Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T00:01:22.519Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotion recognition in autism: coordinating faces and voices

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 July 2009

R. P. Hobson*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, London
J. Ouston
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, London
A. Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, London
*
1 Address for correspondence: Dr R. Peter Hobson, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, De Crespigny Park, London, SE5 8AF.

Synopsis

Autistic and non-autistic mentally retarded adolescents and young adults were individually matched for age and verbal ability and were given tasks in which they chose photographs of faces for emotionally expressive voices, and photographs of non-emotional things or events to accompany recorded sounds. The results were that relative to control subjects, autistic individuals performed less well on the emotion tasks than on the non-emotion tasks. The findings suggest that autistic individuals have a disability in recognizing bodily expressions of emotion, and that there is a degree of task-specificity to this impairment.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1988

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Attwood, A. J. (1984). The gestures of autistic children. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of London.Google Scholar
Bartak, L., Rutter, M. & Cox, A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorder. I. The children. British Journal of Psychiatry 126, 127145.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. (1982). The lateralisation of emotion: a critical review. International Journal of Psychology 17, 211229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, M. E. & Nodine, C. F. (1978). Memory processes in facial recognition and recall. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 12, 317319.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M. (1965). Expanded Manual for the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. American Guidance Service: Circle Pines Minnesota.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M. & Whetton, C. (1982). British Picture Vocabulary Scale. NFER-Nelson: Windsor.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the Face. A Guide to Recognizing Emotions from Facial Cues. Prentice Hall: Englewood Cliffs N.J.Google Scholar
Fein, D. (1987). Social and non-social tasks in PDD and young normal children. Paper presented at a symposium on the Development of Social Cognition in Autistic Spectrum Disorder,SRCD Conference,BaltimoreApril. (Laboratory of Neuropsychology, Boston University School of Medicine, 85 East Newton Street, Boston Mass. 02118, USA.)Google Scholar
Fein, D., Pennington, B., Markowitz, P., Braverman, M. & Waterhouse, L. (1986). Toward a neuropsychological model of infantile autism: Are the social deficits primary? Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry 25, 198212.Google Scholar
Feinberg, T. E., Rifkin, A., Schaffer, C. & Walker, E. (1986). Facial discrimination and emotional recognition in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry 43, 276279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillies, S. (1965). Some abilities of psychotic children and subnormal controls. Journal of Mental Deficiency Research 9, 89101.Google Scholar
Gray, J. M., Fraser, W. L. & Leudar, I. (1983). Recognition of emotion from facial expression in mental handicap. British Journal of Psychiatry 142, 566571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermelin, B. & O'Connor, N. (1985). Logico-affective states and nonverbal language. In Communication Problems in Autism (ed. Schopler, E. & Mesibov, G. B.), pp. 283310. Plenum Press: New York.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1983). The autistic child's recognition of age-related features of people, animals and things. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 1, 343352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1986 a). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 27, 321342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1986 b). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion: A further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 27, 671680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P. (1988). Beyond cognition: a theory of autism. In Autism: New Perspectives on Diagnosis, Nature and Treatment (ed. Dawson, G.). Guílford: New York. (In the press).Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J. & Lee, A. (1988 a). What's in a face? The case of autism. British Journal of Psychology (in the press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J. & Lee, A. (1988 b). Recognition of emotion by mentally retarded adolescents and young adults. (Submitted for publication.)Google Scholar
Jennings, W. B. (1973). A study of the preference for affective cues in autistic children. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Memphis State University.Google Scholar
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child 2, 217250.Google Scholar
Lungdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: an approach to the study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 19, 255268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langdell, T. (1982). Peer perception: toward a unified theory of autism.In Proceedings of the 1981 International Conference on Autism,Boston U.S A. (ed. Park, D.), pp. 8996. National Society for Children and Adults with Autism: Washington DC.Google Scholar
Marcell, M. M. & Jett, DeL. A. (1985). Identification of vocally expressed emotions by mentally retarded and non-retarded individuals. American Journal of Mental Deficiency 85, 537545.Google Scholar
Maurer, H. & Newbrough, J. R. (1987). Facial expressions of mentally retarded and nonretarded children. I. Recognition by mentally retarded and nonretarded adults. American Journal of Mental Deficiency 91, 505510.Google ScholarPubMed
Novic, J., Luchins, D. H. & Perline, R. (1984). Facial affect recognition in schizophrenia. Is there a differential deficit? British Journal of Psychiatry 144, 533537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, N. & Hermelin, B. (1967). The selective visual attention of psychotic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 8, 167179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1960). The Standard Progressive Matrices: Sets A, B, C, D and E. H. K. Lewis: London.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1965). The Coloured Progressive Matrices: Sets A, Ab B. H. K. Lewis: London.Google Scholar
Ricks, D. M. (1975). Vocal communication in pre-verbal normal and autistic children. In Language, Cognitive Deficits, and Retardation. (ed. O'Connor, N.), pp. 7580. Butterworths: London.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1974). The development of infantile autism. Psychological Medicine 4, 147163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, D. W. (1985). Asperger's syndrome and non-verbal communication: a pilot study. Psychological Medicine 15, 683687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tucker, D. M. (1981). Lateral brain function, emotion, and conceptualization. Psychological Bulletin 89, 1946.Google Scholar
Walker, E., McGuire, M. & Bettes, B. (1984). Recognition and identification of facial stimuli by schizophrenics and patients with affective disorders. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 23, 3744.Google Scholar
Weeks, S. J. & Hobson, R. P. (1987). The salience of facial expression for autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 28, 137152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed