Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T20:06:25.208Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Altered social and non-social decision-making in recreational and dependent cocaine users

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 July 2013

L. M. Hulka
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
C. Eisenegger
Affiliation:
Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
K. H. Preller
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
M. Vonmoos
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
D. Jenni
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
K. Bendrick
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany
M. R. Baumgartner
Affiliation:
Institute of Legal Medicine, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
E. Seifritz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
B. B. Quednow*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland
*
*Address for correspondence: B. B. Quednow, Ph.D., Experimental and Clinical Pharmacopsychology, Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, Lenggstrasse 31, CH-8032 Zurich, Switzerland. (Email: [email protected])

Abstract

Background

Maladaptive decision-making is assumed to be a core feature of cocaine addiction. Indeed, numerous studies have reported deficits in non-social decision-making tasks and reward-related impulsivity in dependent cocaine users. However, social decision-making has not been examined in cocaine users yet. Moreover, it is unknown if even recreational and non-dependent cocaine use is linked to decision-making deficits. Therefore, we investigated whether recreational and dependent cocaine users exhibit alterations in social and non-social decision-making.

Method

The performance of healthy controls (n = 68), recreational cocaine users (n = 68) and dependent cocaine users (n = 30) in classical decision-making paradigms (Iowa Gambling Task, Delay Discounting) and in social interaction paradigms (Distribution Game, Dictator Game) was assessed.

Results

Decisions in the social interaction tasks of both cocaine user groups were more self-serving compared with controls as cocaine users preferred higher monetary payoffs for themselves. In the Iowa Gambling Task, only dependent cocaine users were more likely to choose disadvantageous card decks, reflecting worse decision-making. They were also more likely to choose immediate smaller rewards over larger delayed rewards in the Delay Discounting task.

Conclusions

Our results imply that both recreational and dependent cocaine users are more concerned with their own monetary gain when interacting with another person. Furthermore, primarily dependent cocaine users are less foresighted and more impulsive regarding immediate reward. Overall, social interaction deficits are already present in recreational users, while non-social decision-making deficits occur predominantly in dependent cocaine users. Thus, social interaction training and cognitive remediation strategies may improve treatment success and quality of life in cocaine dependence.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainslie, G (1975). Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychological Bulletin 82, 463496.Google Scholar
Bechara, A, Dolan, S, Hindes, A (2002). Decision-making and addiction (part II): myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia 40, 16901705.Google Scholar
Beck, AT, Ward, CH, Mendelson, M, Mock, J, Erbaugh, J (1961). An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry 4, 561571.Google Scholar
Bickel, WK, Landes, RD, Christensen, DR, Jackson, L, Jones, BA, Kurth-Nelson, Z, Redish, AD (2011 a). Single- and cross-commodity discounting among cocaine addicts: the commodity and its temporal location determine discounting rate. Psychopharmacology 217, 177187.Google Scholar
Bickel, WK, Yi, R, Landes, RD, Hill, PF, Baxter, C (2011 b). Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biological Psychiatry 69, 260265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolla, KI, Cadet, JL, London, ED (1998). The neuropsychiatry of chronic cocaine abuse. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 10, 280289.Google Scholar
Bolla, KI, Eldreth, DA, London, ED, Kiehl, KA, Mouratidis, M, Contoreggi, C, Matochik, JA, Kurian, V, Cadet, JL, Kimes, AS, Funderburk, FR, Ernst, M (2003). Orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers performing a decision-making task. Neuroimage 19, 10851094.Google Scholar
Bolla, KI, Eldreth, DA, Matochik, JA, Cadet, JL (2004). Sex-related differences in a gambling task and its neurological correlates. Cerebral Cortex 14, 12261232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buttner, A (2012). Review: the neuropathology of drug abuse. Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology 37, 118134.Google Scholar
Casey, BJ, Somerville, LH, Gotlib, IH, Ayduk, O, Franklin, NT, Askren, MK, Jonides, J, Berman, MG, Wilson, NL, Teslovich, T, Glover, G, Zayas, V, Mischel, W, Shoda, Y (2011). Behavioral and neural correlates of delay of gratification 40 years later. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108, 1499815003.Google Scholar
Charness, G, Rabin, M (2002). Understanding social preferences with simple tests. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 817869.Google Scholar
Colzato, LS, Huizinga, M, Hommel, B (2009 a). Recreational cocaine polydrug use impairs cognitive flexibility but not working memory. Psychopharmacology 207, 225234.Google Scholar
Colzato, LS, van den Wildenberg, WP, Hommel, B (2007). Impaired inhibitory control in recreational cocaine users. PLoS One 2, 1143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colzato, LS, van den Wildenberg, WP, Hommel, B (2009 b). Reduced attentional scope in cocaine polydrug users. PLoS One 4, e6043.Google Scholar
Cooper, GA, Kronstrand, R, Kintz, P (2012). Society of Hair Testing guidelines for drug testing in hair. Forensic Science International 218, 2024.Google Scholar
Couture, SM, Penn, DL, Roberts, DL (2006). The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophrenia Bulletin 32, 4463.Google Scholar
Cunha, PJ, Bechara, A, de Andrade, AG, Nicastri, S (2011). Decision-making deficits linked to real-life social dysfunction in crack cocaine-dependent individuals. American Journal on Addictions 20, 7886.Google Scholar
Engelmann, D, Strobel, M (2004). Inequality aversion, efficiency, and maximin preferences in simple distribution experiments. American Economic Review 94, 857869.Google Scholar
European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction (2012). Annual report 2012: the state of the drugs problem in Europe. Publications Office of the European Union: Luxembourg.Google Scholar
Fehr, E, Camerer, CF (2007). Social neuroeconomics: the neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11, 419427.Google Scholar
Fellows, LK (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of human decision making: a review and conceptual framework. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 3, 159172.Google Scholar
Fischbacher, U (2007). z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments. Experimental Economics 10, 171178.Google Scholar
Fox, HC, Bergquist, KL, Casey, J, Hong, KA, Sinha, R (2011). Selective cocaine-related difficulties in emotional intelligence: relationship to stress and impulse control. American Journal on Addictions 20, 151160.Google Scholar
Haber, S (2008). Parallel and integrative processing through the basal ganglia reward circuit: lessons from addiction. Biological Psychiatry 64, 173174.Google Scholar
Heil, SH, Johnson, MW, Higgins, ST, Bickel, WK (2006). Delay discounting in currently using and currently abstinent cocaine-dependent outpatients and non-drug-using matched controls. Addictive Behaviors 31, 12901294.Google Scholar
Homer, BD, Solomon, TM, Moeller, RW, Mascia, A, DeRaleau, L, Halkitis, PN (2008). Methamphetamine abuse and impairment of social functioning: a review of the underlying neurophysiological causes and behavioral implications. Psychological Bulletin 134, 301310.Google Scholar
Hser, YI (1997). Self-reported drug use: results of selected empirical investigations of validity. NIDA Research Monograph 167, 320343.Google Scholar
Hulka, LM, Wagner, M, Preller, KH, Jenni, D, Quednow, BB (2013). Blue-yellow colour vision impairment and cognitive deficits in occasional and dependent stimulant users. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology 16, 535547.Google Scholar
Ivanov, I, Schulz, KP, London, ED, Newcorn, JH (2008). Inhibitory control deficits in childhood and risk for substance use disorders: a review. American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse 34, 239258.Google Scholar
Johnson, MW, Bickel, WK, Baker, F, Moore, BA, Badger, GJ, Budney, AJ (2010). Delay discounting in current and former marijuana-dependent individuals. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 18, 99107.Google Scholar
Jovanovski, D, Erb, S, Zakzanis, KK (2005). Neurocognitive deficits in cocaine users: a quantitative review of the evidence. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 27, 189204.Google Scholar
Kirby, KN, Petry, NM (2004). Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction 99, 461471.Google Scholar
Kirby, KN, Petry, NM, Bickel, WK (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 128, 7887.Google Scholar
Kishida, KT, King-Casas, B, Montague, PR (2010). Neuroeconomic approaches to mental disorders. Neuron 67, 543554.Google Scholar
Kjome, KL, Lane, SD, Schmitz, JM, Green, C, Ma, L, Prasla, I, Swann, AC, Moeller, FG (2010). Relationship between impulsivity and decision making in cocaine dependence. Psychiatry Research 178, 299304.Google Scholar
Koob, GF (2009). Dynamics of neuronal circuits in addiction: reward, antireward, and emotional memory. Pharmacopsychiatry 42, 3241.Google Scholar
Lehrl, S (1999). Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest (MWT-B) (Multiple Choice Vocabulary Intelligence Test). Hogrefe: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Leland, DS, Paulus, MP (2005). Increased risk-taking decision-making but not altered response to punishment in stimulant-using young adults. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 78, 8390.Google Scholar
Lindenmayer, JP, McGurk, SR, Khan, A, Kaushik, S, Thanju, A, Hoffman, L, Valdez, G, Wance, D, Herrmann, E (2013). Improving social cognition in schizophrenia: a pilot intervention combining computerized social cognition training with cognitive remediation. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39, 507517.Google Scholar
Lucantonio, F, Stalnaker, TA, Shaham, Y, Niv, Y, Schoenbaum, G (2012). The impact of orbitofrontal dysfunction on cocaine addiction. Nature Neuroscience 15, 358366.Google Scholar
Medalia, A, Saperstein, AM (2013). Does cognitive remediation for schizophrenia improve functional outcomes? Current Opinion in Psychiatry 26, 151157.Google Scholar
Mendez, IA, Simon, NW, Hart, N, Mitchell, MR, Nation, JR, Wellman, PJ, Setlow, B (2010). Self-administered cocaine causes long-lasting increases in impulsive choice in a delay discounting task. Behavioral Neuroscience 124, 470477.Google Scholar
Mischel, W, Ayduk, O, Berman, MG, Casey, BJ, Gotlib, IH, Jonides, J, Kross, E, Teslovich, T, Wilson, NL, Zayas, V, Shoda, Y (2011). ‘Willpower’ over the life span: decomposing self-regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 6, 252256.Google Scholar
Moffitt, TE, Arseneault, L, Belsky, D, Dickson, N, Hancox, RJ, Harrington, H, Houts, R, Poulton, R, Roberts, BW, Ross, S, Sears, MR, Thomson, WM, Caspi, A (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108, 26932698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueser, KT, Deavers, F, Penn, DL, Cassisi, J (2013). Psychosocial treatments for schizophrenia. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 9, 465497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nutt, D, King, LA, Saulsbury, W, Blakemore, C (2007). Development of a rational scale to assess the harm of drugs of potential misuse. Lancet 369, 10471053.Google Scholar
O'Brien, CP (2005). Anticraving medications for relapse prevention: a possible new class of psychoactive medications. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 14231431.Google Scholar
Odum, AL (2011). Delay discounting: trait variable? Behavioural Processes 87, 19.Google Scholar
Olausson, P, Jentsch, JD, Krueger, DD, Tronson, NC, Nairn, AC, Taylor, JR (2007). Orbitofrontal cortex and cognitive-motivational impairments in psychostimulant addiction: evidence from experiments in the non-human primate. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1121, 610638.Google Scholar
Olesen, J, Gustavsson, A, Svensson, M, Wittchen, HU, Jonsson, B (2012). The economic cost of brain disorders in Europe. European Journal of Neurology 19, 155162.Google Scholar
Pennings, EJ, Leccese, AP, Wolff, FA (2002). Effects of concurrent use of alcohol and cocaine. Addiction 97, 773783.Google Scholar
Perez de Los Cobos, J, Sinol, N, Puerta, C, Cantillano, V, Lopez Zurita, C, Trujols, J (2011). Features and prevalence of patients with probable adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder who request treatment for cocaine use disorders. Psychiatry Research 185, 205210.Google Scholar
Preller, KH, Ingold, N, Hulka, LM, Vonmoos, M, Jenni, D, Baumgartner, MR, Vollenweider, FX, Quednow, BB (2013). Increased sensorimotor gating in recreational and dependent cocaine users is modulated by craving and ADHD symptoms. Biological Psychiatry 73, 225234.Google Scholar
