, Joyce, Lees and Roiser2010). The discount rates in the PG group were correlated significantly with the UPPS-P subscales of ‘narrow’ impulsivity. This is in keeping with past personality studies in the general population, which primarily find an association with the Barratt non-planning subscale (Kirby et al. Reference Kirby, Petry and Bickel1999; Mobini et al. Reference Mobini, Grant, Kass and Yeomans2007; Koff & Lucas, Reference Koff and Lucas2011). Although statistically significant in the larger studies, it is likely that this state–trait relationship is of only modest strength (r≈0.3) because of the distinct sources of variability in the two measures: one is an introspective evaluation of generalized impulsive tendencies and the other is a state measure involving a series of financial decisions.

Some limitations should be noted. First, the present sample was composed of only treatment-seeking PG, and findings may not generalize to all levels of disordered gambling (although see MacKillop et al. Reference MacKillop, Anderson, Castelda, Mattson and Donovick2006), and our convenience sample included a mixture of cases tested before, during and after treatment. It was reported previously that ratings of gambling distortions are reduced by a course of psychotherapy (Breen et al. Reference Breen, Kruedelbach and Walker2001), although such an effect was not statistically significant in our data, possibly because the CBT programme did not place a strong emphasis on the correction of specific gambling distortions. Nonetheless, the association with delay discounting remained highly significant when partialling for stage of treatment. Second, in this restricted sample we did not exclude participants with some of the common clinical co-morbidities with PG, including mood disorders and substance use disorders, which may be associated with impulsivity and discounting tendencies (Reynolds, Reference Reynolds2006; Takahashi et al. Reference Takahashi, Oono, Inoue, Boku, Kako, Kitaichi, Kusumi, Masui, Nakagawa, Suzuki, Tanaka, Koyama and Radford2008; Dombrovski et al. Reference Dombrovski, Szanto, Siegle, Wallace, Forman, Sahakian, Reynolds and Clark2011). We note that the rate of substance use disorder co-morbidity was relatively low compared to international data in treatment-seeking PG (Ramirez et al. Reference Ramirez, McCormick, Russo and Taber1983; Black & Moyer, Reference Black and Moyer1998), and we did not examine the additive impact of these co-morbidities on psychological functioning (e.g. Petry, Reference Petry2001a) for reasons of statistical power.

Previous studies have described associations between facets of impulsivity and gambling severity (Steel & Blaszczynski, Reference Steel and Blaszczynski1998; Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, Schedlowski and Meyer2005), physiological arousal during gambling (Anderson & Brown, Reference Anderson and Brown1984; Krueger et al. Reference Krueger, Schedlowski and Meyer2005), and short-term treatment outcomes (Leblond et al. Reference Leblond, Ladouceur and Blaszczynski2003; Goudriaan et al. Reference Goudriaan, Oosterlaan, De Beurs and Van Den Brink2008). The present work highlights mood-related impulsivity (‘urgency’) and delay discounting as especially relevant facets in gambling behaviour. The correlation between delay discounting and gambling distortions may indeed shed further light on the nature of these faulty cognitions in disordered gambling. Previous work eliciting gambling distortions with the ‘think aloud’ technique has reported high rates of erroneous verbalizations even in non-problematic regular players (Ladouceur & Walker, Reference Ladouceur, Walker and Salkovskis1996; Delfabbro, Reference Delfabbro2004), and it was not until psychometric measures such as the GRCS and GBQ were developed that the elevated level of distortions in PG became evident (Raylu & Oei, Reference Raylu and Oei2004a; Emond & Marmurek, Reference Emond and Marmurek2010; Myrseth et al. Reference Myrseth, Brunborg and Eidem2010). However, it remains unclear from the psychometric scales whether increased scores reflect the frequency of these cognitions, the conviction with which the beliefs are held, or the tendency to use these beliefs to justify excessive gambling (Ladouceur & Walker, Reference Ladouceur, Walker and Salkovskis1996; Delfabbro, Reference Delfabbro2004). Measures of impulsivity, and the delay discounting paradigm most specifically, capture the conflict between short-term gratification and longer-term gains. The resolution of these inter-temporal decisions recruits neural regions, including the ventral striatum and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (McClure et al. Reference McClure, Laibson, Loewenstein and Cohen2004; Hariri et al. Reference Hariri, Brown, Williamson, Flory, De Wit and Manuck2006; Sellitto et al. Reference Sellitto, Ciaramelli and Di Pellegrino2010), that are implicated in the pathophysiology of problem gambling (Potenza et al. Reference Potenza, Leung, Blumberg, Peterson, Fulbright, Lacadie, Skudlarski and Gore2003; Reuter et al. Reference Reuter, Raedler, Rose, Hand, Glascher and Buchel2005; Lawrence et al. Reference Lawrence, Luty, Bogdan, Sahakian and Clark2009b). These dilemmas may be analogous to the conflict that gamblers experience between the rapid acceptance of a distorted belief, versus a more deliberative, analytical appraisal that their recent gambling results do not reflect skill, luck or other common biases. Measures of impulsivity including delay discounting were also seen to predict more general Beckian cognitive distortions associated with psychopathology in a student sample (Mobini et al. Reference Mobini, Pearce, Grant, Mills and Yeomans2006, Reference Mobini, Grant, Kass and Yeomans2007). Therefore, based on the link between delay discounting and GRCS scores, we hypothesize that the willingness to accept distorted gambling thoughts over more rational explanations may be a key pathological process in disordered gambling.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by a Medical Research Council (MRC) grant (G0802725) and completed within the Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, supported by a consortium award from the MRC and Wellcome Trust (director: T. W. Robbins). A.V.-G. was funded by the grant Jose Castillejo (JC 2009-00265) from the Spanish Ministry of Education and Science hosted by the Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge. Preliminary findings were presented at the National Centre for Responsible Gaming annual meeting (Las Vegas, November 2010) and the Society for the Study of Addiction annual meeting (York, UK, November 2010).

