Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T05:51:57.090Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Childhood inflammatory markers and intelligence as predictors of subsequent persistent depressive symptoms: a longitudinal cohort study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 November 2017

G. M. Khandaker*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
J. Stochl
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Department of Kinanthropology, Charles University, Prague, Czech Republic
S. Zammit
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health, Addiction and Suicide Research, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK Institute of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, MRC Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Cardiff University, Cardiff, UK
I. Goodyer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
G. Lewis
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, University College London, London, UK
P. B. Jones
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Cambridgeshire and Peterborough NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Dr G. M. Khandaker, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

To identify developmental sub-groups of depressive symptoms during the second decade of life, a critical period of brain development, using data from a prospective birth cohort. To test whether childhood intelligence and inflammatory markers are associated with subsequent persistent depressive symptoms.

Methods

IQ, a proxy for neurodevelopment, was measured at age 8 years. Interleukin 6 (IL-6) and C-reactive protein, typical inflammatory markers, were measured at age 9 years. Depressive symptoms were measured six times between 10 and 19 years using the short mood and feelings questionnaire (SMFQ), which were coded as binary variable and then used in latent class analysis to identify developmental sub-groups of depressive symptoms.

Results

Longitudinal SMFQ data from 9156 participants yielded three distinct population sub-groups of depressive symptoms: no symptoms (81.2%); adolescent-onset symptoms (13.2%); persistent symptoms (5.6%). Lower IQ and higher IL-6 levels in childhood were independently associated with subsequent persistent depressive symptoms in a linear, dose–response fashion, but not with adolescent-onset symptoms. Compared with the group with no symptoms the adjusted odds ratio for persistent depressive symptoms per s.d. increase in IQ was 0.80 (95% CI, 0.68–0.95); that for IL-6 was 1.20 (95% CI, 1.03–1.39). Evidence for an association with IL-6 remained after controlling for initial severity of depressive symptoms at 10 years. There was no evidence that IL-6 moderated or mediated the IQ-persistent depressive symptom relationship.

Conclusions

The results indicate potentially important roles for two distinct biological processes, neurodevelopment and inflammation, in the aetiology of persistent depressive symptoms in young people.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017

Introduction

Mood disorders affect about 20% of the population in lifetime and a quarter of all cases of depression emerge before age 20 (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler2003, Reference Kessler2005). The second decade of life is a critical period for brain development; decrease in grey matter volume and increase in connectivity and integrative processing occur during adolescence (Gogtay et al. Reference Gogtay2004). These developmental events are mirrored by a tide of emergent mental disorders such that, excluding neurodegenerative disorders of later life, about half of all mental disorders first occur by the mid-teens (Kessler et al. Reference Kessler2005; Jones, Reference Jones2013). Distinct trajectories of depressive and anxiety symptoms emerge during early- to mid-teen age years; persistent and severe symptoms during adolescence and adulthood are associated with markers of altered neurodevelopment such as delayed motor milestones (Colman et al. Reference Colman2007). High depressive symptoms during the second decade of life may represent a vulnerability factor for subsequent major depression (Owens et al. Reference Owens2014).

Non-specific and transitory elements of depression are common in youth and early adulthood, crystallising into a clinical syndrome in some but not all of those who experience them (Merikangas et al. Reference Merikangas2003). Examining depression at a single time point or average levels over time fails to fully account for population heterogeneity because distinct groups of individuals can arrive at the same end point via different pathways. Therefore, longitudinal studies of depressive symptoms over the early-life course including their antecedents and markers for persistence would be helpful for understanding the causes and mechanisms of depression. Compared with a one off measure of psychopathology longitudinal groups derived from repeated measures is less affected by occasional misreporting or temporary fluctuations in a condition. Such approach is also more efficient as they maximise use of partial responses to inform group membership.

In this study, we have used longitudinal population-based data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort to identify developmental subgroups of young people based on self-reported depressive symptoms measured six times between 10 and 19 years using the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ). We have then examined risk factors associated with persistent depressive symptoms particularly focusing on two biological processes, neurodevelopment and inflammation. Longitudinal studies reporting an association with lower childhood IQ indicate a neurodevelopmental component to adult depression (Zammit et al. Reference Zammit2004; Koenen et al. Reference Koenen2009). We have previously shown that childhood inflammation is associated with increased risks of depressive and psychotic symptoms subsequently in early-adulthood (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014a). There is some evidence for a link between inflammation, neurodevelopment and cognitive function. Both infection (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2013) and immunological genes (Sekar et al. Reference Sekar2016) associated with the risk of psychosis can interfere with neurodevelopment. Inflammation is associated with reduced cognitive performance in experimental studies of healthy volunteers (Reichenberg et al. Reference Reichenberg2001; Harrison et al. Reference Harrison2009) and with cognitive decline in clinical populations at risk of dementia (Iwashyna et al. Reference Iwashyna2010). Although previous longitudinal studies have reported associations between childhood lower IQ, inflammation and depression in adulthood, it is unclear whether these risk factors are associated with persistent symptoms. Furthermore, to our knowledge no study has examined the interplay between inflammation and neurodevelopment with regards to risk of depression.

Using longitudinal population-based data, we have tested the hypothesis that childhood general intelligence, as measured by IQ at 8 years, and inflammation, as measured by elevated serum concentrations of interleukin 6 (IL-6) and C-reactive protein (CRP) at 9 years, are associated with persistent depressive symptoms subsequently between age 10 and 19 years independently of each other. We have explored linearity of association; the effects of past psychological and behavioural problems, initial severity of depressive symptoms, and sex difference on the IQ/inflammation-persistent depressive symptom relationship (see statistical analysis for rationale and approach). In addition, we have examined the relationship between IQ and inflammatory markers. We have tested whether inflammation mediates and/or moderates the relationship between childhood IQ and subsequent persistent depressive symptoms.

Methods

Description of cohort and sample

The ALSPAC birth cohort recruited 14 541 pregnant women resident in county Avon, a geographically defined region in southwest of England, with expected dates of delivery 1st April 1991 to 31st December 1992 (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/) (Boyd et al. Reference Boyd2013). The study website contains details of all available data through a fully searchable ‘data dictionary’ (http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/data-access/data-dictionary/). Parents completed regular postal questionnaires about all aspects of their child's health and development from birth. Since age 7, the children attended an annual assessment clinic during which they participated in various face-to-face interviews and physical tests.

Groups of depressive symptoms were created using 9156 participants who had completed the SMFQ at least once between 10 and 19 years. This approach to sample selection – as opposed to including only those who had completed the SMFQ at all six time points – increased representativeness of the sample, sample size and statistical power. Sample size for the analyses of childhood IQ (N = 6833) and inflammatory markers (N = 4409) vary as these childhood measures were completed by different numbers of people. IQ was assessed on 7354 individuals at 8 years, of which 6833 provided data for depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years. IL-6 and CRP were measured on 5076 participants at age 9 years, of which 4409 provided data for depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years. We excluded 491 children who reported an infection around blood collection from the analyses of inflammatory markers because IL6 and CRP levels sharply increase during an infection, which return to normal after illness subsides; we were interested in inflammatory marker levels in healthy individuals.

Ethical approval

Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Ethics and Law Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees.

Laboratory methods

Data on two archetypal circulating markers of inflammation, CRP (an acute phase protein) and IL-6 (a pleotropic, inflammatory cytokine), were available from the ALSPAC birth cohort. At age 9 years non-fasting blood samples were collected by venepuncture; samples were immediately spun and frozen at −80 °C after collection. IL-6 and CRP levels were assayed in 2008 after a median of 7.5 years in storage with no previous freeze–thaw cycles during this period. IL-6 was measured by enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA) (R&D systems, UK), and high sensitivity CRP by automated particle-enhanced immunoturbidimetric assay (Roche UK). All inter-assay coefficients of variation were <5%. In the total sample, IL-6 values ranged from 0.007 to 20.051 pg/mL and CRP values ranged from 0.01 to 45.17 mg/L (32 subjects over 10 mg/L). No other inflammatory markers were measured on blood samples collected at age 9 years. Serum IL-6 and CRP levels were correlated; Pearson's correlation co-efficient based on natural log-transformed values of IL-6 and CRP was 0.45; p < 0.001.

Psychiatric measures

Psychological and behavioural problems at age 7

Mothers completed the parental version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) when the study child was 7. The SDQ is an age appropriate, valid and reliable tool for measuring psychological and behavioural problems in young children (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997). It measures difficulties in four domains (emotional problems, conduct problems, hyperactivity, peer problems), and gives a total difficulties score of 0–40.

IQ at age 8

Full scale, verbal and performance IQ were measured by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd UK edition (WISC III) (Wechsler et al. Reference Wechsler, Golombok and Rust1992). A shortened version of the test was applied by trained psychologists, whereby alternate items (always starting with item number 1 in the standard form) were used for all ten subtests with the exception of the coding subtest, which was administered in its standard form. Used successfully in other studies (Stricker et al. Reference Stricker, Merbaum and Tangeman1968; Finch & Chihldress, Reference Finch and Chihldress1975; Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014b), this method provides valid IQ measures that show robust correlations with neurodevelopmental disorders, neurocognitive measures, such as working memory, short-term memory and sociodemographic factors such as social class (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014b).

Depressive symptoms between ages 10 and 19

Depressive symptoms were measured six times using the SMFQ completed by the participants at ages 10, 13, 14, 17, 18 and 19 years (Angold et al. Reference Angold, Costello and Messer1995). The SMFQ is a validated tool widely used in epidemiological studies (Thapar & McGuffin, Reference Thapar and McGuffin1998; Sharp et al. Reference Sharp, Goodyer and Croudace2006), which includes 13 items covering core symptoms of depression and anxiety experienced in past 2 weeks. Each item is scored zero (not true), one (sometimes true) or two (true) giving a total score of 0–26. We defined individuals with high depressive symptoms at each assessment using total scores of 11 or above because such scores reflect potentially clinically relevant symptom burden. Using this threshold the specificity and sensitivity for a diagnosis of depression is 85 and 60%, respectively (Angold et al. Reference Angold, Costello and Messer1995), validated against the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (Costello et al. Reference Costello1982).

Assessment of covariates

We included age, sex, father's occupation, ethnicity, maternal Edinburgh Post-natal Depression Scale (EPDS) score, SDQ total difficulties score at age 7, total IQ score at age 8 and body mass index (BMI) at age 9 as potential confounders. Age was recorded at the time of assessment of IQ and inflammatory markers, which was used as a continuous variable. Sex was recoded at birth and was coded as a binary variable. Father's occupation was recorded at birth according to the UK Office of National Statistics Classification System (Class I = professionals and higher managerial workers; II = intermediate occupations; IIIa = skilled non-manual occupations; IIIb= skilled manual occupations; IV = partly skilled occupations; V = unskilled occupations), and was coded as a categorical variable (non-manual, i.e. I + II + IIIa, v. manual occupations, i.e. IIIb + IV + V). Ethnicity was recorded at birth and was coded as a categorical variable (White v. Others). Maternal depression was measured by EPDS at 8-week post-partum and was used as a continuous variable. BMI was recoded at age 9 years at the time of blood collection, which was calculated as weight in kilogram divided by height in metre squared. See above for descriptions of IQ and SDQ.

Statistical analysis

Latent class analysis (LCA) and baseline comparisons

LCA was used to identify classes or population sub-groups based on six longitudinal assessments of depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years (Collins & Lanza, Reference Collins and Lanza2010). Total SMFQ scores from each wave of follow-up were coded as a binary variable using score 11 as cut-off because we were interested in the association between childhood biomarkers and subsequent population sub-groups demarcated by potentially clinically relevant depressive symptom burden. Bayesian information criterion (BIC) was used to assess whether a two, three, or four class model provided a better fit for the data (Schwarz, Reference Schwarz1978); the best-fitting model is the one with the lowest BIC. LCA is the stochastic model, so each person does not belong to a class with 100% probability, unlike for example in cluster analysis, rather each person is assigned to the population sub-group they are most likely to belong. We examined the relationship between population sub-groups of depressive symptoms and known sociodemographic and other correlates of depression at baseline. Baseline comparisons for IQ were carried out using linear regression to calculate mean difference (95% CI) in full scale, verbal and performance IQ between groups.

Association between persistent depressive symptoms and IQ, IL-6 and CRP

We used multinomial logistic regression to calculate odds ratios (OR) for persistent and adolescent-onset depressive symptoms, compared with no symptoms, per s.d. increase in IL-6 levels at baseline. First, we report unadjusted ORs. Second, we report ORs after adjusting for total IQ scores at age 8 years. This is because we wanted to examine whether evidence for association between IL-6 and persistent depressive symptoms remained after taking into account general intelligence. Third, we report ORs after adjusting for childhood psychological and behavioural problems, i.e. SDQ total difficulties score at age 7 years. Childhood psychological and behavioural problems are associated with lower IQ (Horwood et al. Reference Horwood2008), higher levels of inflammatory markers (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014a), and increased risk of depression and other psychiatric disorders subsequently in childhood and adulthood (Rutter et al. Reference Rutter, Kim-Cohen and Maughan2006). Therefore, we wanted to examine whether evidence for association between IL-6 and persistent depressive symptoms remained after taking into account psychological and behavioural problems preceding the measurement of IL-6. Finally, we report ORs after adjusting for all potential confounders, which are age, sex, father's occupation, ethnicity, maternal EPDS score, SDQ total difficulties score, total IQ and BMI (see assessment of covariates for details). We followed the same analytic approach when CRP and IQ were used as predictor. To test whether childhood IQ and IL-6 were independent predictors of subsequent depressive symptoms we controlled the multinomial regression model of IQ for IL-6 and vice versa. Linearity of association between IQ and persistent depressive symptoms was tested by including a quadratic term (IQ2) in the regression models; the same approach was used for IL-6 using IL-62 as quadratic term.

Effect of initial depressive symptom level

Population sub-groups identified by LCA reflect initial differences in depressive symptom severity at age 10 years as well as the subsequent course until age 19 years. To test whether any association with inflammation and IQ pertain to initial difference in symptom severity or subsequent course of depressive symptoms, we controlled multinomial logistic regression models for total SMFQ score at 10 years.

Relationship between IQ and inflammatory markers

Linear regression examined the relationship between total IQ and serum IL-6, CRP levels using natural log-transformed values for the inflammatory markers which were normally distributed. We calculated the beta-coefficient and 95% confidence interval for change in serum inflammatory marker levels, in standard deviation, for each standard deviation increase in IQ. Regression models were adjusted for age at blood collection, sex, social class and ethnicity.

Moderating and mediating effect of IL-6

We tested for any interaction between IQ and IL-6 by including an interaction term (IQxIL-6) in multinomial logistic regression models using IQ and IL-6 as predictor and persistent depressive symptoms as outcome. We also examined whether IL-6 levels at 9 years mediated the relationship between IQ at 8 years and persistent depressive symptoms at 10–19 years based on sample with data on all three variables. Mediation analysis was carried out using logistic regression (MPlus software); persistent depressive symptoms was used as outcome (binary variable), IQ was used as predictor (continuous variable), and IL-6 was used as mediator (continuous variable). The analyses compared persistent depressive symptoms with no symptoms; participants with adolescent-onset symptoms were excluded. We calculated regression co-efficient and standard error for direct and indirect effect of IQ on risk of persistent depressive symptoms. Presence of a significant indirect effect would indicate mediation of association between IQ and persistent depressive symptoms by IL-6.

Sex difference

We examined whether sex differences applied to the association between the adolescent depressive sub-groups and childhood biomarkers. First, we calculated the ORs for persistent depressive symptoms for each childhood biomarker separately for two sexes using multinomial regression as above. Second, we included a sex × biomarker (separately for IQ and IL-6) interaction term in the multinomial regression models based on total sample.