Quednow, BB, Kuhn, KU, Hoenig, K, Maier, W, Wagner, M (2004). Prepulse inhibition and habituation of acoustic startle response in male MDMA (‘ecstasy’) users, cannabis users, and healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology 29, 982990.Google Scholar
Quednow, BB, Kuhn, KU, Hoppe, C, Westheide, J, Maier, W, Daum, I, Wagner, M (2007). Elevated impulsivity and impaired decision-making cognition in heavy users of MDMA (‘ecstasy’). Psychopharmacology 189, 517530.Google Scholar
Rachlin, H, Green, L (1972). Commitment, choice and self-control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 17, 1522.Google Scholar
Rahman, Q, Clarke, CD (2005). Sex differences in neurocognitive functioning among abstinent recreational cocaine users. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 181, 374380.Google Scholar
Reske, M, Delis, DC, Paulus, MP (2011). Evidence for subtle verbal fluency deficits in occasional stimulant users: quick to play loose with verbal rules. Journal of Psychiatry Research 45, 361368.Google Scholar
Reske, M, Eidt, CA, Delis, DC, Paulus, MP (2010). Nondependent stimulant users of cocaine and prescription amphetamines show verbal learning and memory deficits. Biological Psychiatry 68, 762769.Google Scholar
Rilling, JK, Sanfey, AG (2011). The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annual Review of Psychology 62, 2348.Google Scholar
Rosler, M, Retz, W, Retz-Junginger, P, Thome, J, Supprian, T, Nissen, T, Stieglitz, RD, Blocher, D, Hengesch, G, Trott, GE (2004). Tools for the diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. Self-rating behaviour questionnaire and diagnostic checklist. Nervenarzt 75, 888895.Google Scholar
Rounsaville, BJ (2004). Treatment of cocaine dependence and depression. Biological Psychiatry 56, 803809.Google Scholar
Soar, K, Mason, C, Potton, A, Dawkins, L (2012). Neuropsychological effects associated with recreational cocaine use. Psychopharmacology (Berlin) 222, 633643.Google Scholar
Stewart, JL, Flagan, TM, May, AC, Reske, M, Simmons, AN, Paulus, MP (2013). Young adults at risk for stimulant dependence show reward dysfunction during reinforcement-based decision making. Biological Psychiatry 73, 235241.Google Scholar
Sussner, BD, Smelson, DA, Rodrigues, S, Kline, A, Losonczy, M, Ziedonis, D (2006). The validity and reliability of a brief measure of cocaine craving. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 83, 233237.Google Scholar
Takahashi, T, Oono, H, Inoue, T, Boku, S, Kako, Y, Kitaichi, Y, Kusumi, I, Masui, T, Nakagawa, S, Suzuki, K, Tanaka, T, Koyama, T, Radford, MH (2008). Depressive patients are more impulsive and inconsistent in intertemporal choice behavior for monetary gain and loss than healthy subjects – an analysis based on Tsallis' statistics. Neuro Endocrinology Letters 29, 351358.Google Scholar
Tiffany, ST, Singleton, E, Haertzen, CA, Henningfield, JE (1993). The development of a cocaine craving questionnaire. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 34, 1928.Google Scholar
United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime (2011). World Drug Report 2011. United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime: Vienna, Austria.Google Scholar
Vadhan, NP, Hart, CL, Haney, M, van Gorp, WG, Foltin, RW (2009). Decision-making in long-term cocaine users: effects of a cash monetary contingency on Gambling task performance. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 102, 95101.Google Scholar
Verdejo-Garcia, A, Benbrook, A, Funderburk, F, David, P, Cadet, JL, Bolla, KI (2007 a). The differential relationship between cocaine use and marijuana use on decision-making performance over repeat testing with the Iowa Gambling Task. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 90, 211.Google Scholar
Verdejo-Garcia, AJ, Perales, JC, Perez-Garcia, M (2007 b). Cognitive impulsivity in cocaine and heroin polysubstance abusers. Addictive Behaviors 32, 950966.Google Scholar
Vonmoos, M, Hulka, LM, Preller, KH, Jenni, D, Baumgartner, MR, Stohler, R, Bolla, KI, Quednow, BB (2013). Cognitive dysfunctions in recreational and dependent cocaine users: the role of ADHD, craving, and early age of onset. British Journal of Psychiatry. 203, 3543.Google Scholar
Wagner, FA, Anthony, JC (2007). Male–female differences in the risk of progression from first use to dependence upon cannabis, cocaine, and alcohol. Drug and Alcohol Dependence 86, 191198.Google Scholar
Washio, Y, Higgins, ST, Heil, SH, McKerchar, TL, Badger, GJ, Skelly, JM, Dantona, RL (2011). Delay discounting is associated with treatment response among cocaine-dependent outpatients. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 19, 243248.Google Scholar
Wittchen, HU, Wunderlich, U, Gruschwitz, S, Zaudig, M (1997 a). Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Achse I: Psychische Störungen (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV. Axis I: Mental Disorders). Hogrefe: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Wittchen, HU, Wunderlich, U, Gruschwitz, S, Zaudig, M (1997 b). Strukturiertes Klinisches Interview für DSM-IV. Achse II: Persönlichkeitsstörungen (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV. Axis II: Personality Disorders). Hogrefe: Göttingen.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Hulka Supplementary Material

Appendix

Download Hulka Supplementary Material(File)
File 192.2 KB