Declaration of Interest

None.

References

Alessi, SM, Petry, NM (2003). Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behavioral Processes 64, 345354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, G, Brown, RI (1984). Real and laboratory gambling, sensation-seeking and arousal. British Journal of Psychology 75, 401410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anestis, MD, Selby, EA, Fink, EL, Joiner, TE (2007). The multifaceted role of distress tolerance in dysregulated eating behaviors. International Journal of Eating Disorders 40, 718726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bickel, WK, Marsch, LA (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug dependence: delay discounting processes. Addiction 96, 7386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billieux, J, Gay, P, Rochat, L, Van Der Linden, M (2010). The role of urgency and its underlying psychological mechanisms in problematic behaviours. Behaviour Research and Therapy 48, 10851096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Billieux, J, Van Der Linden, M, Ceschi, G (2007). Which dimensions of impulsivity are related to cigarette craving? Addictive Behaviors 32, 11891199.Google Scholar
Black, DW, Moyer, T (1998). Clinical features and psychiatric comorbidity of subjects with pathological gambling behavior. Psychiatric Services 49, 14341439.Google Scholar
Blaszczynski, A, Nower, L (2002). A pathways model of problem and pathological gambling. Addiction 97, 487499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaszczynski, A, Steel, Z, McConaghy, N (1997). Impulsivity in pathological gambling: the antisocial impulsivist. Addiction 92, 7587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaszczynski, AP, Wilson, AC, McConaghy, N (1986). Sensation seeking and pathological gambling. British Journal of Addiction 81, 113117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowden-Jones, H, Clark, L (in press). Pathological gambling: a neurobiological and clinical update. British Journal of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Breen, RB, Kruedelbach, NG, Walker, HI (2001). Cognitive changes in pathological gamblers following a 28-day inpatient program. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 15, 246248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, GB (1996). Temporal discounting and utility for health and money. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 22, 771791.Google Scholar
Clark, L (2010). Decision-making during gambling: an integration of cognitive and psychobiological approaches. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 365, 319330.Google Scholar
Clark, L, Lawrence, AJ, Astley-Jones, F, Gray, N (2009). Gambling near-misses enhance motivation to gamble and recruit win-related brain circuitry. Neuron 61, 481490.Google Scholar
Cummins, LF, Nadorff, MR, Kelly, AE (2009). Winning and positive affect can lead to reckless gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 23, 287294.Google Scholar
Cunningham-Williams, RM, Grucza, RA, Cottler, LB, Womack, SB, Books, SJ, Przybeck, TR, Spitznagel, EL, Cloninger, CR (2005). Prevalence and predictors of pathological gambling: results from the St. Louis Personality, Health and Lifestyle (SLPHL) study. Journal of Psychiatric Research 39, 377390.Google Scholar
Cyders, MA, Smith, GT (2008 a). Clarifying the role of personality dispositions in risk for increased gambling behavior. Personality and Individual Differences 45, 503508.Google Scholar
Cyders, MA, Smith, GT (2008 b). Emotion-based dispositions to rash action: positive and negative urgency. Psychological Bulletin 134, 807828.Google Scholar
Cyders, MA, Smith, GT, Spillane, NS, Fischer, S, Annus, AM, Peterson, C (2007). Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior: development and validation of a measure of positive urgency. Psychological Assessment 19, 107118.Google Scholar
Cyders, MA, Zapolski, TC, Combs, JL, Settles, RF, Fillmore, MT, Smith, GT (2010). Experimental effect of positive urgency on negative outcomes from risk taking and on increased alcohol consumption. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 24, 367375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delfabbro, P (2004). The stubborn logic of regular gamblers: obstacles and dilemmas in cognitive gambling research. Journal of Gambling Studies 20, 121.Google Scholar