Results

Population sub-groups of depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years

Total SMFQ score and the percentage of subjects with a high burden of depressive symptoms (total SMFQ score 11 or above) increased with age between 10 and 19 years (Table 1).

Table 1. Total SMFQ Score between ages 10 and 19 years in the ALSPAC cohort

a Independent sample Kruskal–Wallis test used to compare distribution of SMFQ scores between male and female groups.

After latent class analysis of longitudinal data from 9156 participants a three class model was chosen based on the BIC (Fig. 1). These groups were driven by the probability of high depressive symptoms, defined as SMFQ score 11 or above, at the six follow-up points. The BIC for a two latent class model was 22 582, for a three class model was 22 483, and for a four class model was 22 500.

Fig. 1. Three Class Model of Depressive Symptoms between 10 and 19 years in the ALSPAC cohort. Note: Class membership is driven by the probability of high depressive symptoms, defined as total SMFQ score 11 or above, at six follow-up points. Class 1 = consistently low symptoms at all six follow-ups (‘No symptoms’). Class 2 = symptoms low in the first two follow-ups but increase subsequently (‘Adolescent-onset symptoms’). Class 3 = consistently high symptoms at all six follow-ups (‘Persistent symptoms’).

In the three-class model, class one had consistently low depressive symptoms at all six follow-ups between 10 and 19 years (hereafter, ‘no symptoms’). In class two depressive symptoms were low in the first two follow-ups at 10 and 13 years but increased subsequently (hereafter, ‘adolescent-onset symptoms’). Class three had consistently high depressive symptoms at all six follow-ups (hereafter ‘persistent symptoms’). Therefore, the groups represent individuals defined by consistently low levels of depressive symptoms, occasionally high levels, and consistently high levels of depressive symptoms.

Baseline sociodemographic characteristics and population sub-groups of depressive symptoms

Sociodemographic characteristics, distributions of maternal EPDS score at 8 weeks postpartum, SDQ score at age 7, IQ and inflammatory markers for the total sample have been presented in Table 2. The population sub-groups of depressive symptoms differed significantly from each other in known sociodemographic and other correlates of depression. Compared with the group with no symptoms, the groups with depressive symptoms were more likely to comprise women, had lower socio-economic background, higher SDQ score at 7 years, and higher maternal post-natal depression score (Table 2).

Table 2. Sociodemographic and other correlates of depressive symptoms

s.d., standard deviation; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire ; BMI, body mass index; IQR, interquartile range; pg/mL, picogram per millilitre; mg/L, milligram per litre.

a One way analysis of variance for continuous data (age, mother's depression score, SDQ score, body mass index and mean values of IQ, IL-6 and CRP); Chi-squared test for proportions (male sex, British White ethnicity and father's occupation); independent samples Kruskal–Wallis test for IL-6 and CRP distributions (median).

Association between IQ at age 8 years and persistent depressive symptoms from 10 to 19 years

Childhood IQ was associated with subsequent persistent (but not adolescent-onset) depressive symptoms. The risk of being in the persistent depressive symptoms group between 10 and 19 years relative to the no symptoms group was 27% lower for each 1-s.d. increase in IQ at 8 years. After adjusting for potential confounders the association attenuated slightly, but evidence for an association remained (Table 3; online Supplementary Fig. S1). See online Supplementary Table S1 for fully adjusted model showing ORs for each covariate. The quadratic term (IQ2) in the multinomial regression model was not significant (p = 1.00) suggesting a linear dose-response relationship between childhood IQ and subsequent persistent depressive symptoms. The association between IQ and persistent depressive symptoms remained significant after controlling for IL-6, and vice versa (see Tables 3 and 4), suggesting that IQ and IL-6 are independent predictors of subsequent persistent depressive symptoms.

Table 3. ORs for persistent and adolescent-onset depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years for each s.d. increase in total IQ score at age 8 years

a Analysis was controlled for age at assessment of IQ, sex at birth, father's occupational status at birth, ethnicity at birth, maternal Edinburgh Post-natal Depression Scale (EPDS) total score at 8-week post-partum, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) total difficulties score at age 7 years, serum IL-6 concentration at age 9 years. Please see section ‘assessment of covariates’ for more information.

Table 4. ORs for Persistent and adolescent-onset depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years for each s.d. increase in IL-6, CRP at age 9 years

a Analysis was controlled for age at assessment of IL-6, sex at birth, father's occupational status at birth, ethnicity at birth, maternal Edinburgh Post-natal Depression Scale (EPDS) total score at 8-week post-partum, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) total difficulties score at age 7 years, total IQ score at 8 years, and BMI at age 9 years. Please see section ‘assessment of covariates’ for more information.

Association between IL-6 and CRP levels at age 9 years and persistent depressive symptoms from 10 to 19 years

Higher IL-6 levels at 9 years were associated with subsequent persistent depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years. The adjusted OR for being in the persistent depressive symptoms group relative to the no symptoms group for each 1-s.d. increase in IL-6 levels was 1.20 (95% CI, 1.03–1.39) (Table 4; online Supplementary Fig. S1). The quadratic term (IL-62) in the multinomial regression model was not significant (p = 0.80) suggesting a linear dose–response relationship between childhood IL-6 levels and subsequent persistent depressive symptoms. There was no evidence for an association between CRP and persistent depressive symptoms. See online Supplementary Tables S2 and S3 for fully adjusted models for IL-6 and CRP showing ORs for each covariate.

