Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T22:53:43.391Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Worry or craving? A selective review of evidence for food-related attention biases in obese individuals, eating-disorder patients, restrained eaters and healthy samples

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2014

Jessica Werthmann*
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
Anita Jansen
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
Anne Roefs
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, The Netherlands
*
*Corresponding author: J. Werthmann, fax 0031-433884196, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Living in an ‘obesogenic’ environment poses a serious challenge for weight maintenance. However, many people are able to maintain a healthy weight indicating that not everybody is equally susceptible to the temptations of this food environment. The way in which someone perceives and reacts to food cues, that is, cognitive processes, could underlie differences in susceptibility. An attention bias for food could be such a cognitive factor that contributes to overeating. However, an attention bias for food has also been implicated with restrained eating and eating-disorder symptomatology. The primary aim of the present review was to determine whether an attention bias for food is specifically related to obesity while also reviewing evidence for attention biases in eating-disorder patients, restrained eaters and healthy-weight individuals. Another aim was to systematically examine how selective attention for food relates (causally) to eating behaviour. Current empirical evidence on attention bias for food within obese samples, eating-disorder patients, and, even though to a lesser extent, in restrained eaters is contradictory. However, present experimental studies provide relatively consistent evidence that an attention bias for food contributes to subsequent food intake. This review highlights the need to distinguish not only between different (temporal) attention bias components, but also to take different motivations (craving v. worry) and their impact on attentional processing into account. Overall, the current state of research suggests that biased attention could be one important cognitive mechanism by which the food environment tempts us into overeating.

Type
Conference on ‘Changing dietary behaviour: physiology through to practice’
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2014 

Surrounded by an ‘obesogenic’ food environment?

Obesity and overweight constitute a serious risk for psychological and physical wellbeing( Reference Cali and Caprio 1 8 ). The WHO estimates that 2·8 million people die each year due to the adverse consequences of overweight and obesity( 7 , 8 ). Ultimately, obesity is caused by a long-lasting imbalance of energy intake and energy expenditure. This imbalance is mainly caused by excessive food intake( Reference Hill, Catenacci and Wyatt 9 , Reference Van Zant 10 ). Our ‘obesogenic’ food environment is characterised by an abundance of palatable, high-energy, cheap and convenient food that is constantly available and promoted aggressively( Reference Hill and Peters 11 , Reference Wadden, Brownell and Foster 12 ). Living in such an environment poses a serious challenge for maintaining a healthy weight. However, the majority of individuals have a healthy weight suggesting that not everybody is equally susceptible to the temptations of this obesogenic food environment. What explains these apparent individual differences in susceptibility? One hypothesis is that certain people find high-energy food more attractive and that this increased ‘hedonic hunger’( Reference Lowe and Butryn 13 ) leads to craving and (over)eating thereby contributing to weight gain and obesity. Cognitive processes (i.e. the way in which someone processes and perceives food temptations), could reflect and contribute to individual differences in the susceptibility to food temptations. An attention bias for food could be one important cognitive process in this respect.

A food-related attention bias refers to selective attentional processing of food cues, including both voluntary and involuntary processes. Food is essential for survival and especially high-energy food cues are potent in quickly capturing attention( Reference Harrar, Toepel and Murray 14 ). However, in an obesogenic environment, such a bias for food stimuli could be problematic, especially when trying to lose weight. A growing body of research is investigating if an attention bias for food is indeed related to eating behaviour and obesity. However, an attention bias for food has also been implicated in restrained eating (i.e. dieting intentions) and eating disorders (i.e. worry about food intake). Thus, a major question concerns the meaning of an attention bias for food, as it is unclear if an attention bias for food reflects craving and hedonic motivation for food or worry about food intake.

Attention is biased by craving and by worry: two sides of the same coin?