Dixon, MR, Marley, J, Jacobs, EA (2003). Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 36, 449458.Google Scholar
Dombrovski, AY, Szanto, K, Siegle, GJ, Wallace, ML, Forman, SD, Sahakian, B, Reynolds, CF 3rd, Clark, L (2011). Lethal forethought: delayed reward discounting differentiates high- and low-lethality suicide attempts in old age. Biological Psychiatry. Published online: 18 February 2011. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emond, MS, Marmurek, HH (2010). Gambling related cognitions mediate the association between thinking style and problem gambling severity. Journal of Gambling Studies 26, 257267.Google Scholar
Evenden, JL (1999). Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 146, 348361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferris, J, Wynne, H (2001). Canadian Problem Gambling Index. Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse: Ottawa, Ontario.Google Scholar
Fortune, EE, Goodie, AS (2010). The relationship between pathological gambling and sensation seeking: the role of subscale scores. Journal of Gambling Studies 26, 331346.Google Scholar
Frascella, J, Potenza, MN, Brown, LL, Childress, AR (2010). Shared brain vulnerabilities open the way for nonsubstance addictions: carving addiction at a new joint? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1187, 294315.Google Scholar
Goudriaan, AE, Oosterlaan, J, De Beurs, E, Van Den Brink, W (2008). The role of self-reported impulsivity and reward sensitivity versus neurocognitive measures of disinhibition and decision-making in the prediction of relapse in pathological gamblers. Psychological Medicine 38, 4150.Google Scholar
Green, L, Myerson, J (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin 130, 769792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hariri, AR, Brown, SM, Williamson, DE, Flory, JD, De Wit, H, Manuck, SB (2006). Preference for immediate over delayed rewards is associated with magnitude of ventral striatal activity. Journal of Neuroscience 26, 1321313217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Housden, CR, O'Sullivan, SS, Joyce, EM, Lees, AJ, Roiser, JP (2010). Intact reward learning but elevated delay discounting in Parkinson's disease patients with impulsive-compulsive spectrum behaviors. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 21552164.Google Scholar
Jacobs, D (1986). A general theory of addictions: a new theoretical model. Journal of Gambling Behaviour 2, 1531.Google Scholar
Kirby, KN, Petry, NM, Bickel, WK (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128, 7887.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koff, E, Lucas, M (2011). Mood moderates the relationship between impulsiveness and delay discounting. Personality and Individual Differences 50, 10181022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krueger, TH, Schedlowski, M, Meyer, G (2005). Cortisol and heart rate measures during casino gambling in relation to impulsivity. Neuropsychobiology 52, 206211.Google Scholar
Ladouceur, R, Walker, M (1996). A cognitive perspective on gambling. In Trends in Cognitive and Behavioural Therapies (ed. Salkovskis, P. M.), pp. 89–120. Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK.Google Scholar
Lawrence, AJ, Luty, J, Bogdan, NA, Sahakian, BJ, Clark, L (2009 a). Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 207, 163172.Google Scholar
Lawrence, AJ, Luty, J, Bogdan, NA, Sahakian, BJ, Clark, L (2009 b). Problem gamblers share deficits in impulsive decision-making with alcohol-dependent individuals. Addiction 104, 10061015.Google Scholar
Leblond, J, Ladouceur, R, Blaszczynski, A (2003). Which pathological gamblers will complete treatment? British Journal of Clinical Psychology 42, 205209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ledgerwood, DM, Alessi, SM, Phoenix, N, Petry, NM (2009). Behavioral assessment of impulsivity in pathological gamblers with and without substance use disorder histories versus healthy controls. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 105, 8996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lesieur, HR, Blume, SB (1987). The South Oaks Gambling Screen (SOGS): a new instrument for the identification of pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry 144, 11841188.Google ScholarPubMed
Lloyd, J, Doll, H, Hawton, K, Dutton, WH, Geddes, JR, Goodwin, GM, Rogers, RD (2010). How psychological symptoms relate to different motivations for gambling: an online study of internet gamblers. Biological Psychiatry 68, 733740.Google Scholar