Effect of psychological and behavioural problems at age 7 years

Evidence of associations between childhood IQ at 8 years, IL-6 at 9 years and subsequent persistent depressive symptoms at 10–19 years all persisted after adjusting for SDQ score at age 7 (Tables 3 and 4), suggesting that the associations between persistent depressive symptoms, childhood IQ and IL-6 levels were independent of the effect of past psychological and behavioural problems on these biomarkers.

Effect of initial depressive symptom level at age 10 years

The OR for persistent depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years for childhood IL-6 at 9 years remained significant after controlling for total SMFQ score at 10 years; adjusted OR = 1.16 (95% CI, 1.03–1.31); p = 0.018. This suggests that childhood IL-6 levels at 9 years predict the course of depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years independently of initial symptom severity at age 10 years. However, the OR for persistent depressive symptoms from 10 to 19 years for childhood IQ at 8 years became non-significant after controlling for total SMFQ score at 10 years; adjusted OR = 0.95 (95% CI, 0.85–1.08); p = 0.484.

Association between total IQ and serum IL-6, CRP level

Higher total IQ scores at age 8 years were associated with lower serum CRP levels at age 9 years; β = −0.071 (95% CI, −1.00, −0.041); p < 0.001. Evidence for this association remained after adjusting for age, sex, father's occupation and ethnicity; adjusted β = −0.048 (95% CI, −0.080, −0.016); p = 0.003. Similarly, higher total IQ scores at age 8 years was associated with lower serum IL-6 levels at age 9 years; β = −0.046 (95% CI, −0.077, −0.015); p = 0.004. However, this association was attenuated after adjusting for age, sex, father's occupation and ethnicity; adjusted β = −0.024 (95% CI, −0.050, 0.010); p = 0.163.

Moderating and mediating effects of IL-6 on the IQ-persistent depressive symptoms relationship

There was no evidence for any interaction between IQ and IL-6 in relation to risk of persistent depressive symptoms (p value for interaction term = 0.149). With regards to results of mediation analysis, based on 3350 participants the direct effect of IQ on persistent depressive symptoms was significant indicating an association between IQ and persistent depressive symptoms; regression coefficient for direct effect = −0.188, s.e. = 0.037; p < 0.001. However, there was no evidence that IL-6 at 9 years mediated the relationship between IQ at 8 years and persistent depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years; regression co-efficient for indirect effect = −0.004, s.e. = 0.002; p = 0.073. The regression coefficient for total effect (a sum of indirect and direct effect) was −0.192, s.e. = 0.037; p < 0.001.

Sex difference in the association between IQ, IL-6 and persistent depressive symptoms

In analyses stratified by sex, childhood IQ was associated with subsequent persistent depressive symptoms in both sexes (see online Supplementary Table S4). For IL-6, although the ORs were similar between sexes that for men was no longer statistically significant. However, no tests for interaction between IQ and sex and IL-6 and sex in the respective regression models approached statistical significance (both p > 0.10), suggesting no significant sex difference in the overserved associations between IQ, IL-6 and persistent depressive symptoms.

Discussion

Using population-based longitudinal birth cohort data, we defined a population sub-group of young people characterised by persistent depressive symptoms during the second decade of life. Lower IQ at 8 years, a measure of neurodevelopment, and higher IL-6 at 9 years, a measure of inflammation, were independently associated with increased risk of subsequent persistent (but not adolescent-onset) depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years in a linear, dose–response fashion. Evidence for an association persisted after controlling for a number of potential confounders including sex, body mass, social class, ethnicity, maternal post-natal depression and psychological and behavioural problems measured before the biomarkers at 7 years. To our knowledge this is one of the first longitudinal studies of childhood IQ, inflammatory markers and subsequent persistent depressive symptoms. The results are consistent with potentially important roles for inflammation and neurodevelopment in the aetiology of persistent depressive symptoms, and indicate important differences between adolescent-onset and persistent depressive symptoms during the second decade of life.

Findings from the current study are consistent with previous longitudinal studies reporting an association between adult depression and lower IQ in childhood (van Os et al. Reference van Os1997; Zammit et al. Reference Zammit2004; Koenen et al. Reference Koenen2009). The results are also consistent with previous longitudinal studies showing an association between adult depression, higher IL-6 levels in childhood (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014a) and in adulthood (Zalli et al. Reference Zalli2016). Poorer childhood IQ has been associated with increased risk of adult schizophrenia (Jones et al. Reference Jones1994; David et al. Reference David1997; Davidson et al. Reference Davidson1999; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon2000; Cannon et al. Reference Cannon2002; Woodberry et al. Reference Woodberry, Giuliano and Seidman2008; Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2011), post-traumatic stress disorder (Koenen et al. Reference Koenen2007; Kremen et al. Reference Kremen2007), anxiety (Koenen et al. Reference Koenen2009) and substance misuse disorders (Mortensen et al. Reference Mortensen2005). It has been suggested that general cognitive ability as measured by IQ may be a marker of the integrity of the nervous system (Whalley & Deary, Reference Whalley and Deary2001) and of cognitive reserve (Barnett et al. Reference Barnett2006), which might underlie the association between childhood IQ and subsequent risk of mental illness. Association with lower IQ indicates a neurodevelopmental component to depressive symptoms persistent during late childhood and teenage years.

