Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:01:47.989Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Confirmation bias: why psychiatrists stick to wrong preliminary diagnoses

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 May 2011

R. Mendel
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Technische Universität München, Germany
E. Traut-Mattausch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Germany
E. Jonas
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Universität Salzburg, Austria
S. Leucht*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Technische Universität München, Germany
J. M. Kane
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, The Zucker Hillside Hospital of the North Shore-Long Island Jewish Health System, USA
K. Maino
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Germany
W. Kissling
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Technische Universität München, Germany
J. Hamann
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Technische Universität München, Germany
*
*Address for correspondence: PD Dr S. Leucht, Department of Psychiatry, Technische Universität München, Ismaninger Straße 22, 81675 München, Germany. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

Diagnostic errors can have tremendous consequences because they can result in a fatal chain of wrong decisions. Experts assume that physicians' desire to confirm a preliminary diagnosis while failing to seek contradictory evidence is an important reason for wrong diagnoses. This tendency is called ‘confirmation bias’.

Method

To study whether psychiatrists and medical students are prone to confirmation bias and whether confirmation bias leads to poor diagnostic accuracy in psychiatry, we presented an experimental decision task to 75 psychiatrists and 75 medical students.

Results

A total of 13% of psychiatrists and 25% of students showed confirmation bias when searching for new information after having made a preliminary diagnosis. Participants conducting a confirmatory information search were significantly less likely to make the correct diagnosis compared to participants searching in a disconfirmatory or balanced way [multiple logistic regression: odds ratio (OR) 7.3, 95% confidence interval (CI) 2.53–21.22, p<0.001; OR 3.2, 95% CI 1.23–8.56, p=0.02]. Psychiatrists conducting a confirmatory search made a wrong diagnosis in 70% of the cases compared to 27% or 47% for a disconfirmatory or balanced information search (students: 63, 26 and 27%). Participants choosing the wrong diagnosis also prescribed different treatment options compared with participants choosing the correct diagnosis.

Conclusions

Confirmatory information search harbors the risk of wrong diagnostic decisions. Psychiatrists should be aware of confirmation bias and instructed in techniques to reduce bias.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arkes, HR (1981). Impediments to accurate clinical judgment and possible ways to minimize their impact. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 49, 323330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arocha, JF, Patel, VL, Patel, YC (1993). Hypothesis generation and the coordination of theory and evidence in novice diagnostic reasoning. Medical Decision Making 13, 198211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ask, K, Granhag, PA (2007). Motivational bias in criminal investigators' judgments of witness reliability. Journal of Applied Psychology 37, 561591.Google Scholar
Berner, ES (2007). Clinical Decision Support Systems: Theory and Practice. Springer-Verlag: New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berner, ES, Maisiak, RS, Cobbs, CG, Taunton, OD (1999). Effects of a decision support system on physicians' diagnostic performance. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association 6, 420427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crichton, M (2007). Where does it hurt? New York Times (www.nytimes.com/2007/04/01/books/review/Crichton.t.html?scp=2&sq=Where%20does%20it%20hurt&st=cse). Accessed 14 May 2009.Google Scholar
Croskerry, P (2002). Achieving quality in clinical decision making: cognitive strategies and detection of bias. Academic Emergency Medicine 9, 11841204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Croskerry, P (2003 a). The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis and strategies to minimize them. Academic Medicine 78, 775780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croskerry, P (2003 b). Cognitive forcing strategies in clinical decision making. Annals of Emergency Medicine 41, 110120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, NV (2000). Physician judgments of uncertainty. In Decision Making in Health Care: Theory, Psychology, and Applications (ed. Chapman, G. B. and Sonnenberg, F. A.), pp. 211252. Cambridge University Press: New York.Google Scholar
Elstein, AS, Schwartz, A (2002). Clinical problem solving and diagnostic decision making: selective review of the cognitive literature. British Medical Journal 324, 729732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L (1964). Conflict, Decision, and Dissonance. Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA.Google Scholar
Fischer, P, Jonas, E, Frey, D, Schulz-Hardt, S (2005). Selective exposure to information: the impact of information limits. European Journal of Social Psychology 35, 469492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frey, D (1981). Informationssuche und Informationsbewertung bei Entscheidungen [Information Seeking and Information Evaluation in Decision Making]. Verlag Hans Huber: Bern.Google Scholar
Frey, D (1986). Recent research on selective exposure to information. In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (ed. Berkowitz, L.), pp. 4180. Academic Press: New York.Google Scholar
Friedman, MH, Connell, KJ, Olthoff, AJ, Sinacore, JM, Bordage, G (1998). Medical student errors in making a diagnosis. Academic Medicine 73 (Suppl. 10), S19S21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, EJ (2003). Thinking about thinking. Annals of Emergency Medicine 41, 121122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gambrill, E (2005). Critical Thinking in Clinical Practice: Improving the Quality of Judgements and Decisions. John Wiley & Sons Inc.: Hoboken, NJ.Google Scholar
Graber, MA, VanScoy, D (2003). How well does decision support software perform in the emergency department? Emergency Medicine Journal 20, 426428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groopman, JE (2007). How Doctors Think. Houghton Mifflin Publishing Co: Boston, MA.Google Scholar
Gurmankin, AD, Baron, J, Hershey, JC, Ubel, PA (2002). The role of physicians' recommendations in medical treatment decisions. Medical Decision Making 22, 262271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirt, ER, Markman, KD (1995). Multiple explanation: a consider-an-alternative strategy for debiasing judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69, 10691086.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonas, E, Frey, D (2003). Information search and presentation in advisor-client interaction. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 91, 154168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonas, E, Schulz-Hardt, S, Frey, D, Thelen, N (2001). Confirmation bias in sequential information search after preliminary decisions: an expansion of dissonance theoretical research on selective exposure to information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80, 557571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kern, L, Doherty, ME (1982). ‘Pseudodiagnosticity’ in an idealized medical problem-solving environment. Journal of Medical Education 57, 100104.Google Scholar
Klayman, J (1995). Varieties of confirmation bias. Psychology of Learning and Motivation 32, 358418.Google Scholar
Klein, JG (2005). Five pitfalls in decisions about diagnosis and prescribing. British Medical Journal 330, 781783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krems, JF, Zierer, C (1994). Are experts immune to cognitive bias? Dependence of ‘confirmation bias’ on specialist knowledge [in German]. Zeitschrift für experimentelle und angewandte Psychologie 41, 98–115.Google ScholarPubMed
Mynatt, CR, Doherty, ME, Tweney, RD (1978). Consequences of confirmation and disconfirmation in a simulated research environment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 30, 395406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nemeth, CJ, Connell, JB, Rogers, JD, Brown, KS (2001). Improving decision making by means of dissent. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 31, 4858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman-Toker, DE, Pronovost, PJ (2009). Diagnostic errors – the next frontier for patients safety. Journal of the American Medical Association 301, 10601062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nickerson, RS (1998). Confirmation bias: a ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology 2, 175220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parmley, MC (2006). The effects of the confirmation bias on diagnostic decision making (http://idea.library.drexel.edu/bitstream/1860/1164/1/Parmley_Meagan.pdf). Accessed 14 May 2009.Google Scholar
Pines, JM (2006). Profiles in patient safety: confirmation bias in emergency medicine. Academic Emergency Medicine 13, 9094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramnarayan, P, Cronje, N, Brown, R, Negus, R, Coode, B, Moss, P, Hassan, T, Hamer, W, Britto, J (2007). Validation of a diagnostic reminder system in emergency medicine: a multi-centre study. Emergency Medicine Journal 24, 619624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weingart, SN, McL Wilson, R, Gibberd, RW, Harrison, B (2000). Epidemiology of medical error. Western Journal of Medicine 172, 390393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, FM, Gruppen, LD, Billim, JE (1985). Differential diagnosis and the competing-hypotheses heuristic. A practical approach to judgment under uncertainty and Bayesian probability. Journal of the American Medical Association 253, 28582862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, FM, Gruppen, LD, Billi, JE (1988). Use of the competing-hypotheses heuristic to reduce ‘pseudodiagnosticity’. Journal of Medical Education 63, 548554.Google ScholarPubMed