According to addiction theories, an attention bias for desired (substance-related) cues is a major force in drug seeking and drug taking behaviour. According to the incentive sensitisation model by Berridge et al. ( Reference Berridge 15 Reference Robinson and Berridge 17 ), addictive cues signalling the imminent drug consumption gain incentive properties during a conditioning process of repeated signalling and subsequent drug consumption. As a result of this process, these stimuli become salient in the environment and are then potent to ‘grab attention’ of drug users. This model has been extended as an explanation for overeating in the context of obesity( Reference Nijs and Franken 18 ): through a conditioning process based on the rewarding effects of food intake, cues (such as the sight of food) can gain incentive salience and thus become potent to attract attention. It has further been posited that attention bias for food cues and craving stand in a reciprocal relationship: biased attention for food cues is thought to evoke food cravings, whereas the opposite is also possible: craving for food steers attention bias for food cues( Reference Nijs, Muris and Euser 19 , Reference Smeets, Roefs and Jansen 20 ). Thus, according to this addiction account of overeating, obesity should be associated with increased attentional biases towards food reflecting motivational approach for eating.

However, theoretically, it is also possible that eating-related worries or dieting intentions are associated with an attention bias for food cues. According to a cognitive-behavioural account of eating disorders, an attentional bias for food reflects fear of gaining weight or losing control over eating( Reference Fairburn, Clark and Fairburn 21 , Reference Lee and Shafran 22 ). Schemas related to body size and shape concerns can be activated by external or internal cues. These schemas then bias the individual's attention for food. Hence, in the context of weight concerns, an attention bias for food could contribute to dietary restraint and (behavioural) avoidance of food stimuli( Reference Fairburn, Clark and Fairburn 21 , Reference Williamson, White and York-Crowe 23 ). Considering that an obese person undertakes many dieting attempts( Reference Andreyeva, Long and Henderson 24 Reference Williamson, Serdula and Anda 26 ), and that obesity can be associated with body concerns( Reference Legenbauer, Vocks and Betz 27 ), it is possible that in the context of obesity an attention bias for food might reflect worry about food intake or concerns about weight and body shape. This implies that the interpretation of an attentional bias for food in obese individuals may not be so straightforward as sometimes is suggested. Thus, it is not immediately clear whether an observed attention bias reflects a craving for food or concerns regarding food intake or even both.

To answer this question, the aim of the present review was to critically summarise existing empirical studies to find out whether an attention bias for food reflects worry over food intake or appetitive motivation. For this aim, research findings on attention bias for food in overweight and obese and in healthy-weight participants, attention bias for food in eating-disorder samples and attention bias for food in restrained eaters have been reviewed.

Another important question is whether biased attention for food is more than the expression of motivational states (worry about intake or craving) and can causally influence eating behaviour. Thus, to determine whether biased attention is more than an epiphenomenon reflecting individual differences in craving and/or eating concerns, empirical evidence testing the causal impact of biased attention for food on subsequent eating behaviour was also reviewed.

Assessment of visual attention for food: many roads lead to Rome?

Conceptualisations of attention bias and attentional components vary a lot in current research on food-related attention. In general, two attentional mechanisms can be distinguished: (1) attention prioritises relevant information and this selection mechanism can be involuntary (i.e. a bottom-up process, for example, when we see an ambulance); or (2) attention can be steered voluntarily (i.e. a top-down process, for example, when we search for a certain brand of pasta on the shelves in a supermarket). Early attention components are associated with more involuntary, less controlled mechanisms, whereas later attention components are thought to reflect the slower top-down mechanisms of voluntary or more controlled processing( Reference Knudsen 28 , Reference LaBerge 29 ).

At least three different methodologies have been applied in previous studies to asses visual attentional biases for food: (1) measuring response latencies or the calculation of an interference effect during a food- Stroop task; (2) assessment of response latencies during a spatial attention paradigm, such as the visual probe task, the exogenous cueing task or the visual search task; (3) recordings of eye-movements during an attention paradigm.

Most research on food-related attention, especially in eating-disorder patients and restrained eaters, applied the food-Stroop task( Reference Overduin, Jansen and Louwerse 30 , Reference Williams, Mathews and MacLeod 31 ). During this task, coloured food and non-food words are presented, and participants are required to indicate the colour of the word as quickly as possible, irrespective of the meaning of the word. The Stroop interference score is calculated by obtaining a difference score between the average response latency on food v. non-food trials. This interference score should reflect biased attention. That is, an attentional bias for food stimuli is assumed if the response latency is relatively prolonged on food trials. A disadvantage of this paradigm is that it cannot inform on the underlying attentional processes. The slow-down in colour-naming could be caused both, by increased attention for the semantic meaning, or by avoidance of processing the stimulus word( Reference Field and Cox 32 ). Moreover, it is unclear which attentional components are reflected in the interference effect: while it has been argued that the Stroop effect could reflect an early attentional process (i.e. involuntary semantic processing)( Reference Cox, Fadardi, Klinger, Wiers and Stacy 33 ), results of a meta-analysis suggested that it is more likely to reflect later attentional processes( Reference Phaf and Kan 34 ). Taken together, a disadvantage of the food-Stroop task is that it is not clear which attentional component (early or later attentional processes) is captured, and that the interference effect cannot provide information on the direction (approach, thus increased attention towards food, v. avoidance, thus reduced attention for food) of attention.

The visual probe task( Reference MacLeod, Mathews and Tata 35 ) relies on the assessment of response latencies. During this task, two stimuli (a critical stimulus and a non-critical stimulus) are presented side by side on the computer screen for a fixed duration (typically 2000 ms). Then, both stimuli disappear, and a small probe appears in the position of one of the stimuli. Participants are instructed to press a corresponding key on the keyboard to indicate the location of the probe (e.g. left or right). The logic of this task presumes that participants react faster to indicate the position of the probe if their attention was already directed to the location (thus on the stimulus) in which the probe appears. An advantage of the visual probe is that it is possible to distinguish early and later attentional processes by including different presentation times: shorter stimulus presentations (100–500 ms) are thought to assess initial orientation, whereas longer stimulus presentations (≥500 ms) are thought to assess maintained attention( Reference Mogg, Bradley and De Bono 36 , Reference Mogg, Bradley and Miles 37 ). Moreover, the calculation of a response latency based attention bias allows for the interpretation of the direction of attention: the mean response latency in congruent trials (when the probe replaces the relevant picture) is subtracted from the mean response latency in incongruent trials (when the probe replaces the neutral stimulus). According to this calculation positive bias scores reflect attentional approach and negative bias scores suggest attentional avoidance( Reference Mogg, Bradley and Miles 37 ).

The exogenous cueing paradigm relies on the same logic as the visual probe paradigm( Reference Koster, Crombez and Verschuere 38 , Reference Koster, Crombez and Verschuere 39 ). The only difference between these two paradigms is that in the exogenous cueing paradigm only one stimulus (without counterpart) is presented as cue per trial whereas in the visual probe task two stimuli are presented side by side. Similar to the visual probe task participants have to indicate the location of a probe following the cue (e.g. picture of a food item) as fast as possible.

During the visual search task( Reference Rinck, Becker and Kellermann 40 ), participants view search matrices depicting several stimuli, with either one presentation of a relevant stimulus among several irrelevant stimuli (measuring speeded detection) or the presentation of an irrelevant stimulus among several relevant stimuli (measuring increased distraction). Participants have to indicate the ‘odd-one-out’ stimulus as fast as possible. An attention bias is evidenced by: (a) speeded detection of the relevant among irrelevant stimuli (i.e. early attention) and/or (b) in increased distraction by relevant stimuli when searching for the irrelevant stimulus (i.e. later attention component). An advantage of this task is that these two attention components can be identified, and that an indication of the direction of attention (at least for increased attentional approach) can be provided. A disadvantage of this task is that the attentional component of increased distraction does not inform whether the assessed reaction time is due to the inability to shift attention away from the distracting stimuli or due to successive attentional attraction by the distracting stimuli( Reference Smeets, Roefs and van Furth 41 ).

In contrast to the indirect assessment of attention by measuring response latencies, visual attention can be measured directly through ‘eye-tracking’ by measuring eye-movements during the stimulus presentation. This technique can (partly) overcome the ‘snap-shot’ view that is obtained by an indirect assessment of attention, thereby providing a more sensitive measure for the temporal components and the direction of attention processing( Reference Field and Christiansen 42 , Reference Field, Munafó and Franken 43 ).

Owing to these methodological considerations, only those studies were reviewed that applied an attention paradigm that can inform on the temporal components of attention and/or can distinguish between attentional avoidance and approach processes based on recorded response latencies as an indirect measure of visual attention bias or on recorded eye-movements as a direct measure of visual attentional bias or on a combination of both. Eleven studies on attention bias for food in obese or overweight samples were reviewed. Five studies on attention bias for food in eating-disorder patients and nine studies on attentional bias for food in restrained eaters were included in this review. As another aim of the present review was to summarise research on the causal influence of attention bias for food on eating behaviour, five studies that manipulated an attention bias for food and measured subsequent food intake were also summarised.

Attention bias for food and obesity

An overview of studies that examined food-related attention biases in relation to BMI and/or obesity is provided in Table 1. In line with an addiction account of food-related attention bias, some studies suggest that obese individuals, as compared with healthy-weight participants, showed an increased attentional approach bias to food cues. For example, obese participants initially oriented their attention more often towards food cues than towards non-food cues and maintained their gaze longer on food cues than did healthy-weight participants, when they were satiated( Reference Castellanos, Charboneau and Dietrich 44 ). Similarly, overweight and obese participants had a trend-significant increased attention bias for food (v. non-food) cues in comparison with healthy-weight participants (( Reference Nijs, Muris and Euser 19 ), recordings of response latencies to food cues at 100 ms). Correspondingly, obese participants, who did not restrain their food intake, preferentially attended food words in comparison with non-food words (( Reference Nathan, O'Neill and Mogg 45 ), trend in one sample t tests within low restrained participants in the placebo condition; note that it has not been reported whether high restrained participants differed significantly from low restrained participants in their attention bias for food). Moreover, another study showed that both obese and obese binge-eating disorder patients were significantly slower to disengage from food cues than from non-food cues, whereas only obese binge-eating disorder patients showed increased engagement with food v. non-food cues( Reference Schmitz, Naumann and Trentowska 46 ).

Table 1. Overview of evidence on attentional processing of food cues in obese, overweight and healthy weight samples (2009–2014)

VP, visual probe task; ST, Stroop task; VS, Visual Search; FV, free viewing; CT, clarification task; EC, exogenous cueing task; EM, tracking of eye-movements; RT, response latencies; ERP, event-related potentials; HC, high-energy food cues; LC, low-energy food cues; OB, obese participants; OW, overweight participants; HW, healthy weight participants; RS, restrained eating; BED, binge-eating disorder patients; M, mean; n.sp., not specified; sr , self-report. a Note that a main effect for attention bias for food cues over non-food cues was reported. * P < 0·05.

Including 69 female and 59 male participants.

Two VP version were administered, one with 500 ms stimulus duration and one with 1250 ms stimulus duration time.

§ Note that (parts of) the sample, methodology and data in Calitri et al.( Reference Calitri, Pothos and Tapper 53 ) is based on data collection of Pothos et al.( Reference Pothos, Tapper and Calitri 52 ).

|| Including 58 female and 44 male participants.

Half of the trials were presented with 100 ms presentation duration, and half of the trials were presented with 500 ms presentation duration.

†† Including three underweight participants.

‡‡ Including four male participants in both studies, respectively.

§§ Including fifteen male and eleven female participants.

|| || Half of the trials were presented either with 500 or 2000 ms duration.

¶¶ Including seven male and thirteen female participants per group.

In contrast, other research suggests that a higher BMI is associated with attentional avoidance of food. For example, Nummenmaa et al. ( Reference Nummenmaa, Hietanen and Calvo 47 ) reported a negative association between initial orientation towards food cues and BMI, in a group of mainly healthy-weight participants. Another study showed that healthy-weight participants paid significantly less attention to low-energy than high-energy food compared with obese participants with dieting intentions( Reference Graham, Hoover and Ceballos 48 ). This finding suggests that obese individuals who want to lose weight pay more attention to low-energy foods than healthy-weight individuals. Thus, weight groups differed only in their attention bias for low-energy food, but not for high-energy food cues. Similarly, within a sample of obese participants, higher BMI was found to be associated with less initial attention bias for fried foods( Reference Gearhardt, Treat and Hollingworth 49 ). Moreover, high restrained obese participants did not show an attention bias towards food (( Reference Nathan, O'Neill and Mogg 45 ), one-sample t tests within highly restrained participants). Together, these results seem to suggest that a higher BMI is associated with attentional avoidance of palatable high-energy food, or with an increased attention focus on low-energy food.

One study seems to combine these contradictory findings, by reporting an approach-avoidance pattern of results( Reference Werthmann, Roefs and Nederkoorn 50 ). This study found that obese and overweight participants showed increased attentional approach towards high-energy food pictures on an early measure of attention bias (i.e. direction bias) as compared to healthy-weight participants, under conditions of satiety. However, in a slightly later attention process (i.e. durations of initial fixations), obese participants showed attentional avoidance of high-fat food pictures. This finding might suggest an approach-avoidance process of attention processing that could reflect the inner conflict of obese or overweight participants, who might feel automatically attracted towards food, yet might also try to avoid looking at food in an attempt to down-regulate their craving for this food.

However, there are also several studies that did not find a significant relation of BMI and attentional processing of food cues (e.g. ( Reference Loeber, Grosshans and Korucuoglu 51 , Reference Pothos, Tapper and Calitri 52 )). Moreover, one study explicitly tested that healthy and obese individuals alike paid significantly more attention to food pictures than to non-food pictures, independent of their hunger or satiety state (( Reference Nijs, Muris and Euser 19 ), see data on eye-tracking).

Evidence on the predictive validity of attention bias for food on changes in BMI is still rare. Only one published study has so far tested if attention bias towards unhealthy or healthy food cues was associated with BMI change over 12 months, in a mostly healthy-weight student sample( Reference Calitri, Pothos and Tapper 53 ). Results showed that food- Stroop interference for unhealthy food words was related to BMI increase after 1 year. However, an attention bias based on response latencies during the visual probe task was not related to weight change over time.

In summary, current evidence on attention bias for food cues in obese participants is very mixed, as there is empirical evidence for approach, avoidance and approach-avoidance attention processes in obese v. healthy-weight participants when viewing food cues. Moreover, it is questionable if obese participants do at all hold stronger attentional biases for food in comparison to healthy-weight participants, considering that some studies indicated no differences in attention allocation for food cues between healthy-weight and obese participants.

Attention bias for food and eating disorders

Giel et al. ( Reference Giel, Friederich and Teufel 54 ) showed that anorexic patients as well as (8 h-fasted and 1 h-fasted) healthy-weight control participants all initially directed their attention more often towards food than non-food cues. However, healthy-weight participants maintained their gaze significantly longer on food than on non-food pictures in comparison to anorexic patients who did not maintain their attention longer on food pictures than on non-food pictures. This observation indicates attentional avoidance of food cues during a later attention process in anorexic patients, compared to healthy participants. Similarly, in two experiments by Shafran et al. ( Reference Shafran, Lee and Cooper 55 , Reference Shafran, Lee and Cooper 56 ), women with various types of eating disorder exhibited attentional avoidance of low-energy eating scenarios and attentional approach towards high-energy eating scenarios in a later attention process, in comparison to non-eating disordered control participants. In contrast, Smeets et al. ( Reference Smeets, Roefs and van Furth 41 ) observed that eating-disorder patients (including a similar proportion of anorexic and bulimic patients) showed increased distraction by high-energy food words compared to healthy participants. This finding shows attentional approach towards high-energy food cues in eating-disorder patients. Contradictory to these studies, which observed significant differences in attention allocation for food between eating-disorder patients and healthy control groups, Veenstra and de Jong( Reference Veenstra and de Jong 57 ) reported that both healthy controls and eating-disorder patients alike, showed attentional avoidance of high-energy food (500 ms data).