MacKillop, J, Anderson, EJ, Castelda, BA, Mattson, RE, Donovick, PJ (2006). Convergent validity of measures of cognitive distortions, impulsivity, and time perspective with pathological gambling. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 20, 7579.Google Scholar
McClure, SM, Laibson, DI, Loewenstein, G, Cohen, JD (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science 306, 503507.Google Scholar
Miller, NV, Currie, SR (2008). A Canadian population level analysis of the roles of irrational gambling cognitions and risky gambling practices as correlates of gambling intensity and pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies 24, 257274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitzner, GB, Whelan, JP, Meyers, AW (2010). Comments from the trenches: proposed changes to the DSM-V classification of pathological gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies. Published online: 24 October 2010. doi:10.1007/s10899-010-9225-x.Google Scholar
Mobini, S, Grant, A, Kass, AE, Yeomans, MR (2007). Relationships between functional and dysfunctional impulsivity, delay discounting and cognitive distortions. Personality and Individual Differences 43, 15171528.Google Scholar
Mobini, S, Pearce, M, Grant, A, Mills, J, Yeomans, MR (2006). The relationship between impulsivity, sensation seeking and cognitive distortions in a sample of non-clinical population. Personality and Individual Differences 40, 11531163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myerson, J, Green, L, Warusawitharana, M (2001). Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 76, 235243.Google Scholar
Myrseth, H, Brunborg, GS, Eidem, M (2010). Differences in cognitive distortions between pathological and non-pathological gamblers with preferences for chance or skill games. Journal of Gambling Studies 26, 561569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, HE, Willison, J (1991). National Adult Reading Test (NART) Test Manual. NFER-Nelson: Windsor, UK.Google Scholar
Nower, L, Derevensky, JL, Gupta, R (2004). The relationship of impulsivity, sensation seeking, coping, and substance use in youth gamblers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 18, 4955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oei, TP, Gordon, LM (2008). Psychosocial factors related to gambling abstinence and relapse in members of Gamblers Anonymous. Journal of Gambling Studies 24, 91–105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parke, A, Griffiths, M, Irwing, P (2004). Personality traits in pathological gambling: sensation seeking, deferment of gratification and competitiveness as risk factors. Addiction Research and Theory 12, 201212.Google Scholar
Petry, NM (2001 a). Pathological gamblers, with and without substance use disorders, discount delayed rewards at high rates. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 110, 482487.Google Scholar
Petry, NM (2001 b). Substance abuse, pathological gambling, and impulsiveness. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 63, 2938.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potenza, MN (2006). Should addictive disorders include non-substance-related conditions? Addiction 101 (Suppl. 1), 142151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potenza, MN, Leung, HC, Blumberg, HP, Peterson, BS, Fulbright, RK, Lacadie, CM, Skudlarski, P, Gore, JC (2003). An FMRI Stroop task study of ventromedial prefrontal cortical function in pathological gamblers. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 19901994.Google Scholar
Ramirez, LF, McCormick, RA, Russo, AM, Taber, JI (1983). Patterns of substance abuse in pathological gamblers undergoing treatment. Addictive Behaviors 8, 425428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raylu, N, Oei, TP (2004 a). The Gambling Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS): development, confirmatory factor validation and psychometric properties. Addiction 99, 757769.Google Scholar
Raylu, N, Oei, TP (2004 b). Role of culture in gambling and problem gambling. Clinical Psychology Review 23, 10871114.Google Scholar
Reuter, J, Raedler, T, Rose, M, Hand, I, Glascher, J, Buchel, C (2005). Pathological gambling is linked to reduced activation of the mesolimbic reward system. Nature Neuroscience 8, 147148.Google Scholar
Reynolds, B (2006). A review of delay-discounting research with humans: relations to drug use and gambling. Behavioural Pharmacology 17, 651667.Google Scholar
Sellitto, M, Ciaramelli, E, Di Pellegrino, G (2010). Myopic discounting of future rewards after medial orbitofrontal damage in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 30, 1642916436.Google Scholar
Shaffer, HJ, Labrie, R, Scanlan, KM, Cummings, TN (1994). Pathological gambling among adolescents: Massachusetts Gambling Screen (MAGS). Journal of Gambling Studies 10, 339362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharpe, L (2002). A reformulated cognitive-behavioral model of problem gambling. A biopsychosocial perspective. Clinical Psychology Review 22, 125.Google Scholar
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, Amorim, P, Janavs, J, Weiller, E, Hergueta, T, Baker, R, Dunbar, GC (1998). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59 (Suppl. 20), 2233; quiz 34–57.Google ScholarPubMed
Slutske, WS, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Poulton, R (2005). Personality and problem gambling: a prospective study of a birth cohort of young adults. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 769775.Google Scholar
Steel, Z, Blaszczynski, A (1998). Impulsivity, personality disorders and pathological gambling severity. Addiction 93, 895905.Google Scholar
Steenbergh, TA, Meyers, AW, May, RK, Whelan, JP (2002). Development and validation of the Gamblers' Beliefs Questionnaire. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors 16, 143149.Google Scholar
Takahashi, T, Oono, H, Inoue, T, Boku, S, Kako, Y, Kitaichi, Y, Kusumi, I, Masui, T, Nakagawa, S, Suzuki, K, Tanaka, T, Koyama, T, Radford, MH (2008). Depressive patients are more impulsive and inconsistent in intertemporal choice behavior for monetary gain and loss than healthy subjects – an analysis based on Tsallis' statistics. Neuroendocrinology Letters 29, 351358.Google ScholarPubMed
Toneatto, T, Blitz-Miller, T, Calderwood, K, Dragonetti, R, Tsanos, A (1997). Cognitive distortions in heavy gambling. Journal of Gambling Studies 13, 253266.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, AP, Creemers, HE, Verhulst, FC, Ormel, J, Huizink, AC (2010). Are adolescents gambling with cannabis use? A longitudinal study of impulsivity measures and adolescent substance use: the TRAILS study. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs 72, 7088.Google Scholar
Verdejo-Garcia, A, Lawrence, AJ, Clark, L (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 32, 777810.Google Scholar
Vitaro, F, Arseneault, L, Tremblay, RE (1999). Impulsivity predicts problem gambling in low SES adolescent males. Addiction 94, 565575.Google Scholar
Wardle, H, Moody, A, Spence, S, Orford, J, Volberg, R, Jotangia, D, Griffiths, M, Hussey, D, Dobbie, F (2010). British Gambling Prevalence Survey. National Centre for Social Research: London.Google Scholar
Whiteside, SP, Lynam, DR (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity: using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences 30, 669689.Google Scholar
Xian, H, Shah, KR, Phillips, SM, Scherrer, JF, Volberg, R, Eisen, SA (2008). Association of cognitive distortions with problem and pathological gambling in adult male twins. Psychiatry Research 160, 300307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Mean scores (s.d.) on the GRCS and UPPS-P Impulsivity Scale in the participants with pathological gambling (PG) and healthy controls

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Delay discounting (ln k) on the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ), in patients with pathological gambling (PG) and healthy controls (HC). Errors bars indicate standard error of the mean.

Figure 2

Table 2. Untransformed delay discounting measures on the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ), in the participants with pathological gambling (PG) and healthy controls

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Delay discounting (ln k scores, averaged across the three magnitude levels) is correlated significantly with total score on the Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS), in the pathological gamblers.

Figure 4

Table 3. Univariate correlations (Pearson's r) between the delay discounting (Kirby k; n=29), Gambling-Related Cognitions Scale (GRCS) (n=30) and UPPS-P (n=30) in the pathological gamblers