Accumulating evidence implicates the immune system, particularly low-grade systemic inflammation defined by elevated concentrations of circulating inflammatory markers, in the pathogenesis of depression, psychosis and other major mental disorders (Raison et al. Reference Raison, Capuron and Miller2006; Dantzer et al. Reference Dantzer2008; Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2015). CRP is an archetypal acute phase protein and IL-6 is a pleotropic proinflammatory cytokine, both of which have been studied extensively in depression and shown to be elevated in acutely unwell patients compared with controls (Howren et al. Reference Howren, Lamkin and Suls2009; Haapakoski et al. Reference Haapakoski2015; Goldsmith et al. Reference Goldsmith, Rapaport and Miller2016). Longitudinal studies including our own have shown that elevated IL-6 or CRP levels are associated with increased risks of developing depression and psychosis in future (Gimeno et al. Reference Gimeno2009; Wium-Andersen et al. Reference Wium-Andersen, Orsted and Nordestgaard2014; Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014a; Metcalf et al. Reference Metcalf2017). The current study adds to the existing evidence by showing that childhood IL-6 levels are associated with subsequent persistent depressive symptoms, which might indicate that low-grade systemic inflammation contribute to development and persistence of depressive symptoms.

The current study is distinct from previous studies from the ALSPAC birth cohort that have examined the relationship between inflammation and psychiatric symptoms. Childhood IL-6 levels at 9 years have been reported to be associated with atopic disorders (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014c), depressive and psychotic symptoms assessed at 18 years (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2014a), and hypomanic symptoms assessed at age 22 years (Hayes et al. Reference Hayes2016). Another study has reported association between serum CRP levels and generalised anxiety disorder both assessed at age 15 years (Khandaker et al. Reference Khandaker2016). None of the previous studies concerned persistent depressive symptoms between ages 10 and 19.

The use of a general population birth cohort, relatively large sample, and prospective assessments for depressive symptoms, IQ and inflammatory markers are some of the strengths of the study. Limitations include missing data. The number of respondents with missing data for SMFQ increased in successive follow-ups between 10 and 19 years. Only about 1700 participants completed the questionnaire at all six follow-up points, a group unlikely to be representative of the general population. Therefore, in order to avoid any bias from missing data we included individuals who completed at least one out of six SMFQ questionnaires between 10 and 19 years for the calculation of latent classes. In constructing the latent classes of depressive symptoms we deliberately used dichotomised SMFQ scores at six follow-up points as opposed to SMFQ total scores or item level data. This is because we were interested in the evolution of clinically relevant depressive symptoms during critical period of brain development; the threshold used to dichotomise SMFQ data (total score 11 or above) has been reported to represent burden of depressive symptoms that may be clinically relevant (Angold et al. Reference Angold, Costello and Messer1995).

We did not have any data on depression at the time of IQ or IL-6 measurements at ages 8 and 9 years, respectively, so the possibility of reverse causality cannot be ruled out. It is plausible that persistent depressive symptoms between ages 10 and 19 represent continuation of depressive symptoms that started earlier in childhood before the assessment of the biomarkers. However, the results remained significant after controlling for SDQ score at 7 years providing some reassurance that reverse causality is unlikely to be the sole explanation for the observed results.

We acknowledge any diurnal variation in serum IL-6 and CRP concentrations may not be adequately captured by non-fasting blood samples (de Jager et al. Reference de Jager2009). However, the use of non-fasting samples would increase measurement error likely to be random in relation to the outcome. No other blood samples from childhood or adolescence were available to examine long-term within-individual consistency of inflammatory marker levels. However, one study of healthy volunteers found no significant changes in serum IL-6 levels over a 3-year period (Knudsen et al. Reference Knudsen2008). Similarly, a large meta-analysis of population-based studies of coronary heart disease has found that values for CRP are sufficiently stable over time (Danesh et al. Reference Danesh2004). There was some evidence that higher IQ at baseline was associated with subsequent lower inflammatory marker levels. However, an opposite effect, i.e. inflammation influencing cognitive function in young people, is also plausible. In future prospective studies with data on inflammatory markers at baseline and cognitive function in follow-up are needed to examine this hypothesis. Childhood and adolescent infection/inflammation affect adolescent neurodevelopment, which is further complicated by malnutrition (Galler et al. Reference Galler, Koethe and Yolken2017; John et al. Reference John, Black and Nelson2017). While this is particularly relevant for low and middle-income countries, and that serious malnutrition is unlikely to be a major issue in a contemporary UK cohort, in future studies should examine the relationship between inflammation, nutrition and development.

In summary, we report one of the first longitudinal studies of childhood IQ, inflammatory markers and subsequent persistent depressive symptoms during the second decade of life. The findings indicate important roles for two distinct biological processes, inflammation and neurodevelopment, in the aetiology of persistent depressive symptoms.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003038.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all families who took part in this study, midwives for their help in recruitment, and the whole ALSPAC team, including interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses. Dr Khandaker is supported by an Intermediate Clinical Fellowship from the Wellcome Trust (201486/Z/16/Z) and a Clinical Lecturer Starter Grant from the Academy of Medical Sciences, UK (grant no. 80354). Professor Jones acknowledges grant support from the Wellcome Trust (095844/Z/11/Z & 088869/Z/09/Z) and NIHR (RP-PG-0606-1335, Cambridge Biomedical Research Centre and CLAHRC East of England). Dr Stochl is supported by the UK Medical Research Council (MRC award ref: MR/K006665/1) and by the NIHR CLAHRC East of England. The UK Medical Research Council (Grant ref: 74882), the Wellcome Trust (Grant ref: 092731) and the University of Bristol provide core support for the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort.