To summarise, findings on an attention bias for food in eating-disorder patients are also contradictory. Giel et al.( Reference Giel, Friederich and Teufel 54 ) and also Shafran et al.( Reference Shafran, Lee and Cooper 55 , Reference Shafran, Lee and Cooper 56 ) observed attentional avoidance of food cues during a later attention process in eating-disorder patients when compared to healthy controls. However, there is also evidence that eating-disorder patients show increased distraction by food cues( Reference Smeets, Roefs and van Furth 41 ) in comparison to healthy controls, that eating-disorder patients show increased attention towards (high-energy) food( Reference Shafran, Lee and Cooper 55 , Reference Shafran, Lee and Cooper 56 ) and that eating-disorder patients initially fixate increasingly more often on food than on non-food cues (( Reference Giel, Friederich and Teufel 54 ), result of one sample t test within anorexia nervosa patients). However, there is also evidence that eating-disorder patients do not differ from healthy controls in avoiding to look at high-fat food cues( Reference Veenstra and de Jong 57 ); see Table 2 for an overview of studies.

Table 2. Summary of food-related attention bias studies in ED and control participants (2007–2011)

ED, eating disorder patients; AN, anorexia nervosa patients; BN, bulimia nervosa patients; EDNOS, eating disorder not otherwise specified patients; BED, binge-eating disorder patients; VP, visual probe task; VS, visual search task; FV, free viewing; EC, exogenous cueing task; EM, eye movements; RT, response latencies; HC, high-energy food cues; LC, low-energy food cues.

Note that in this case>means that stronger attention bias were observed in ED (i.e. ED showed significantly more attentional avoidance of ‘positive eating’ and significantly more attentional approach for ‘negative eating’ stimuli).

Note that AN directed significantly more attention to food than non-food cues.

§ Note that healthy controls remained with their attention significantly longer on food cues than non-food cues.

|| Third of the participants received a stimulus duration of 300 ms, third of the participants received a stimulus duration of 500 ms, third received a stimulus duration of 1000 ms.

Both groups showed attention avoidance (i.e. significant negative cue-validity effect and less attentional engagement) of high fat food (but not low-fat food or non-food cues).

†† The VP included also shape/weight trials to assess an attention bias for weight and shape information, yet results on this bias were not of interest in this review on attention bias for food and are therefore not reported here.

‡‡ During this study another VS with body-related words was conducted to assess an attention bias for body-related information, yet results on this bias were not of interest in this review on attention bias for food and are therefore not reported here.

Attention bias for food and restrained eating

An overview of studies on restrained eating and attention bias for food is provided in Table 3. Two studies reported some evidence for increased attention approach of food cues in restrained eaters when compared to unrestrained eaters. For example, one study reported that restrained eaters, compared to unrestrained eaters, detected high-energy food targets faster than non-food targets (e.g. vehicles) in a matrix of non-food distractors of another category (e.g. musical instruments) yet restrained eaters were also faster to disengage their attention from food cues to find another non-food cue (e.g. a vehicle)( Reference Hollitt, Kemps and Tiggemann 58 ). However, when closely inspecting the depicted graph in this article it seems that the reported differences in attention bias scores were due to groups differences during non-food/non-food trials, as restrained and unrestrained eaters seem to have very similar scores during food relevant trials. Another study reported that restrained eaters reacted faster to high-energy food pictures than to non-food pictures in a flanker task( Reference Meule, Vögele and Kübler 59 ), thereby suggesting attentional approach towards food cues in restrained eaters. Forestell et al. ( Reference Forestell, Lau and Gyurovski 60 ), also using a flanker task, reported that when hungry, unrestrained eaters were distracted by high-energy flankers regardless of whether they reacted to high- or low-energy targets, whereas restrained eaters were only distracted by high-energy flankers when they responded to low-energy targets. This finding suggests that restrained eaters feel conflicted when seeing high-energy food while aiming for low-energy food, when hungry. However, restrained and unrestrained eaters did not differ in their responses to high and low-energy food pictures when they were satiated. Together, these studies suggest that restrained eaters pay increased attention to (high-energy) food in comparison to unrestrained eaters (and low-energy food). In addition, findings from a study that assessed a temporal (i.e. not spatial) component of attentional processing showed that restrained eaters prioritise processing of food cues to a larger extent than unrestrained eaters, even if this could interfere with their (unrelated) task performance( Reference Neimeijer, de Jong and Roefs 61 ).

