Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:13:31.641Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Memory and metamemory in schizophrenia: a liberal acceptance account of psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 January 2008

S. Moritz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Centre Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
T. S. Woodward*
Affiliation:
Department of Research, Riverview Hospital, Coquitlam, BC, Canada Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
L. Jelinek
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Centre Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
R. Klinge
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University Medical Centre Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany
*
*Address for correspondence: Dr T. S. Woodward, Department of Research, Riverview Hospital, 2601 Lougheed Highway, Coquitlam, BC, Canada. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

In previous studies we suggested that liberal acceptance (LA) represents a fundamental cognitive bias in schizophrenia and may explain why patients are more willing to accept weak response alternatives and display overconfidence in incorrect responses. The aim of the present study was to test a central assumption of the LA account: false alarms in schizophrenia should be particularly increased when the distractor–target resemblance is weak relative to a control group.

Method

Sixty-eight schizophrenia patients were compared to 25 healthy controls on a visual memory task. At encoding, participants studied eight complex displays, each consisting of a unique pairing of four stimulus attributes: symbol, shape, position and colour. At recognition, studied items were presented along with distractors that resembled the targets to varying degrees (i.e. the match between distractors and targets ranged from one to three attributes). Participants were required to make old/new judgements graded for confidence.

Results

The hypotheses were confirmed: false recognition was increased for patients compared to controls for weakly and moderately related distractors only, whereas strong lure items induced similar levels of false recognition for both groups. In accordance with prior research, patients displayed a significantly reduced confidence gap and enhanced knowledge corruption compared to controls. Finally, higher neuroleptic dosage was related to a decreased number of high-confident ratings.

Conclusions

These data assert that LA is a core mechanism contributing to both enhanced acceptance of weakly supported response alternatives and metamemory deficits, and this may be linked to the emergence of positive symptomatology.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aleman, A, Hijman, R, de Haan, EH, Kahn, RS (1999). Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 13581366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benkert, O, Hippius, H (2003). Compendium of Psychiatric Pharmacotherapy, 4th edn [in German]. Springer: Berlin.Google Scholar
Bleuler, E (1911). Dementia praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien. Franz Deuticke: Leipzig.Google Scholar
Brébion, G, Amador, X, Smith, MDM, Sharif, Z, Gorman, JM (2000). Depression, psychomotor retardation, negative symptoms, and memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology 13, 177183.Google ScholarPubMed
Colbert, SM, Peters, ER (2002). Need for closure and jumping-to-conclusions in delusion-prone individuals. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 190, 2731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danion, J-M, Kazes, M, Huron, C, Karchouni, N (2003). Do patients with schizophrenia consciously recollect emotional events better than neutral events? American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 18791881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Danion, J-M, Rizzo, L, Bruant, A (1999). Functional mechanisms underlying impaired recognition memory and conscious awareness in patients with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 56, 639644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elvevåg, B, Fisher, JE, Weickert, TW, Weinberger, DR, Goldberg, TE (2004). Lack of false recognition in schizophrenia: a consequence of poor memory? Neuropsychologia 42, 546554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fioravanti, M, Carlone, O, Vitale, B, Cinti, ME, Clare, L (2005). A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in adults with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology Review 15, 7395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinrichs, RW, Zakzanis, KK (1998). Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: a quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology 12, 426445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huron, C, Danion, J-M (2002). Impairment of constructive memory in schizophrenia. International Clinical Psychopharmacology 17, 127133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huron, C, Danion, J-M, Giacomoni, F, Grengé, D, Robert, P, Rizzo, L (1995). Impairment of recognition memory with, but not without, conscious recollection in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 152, 17371742.Google Scholar
Kay, SR, Opler, LA, Lindenmayer, J-P (1989). The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS). Rationale and standardisation. British Journal of Psychiatry 155 (Suppl. 7), 5965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kircher, TT, Koch, K, Stottmeister, F, Durst, V (2007). Metacognition and reflexivity in patients with schizophrenia. Psychopathology 40, 254260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klosterkotter, J (1992). The meaning of basic symptoms for the genesis of the schizophrenic nuclear syndrome. Japanese Journal of Psychiatry and Neurology 46, 609630.Google ScholarPubMed
Lehrl, S (1995). Multiple Choice Vocabulary Intelligence Test: MWT-B. Perimed-spitta: Balingen.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Heeren, D, Andresen, B, Krausz, M (2001). An analysis of the specificity and the syndromal correlates of verbal memory impairments in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 101, 2331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS (2004). Plausibility judgment in schizophrenic patients: evidence for a liberal acceptance bias. German Journal of Psychiatry 7, 6674.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS (2005). Jumping to conclusions in delusional and non-delusional schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology 44, 193207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS (2006 a). The contribution of metamemory deficits to schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 115, 1525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS (2006 b). Metacognitive control over false memories: a key determinant of delusional thinking. Current Psychiatry Reports 8, 184190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS (2007). Metacognitive training in schizophrenia: from basic research to knowledge translation and intervention. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 20, 619625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS, Burlon, M (2005 a). Metacognitive Skill Training for Patients with Schizophrenia. Manual. VanHam Campus Verlag: Hamburg.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS, Cuttler, C, Whitman, JC, Watson, JM (2004). False memories in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 18, 276283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS, Hausmann, D (2006 a). Incautious reasoning as a pathogenetic factor for the development of psychotic symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 32, 327331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS, Metacognition Study Group (2007). Metacognitive Training for Patients with Schizophrenia (MCT). Manual, 2nd edn. VanHam Campus Verlag: Hamburg.Google Scholar
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS, Rodriguez-Raecke, R (2006 b). Patients with schizophrenia do not produce more false memories than controls but are more confident in them. Psychological Medicine 36, 659667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS, Ruff, C (2003). Source monitoring and memory confidence in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 33, 131139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, S, Woodward, TS, Whitman, JC, Cuttler, C (2005 b). Confidence in errors as a possible basis for delusions in schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 193, 916.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehan, DV, Lecrubier, Y, Sheehan, KH, Amorim, P, Janavs, J, Weiller, E, Hergueta, T, Baker, R, Dunbar, G (1998). The MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59 (Suppl. 20), 2233.Google ScholarPubMed
Weiss, AP, Dodson, CS, Goff, DC, Schacter, DL, Heckers, S (2002). Intact suppression of increased false recognition in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 15061513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, TS, Moritz, S, Arnold, MM, Cuttler, C, Whitman, JC, Lindsay, DS (2006 a). Increased hindsight bias in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 20, 461467.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, TS, Moritz, S, Cuttler, C, Whitman, JC (2006 b). The contribution of a cognitive bias against disconfirmatory evidence (BADE) to delusions in schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 28, 605617.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed