Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:40:59.605Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychological, Social, and Epistemic Factors in the Theory of Science

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 February 2022

Alvin I. Goldman*
Affiliation:
University of Arizona

Extract

Traditional philosophy of science concentrated on logic and methodology, dismissing psychology and sociology as irrelevant (except for the context of discovery). Recent movements to ‘naturalize’ philosophy of science have reinstated psychological and social factors repudiated by the tradition, but these two types of factors have mostly been addressed independently and by different authors. Most cognitive approaches (e.g., Thagard 1989a, 1992; Churchland 1989) pursue the microcognition of science in abstraction from the social, and most sociologists of science ignore, post pone, or reject cognitive explanations of science (Latour and Woolgar 1986, 280). More recently, however, several philosophers of science have urged a blending of the cognitive and the social (Thagard 1993, in press, Solomon 1992, 1994a, 1994b, Kitcher 1993, Giere 1988), and this paper has a similar thrust. I shall not endorse a reduction of the social to the psychological, but a substantial overlap or intermingling of the two.

Type
Part IX. Integrating Cognitive and Social Models of Science
Copyright
Copyright © 1995 by the Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Churchland, P.M. (1989), A Neurocomputational Perspective. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cole, S. (1992), Making Science: Between Nature and Society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Galison, P. (1987), How Experiments End. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Giere, R.N. (1988), Explaining Science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A.I. (1991), “Epistemic Paternalism: Communication Control in Law and Society,Journal of Philosophy 88: 113131. Reprinted in Goldman (1992).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A.I. (1992), Liaisons: Philosophy Meets the Cognitive and Social Sciences. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A.I. (forthcoming), “Social Epistemology, Interests, and Truth,” Philosophical Topics.Google Scholar
Goldman, A.I. and Shaked, M. (1992), “An Economic Model of Scientific Activity and Truth Acquisition,” in Goldman (1992).Google Scholar
Hull, D. (1988), Science as a Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1990), “The Division of Cognitive Labor,Journal of Philosophy 87: 522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1993), The Advancement of Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kruglanski, A.W. (1980), “Lay Epistemology: Process and Contents,Psychological Review 87: 7087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1987), “Motivation and Inference: Self-Serving Generation and Evaluation of Evidence,Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53: 636647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1990), “The Case for Motivated Reasoning,Psychological Bulletin 108: 480498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Latour, B. (1987), Science in Action. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Latour, B. and Woolgar, S. (1986), Laboratory Life. 2d ed. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Lord, C.G., Ross, L., and Lepper, M.R. (1979), “Biased Assimilation and Attitude Polarization: The Effects of Prior Theories on Subsequently Considered Evidence,Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 37: 20982109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Keefe, D.J. (1990), Persuasion: Theory and Research. Newbury Park,: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Petty, R.E. and Cacioppo, J.T. (1986a), Communication and Persuasion: Central and Peripheral Routes to Attitude Change. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petty, R.E. and Cacioppo, J.T. (1986b), “The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion,” in Berkowitz, L. (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 19. New York: Academic Press, pp. 123205.Google Scholar
Sanitioso, R., Kunda, Z., and Fong, G.T. (1990), “Motivated Recruitment of Autobiographical Memory,Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59: 229241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapin, S. and Schaffer, S. (1985), Leviathan and the Air Pump. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Solomon, M. (1992), “Scientific Rationality and Human Reasoning,Philosophy of Science 59: 439455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, M. (1994a), “Social Empiricism,Nous 28: 325343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, M. (1994b) “A More Social Epistemology,” in Schmitt, F., ed., Socializing Epistemology. Lanham, MD.: Rowman and Littlefield.Google Scholar
Thagard, P.R. (1989a), “Explanatory Coherence,Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12: 435502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P.R. (1989b), “Scientific Cognition: Hot or Cold?”, in Fuller, S. et al, eds., The Cognitive Turn. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Thagard, P.R. (1992), Conceptual Revolutions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P.R. (1993), “Societies of Minds: Science as Distributed Computing,Studies in History and Philosophy of Science 24: 4967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P.R. (in press), “Mind, Society, and the Growth of Knowledge,” Philosophy of Science.Google Scholar