Declaration of Interest

None of the authors has any conflicts of interest to declare.

References

Angold, A, Costello, EJ and Messer, SC (1995) Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression in children and adolescents. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research 5, 237249.Google Scholar
Barnett, JH et al. (2006) Cognitive reserve in neuropsychiatry. Psychological Medicine 36, 10531064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, A et al. (2013) Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s’ – the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International Journal of Epidemiology 42, 111127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, M et al. (2002) Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 59, 449456.Google Scholar
Cannon, TD et al. (2000) Childhood cognitive functioning in schizophrenia patients and their unaffected siblings: a prospective cohort study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 26, 379393.Google Scholar
Collins, LM and Lanza, ST (2010) Latent Class and Latent Transition Analysis: With Applications in the Social, Behavioral, and Health Sciences, pp. 287–295. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Colman, I et al. (2007) A longitudinal typology of symptoms of depression and anxiety over the life course. Biological Psychiatry 62, 12651271.Google Scholar
Costello, EJ et al. (1982) The National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC). NIMH, Bethesda, MD, USA.Google Scholar
Danesh, J et al. (2004) C-reactive protein and other circulating markers of inflammation in the prediction of coronary heart disease. New England Journal of Medicine 350, 13871397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dantzer, R et al. (2008) From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9, 4656.Google Scholar
David, AS et al. (1997) IQ and risk for schizophrenia: a population-based cohort study. Psychological Medicine 27, 13111323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, M et al. (1999) Behavioral and intellectual markers for schizophrenia in apparently healthy male adolescents. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 13281335.Google Scholar
de Jager, W et al. (2009) Prerequisites for cytokine measurements in clinical trials with multiplex immunoassays. BMC Immunology 10, 52.Google Scholar
Finch, AJ and Chihldress, WB (1975) A comparison of WISC selected subtest short forms with MR children. Mental Retardation 13, 2021.Google Scholar
Galler, JR, Koethe, JR and Yolken, RH (2017) Neurodevelopment: the impact of nutrition and inflammation during adolescence in low-resource settings. Pediatrics Supplement 139, s72s84.Google Scholar
Gimeno, D et al. (2009) Associations of C-reactive protein and interleukin-6 with cognitive symptoms of depression: 12-year follow-up of the Whitehall II study. Psychological Medicine 39, 413423.Google Scholar
Gogtay, N et al. (2004) Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101, 81748179.Google Scholar
Goldsmith, DR, Rapaport, MH and Miller, BJ (2016) A meta-analysis of blood cytokine network alterations in psychiatric patients: comparisons between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and depression. Molecular Psychiatry 21, 16961709.Google Scholar
Goodman, R (1997) The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38, 581586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haapakoski, R et al. (2015) Cumulative meta-analysis of interleukins 6 and 1beta, tumour necrosis factor alpha and C-reactive protein in patients with major depressive disorder. Brain Behaviour and Immunity 49, 206215.Google Scholar
Harrison, NA et al. (2009) Inflammation causes mood changes through alterations in subgenual cingulate activity and mesolimbic connectivity. Biological Psychiatry 66, 407414.Google Scholar
Hayes, JF et al. (2016) Childhood interleukin-6, C-reactive protein and atopic disorders as risk factors for hypomanic symptoms in young adulthood: a longitudinal birth cohort study. Psychological Medicine 47, 2333.Google Scholar
Horwood, J et al. (2008) IQ and non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. British Journal of Psychiatry 193, 185191.Google Scholar
Howren, MB, Lamkin, DM and Suls, J (2009) Associations of depression with C-reactive protein, IL-1, and IL-6: a meta-analysis. Psychosomatic Medicine 71, 171186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iwashyna, TJ et al. (2010) Long-term cognitive impairment and functional disability among survivors of severe sepsis. JAMA 304, 17871794.Google Scholar
John, CC, Black, MM and Nelson, CA III (2017) Neurodevelopment: the impact of nutrition and inflammation during early to middle childhood in low-resource settings. Pediatrics Supplement 139, s59s71.Google ScholarPubMed
Jones, P et al. (1994) Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort. Lancet 344, 13981402.Google Scholar
Jones, PB (2013) Adult mental health disorders and their age at onset. British Journal of Psychiatry Supplement 54, s5s10.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC et al. (2003) The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the national comorbidity survey replication (NCS-R). JAMA 289, 30953105.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC et al. (2005) Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Archives of General Psychiatry 62, 593602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khandaker, GM et al. (2011) A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 132, 220227.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM et al. (2015) Inflammation and immunity in schizophrenia: implications for pathophysiology and treatment. The Lancet Psychiatry 2, 258270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khandaker, GM et al. (2014 a). Association of serum interleukin 6 and C-reactive protein in childhood with depression and psychosis in young adult life: a population-based longitudinal study. JAMA Psychiatry 71, 11211128.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM et al. (2014 b). A population-based longitudinal study of childhood neurodevelopmental disorders, IQ and subsequent risk of psychotic experiences in adolescence. Psychological Medicine 44, 32293238.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM et al. (2014 c). A population-based study of atopic disorders and inflammatory markers in childhood before psychotic experiences in adolescence. Schizophrenia Research 152, 139145.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM et al. (2016) Association between serum C-reactive protein and DSM-IV generalized anxiety disorder in adolescence: findings from the ALSPAC cohort. Neurobiology of Stress 4, 5561.Google Scholar
Khandaker, GM et al. (2013) Prenatal maternal infection, neurodevelopment and adult schizophrenia: a systematic review of population-based studies. Psychological Medicine 43, 239257.Google Scholar
Knudsen, LS et al. (2008) Pre-analytical and biological variability in circulating interleukin 6 in healthy subjects and patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Biomarkers 13, 5978.Google Scholar
Koenen, KC et al. (2007) Early childhood factors associated with the development of post-traumatic stress disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Psychological Medicine 37, 181192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenen, KC et al. (2009) Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: a test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. American Journal of Psychiatry 166, 5057.Google Scholar
Kremen, WS et al. (2007) Pretrauma cognitive ability and risk for posttraumatic stress disorder: a twin study. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 361368.Google Scholar
Merikangas, KR et al. (2003) Longitudinal trajectories of depression and anxiety in a prospective community study: the Zurich cohort study. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 9931000.Google Scholar
Metcalf, SA et al. (2017) Serum C-reactive protein in adolescence and risk of schizophrenia in adulthood: a prospective birth cohort study. Brain Behaviour and Immunity 59, 253259.Google Scholar
Mortensen, EL et al. (2005) IQ and mental disorder in young men. British Journal of Psychiatry 187, 407415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owens, M et al. (2014) Elevated morning cortisol is a stratified population-level biomarker for major depression in boys only with high depressive symptoms. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111, 36383643.Google Scholar
Raison, CL, Capuron, L and Miller, AH (2006) Cytokines sing the blues: inflammation and the pathogenesis of depression. Trends in Immunology 27, 2431.Google Scholar
Reichenberg, A et al. (2001) Cytokine-associated emotional and cognitive disturbances in humans. Archives of General Psychiatry 58, 445452.Google Scholar
Rutter, M, Kim-Cohen, J and Maughan, B (2006) Continuities and discontinuities in psychopathology between childhood and adult life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 47, 276295.Google Scholar
Schwarz, GE (1978) Estimating the dimension of a model. Annals of Statistics 6, 461464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sekar, A et al. (2016) Schizophrenia risk from complex variation of complement component 4. Nature 530, 177183.Google Scholar
Sharp, C, Goodyer, IM and Croudace, TJ (2006) The short mood and feelings questionnaire (SMFQ): a unidimensional item response theory and categorical data factor analysis of self-report ratings from a community sample of 7-through 11-year-old children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 34, 379391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stricker, G, Merbaum, M and Tangeman, P (1968) WAIS short forms, information transmission and approximation of full scale IQ. Journal of Clinical Psychology 25, 170172.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thapar, A and McGuffin, P (1998) Validity of the shortened mood and feelings questionnaire in a community sample of children and adolescents: a preliminary research note. Psychiatry Research 81, 259268.Google Scholar
van Os, J et al. (1997) Developmental precursors of affective illness in a general population birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 54, 625631.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D, Golombok, S and Rust, J (1992) Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edn. (WISC–III UK). The Psychological Corporation, Sidcup.Google Scholar
Whalley, LJ and Deary, IJ (2001) Longitudinal cohort study of childhood IQ and survival up to age 76. BMJ 322, 819.Google Scholar
Wium-Andersen, MK, Orsted, DD and Nordestgaard, BG (2014) Elevated C-reactive protein associated with late- and very-late-onset schizophrenia in the general population: a prospective study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 40, 11171127.Google Scholar
Woodberry, KA, Giuliano, AJ and Seidman, LJ (2008) Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. American Journal of Psychiatry 165, 579587.Google Scholar
Zalli, A et al. (2016) Low-grade inflammation predicts persistence of depressive symptoms. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 233, 16691678.Google Scholar
Zammit, S et al. (2004) A longitudinal study of premorbid IQ score and risk of developing schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, severe depression, and other nonaffective psychoses. Archives of General Psychiatry 61, 354360.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Total SMFQ Score between ages 10 and 19 years in the ALSPAC cohort

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Three Class Model of Depressive Symptoms between 10 and 19 years in the ALSPAC cohort. Note: Class membership is driven by the probability of high depressive symptoms, defined as total SMFQ score 11 or above, at six follow-up points. Class 1 = consistently low symptoms at all six follow-ups (‘No symptoms’). Class 2 = symptoms low in the first two follow-ups but increase subsequently (‘Adolescent-onset symptoms’). Class 3 = consistently high symptoms at all six follow-ups (‘Persistent symptoms’).

Figure 2

Table 2. Sociodemographic and other correlates of depressive symptoms

Figure 3

Table 3. ORs for persistent and adolescent-onset depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years for each s.d. increase in total IQ score at age 8 years

Figure 4

Table 4. ORs for Persistent and adolescent-onset depressive symptoms between 10 and 19 years for each s.d. increase in IL-6, CRP at age 9 years

Supplementary material: File

Khandaker et al supplementary material

Tables S1-S4

Download Khandaker et al supplementary material(File)
File 28.6 KB