Table 3. A summary of food-related attention bias studies in restrained and unrestrained eaters (2000–2013)

RS, restrained eaters, URS, unrestrained eaters; VP, visual probe task; VS, visual search task; EC, exogenous cueing task; FT, flanker task; mST, modified Stroop Task; rsVPT, rapid serial visual presentation task; EM, eye movements; RT, response latencies; HC, high-energy food cues; LC, low-energy food cues; n.sp., not specified; sr, self-report.

Note that picture pairs were individualised for participants.

Half of the participants received a stimulus duration of 500 ms, and the other half of 1500 ms.

§ Under satiety conditions.

|| Under hunger conditions, * P < 0·05.

a A stronger attention bias, thus in this case faster reactions.

b Note that a significant stronger attention bias for food v. non-food cues was reported (i.e. attentional approach for food cues).

c Note that a significant stronger attention bias for non-food v. food cues was reported (i.e. attentional avoidance of food cues).

In contrast to these findings, there is also evidence that restrained eating is associated with attentional avoidance of high-energy food pictures in a later stage of attention processing (( Reference Veenstra, de Jong and Koster 62 ), data on 500 ms disengagement scores). Results from this study( Reference Veenstra, de Jong and Koster 62 ) further showed that in general all participants, unrestrained as well as restrained eaters, displayed attentional avoidance of (high-fat) food cues. Similarly, Hollit et al.( Reference Hollitt, Kemps and Tiggemann 58 ) observed that restrained eaters were faster than unrestrained eaters to disengage their attention from food cues when searching for a non-food cue which could also be interpreted as attentional avoidance in a later stage of attentional processing.

The studies discussed earlier found significant differences between restrained and unrestrained eaters in an attention bias for food, despite opposing patterns of results. However, several other studies did not yield significant differences in attention bias, based on restraint status: a study that applied a modified version of the Stroop task, which can disentangle early and later attentional components, reported that all participants, irrespective of their restraint status, were significantly slower to disengage their attention from food words than from control words( Reference Wilson and Wallis 63 ). Moreover, two studies using response latency based visual probe tasks yielded no significant differences in attentional bias between restrained and unrestrained eaters( Reference Ahern, Field and Yokum 64 , Reference Boon, Vogelzang and Jansen 65 ). Thus, these findings indicate that restraint might not be associated with biased attention allocation for food cues.

One possible explanation for the inconsistent pattern of results could be that restrained eaters are torn in an approach-avoidance conflict between wanting to eat and at the same time wanting to pursue their dieting goals, which could affect their attention processing of tempting food cues( Reference Ahern, Field and Yokum 64 , Reference Papies, Stroebe and Aarts 66 ). This approach-avoidance conflict could lead to the net-effect of a null finding when testing this hypothesis by relying on an indirect attention assessment that cannot provide a dynamic course of attention allocation. Another possibility for the divergent results of previous studies is that attention bias studies in restrained eaters are confounded by BMI differences between restrained and unrestrained eaters: restrained eaters are often heavier than their unrestrained counterparts (( Reference Klesges, Isbell and Klesges 67 ), and see also Table 3). Accordingly, one study that used eye-tracking and matched weight of restrained and unrestrained eaters, found that all participants had biased attention towards food cues (over non-food cues), with no difference between restrained and unrestrained eaters( Reference Werthmann, Roefs and Nederkoorn 68 ). This finding suggests that not restraint per se, but rather weight problems (i.e. overweight and obesity) could be an underlying factor that contributed to previously observed differences in attentional bias for food between restrained and unrestrained eaters.

To summarise, two of the nine studies suggested attention approach of food cues in restrained eaters. Note however, that one of these studies, Hollit et al.( Reference Hollitt, Kemps and Tiggemann 58 ), also found faster disengagement from food cues in restrained eaters, which could be interpreted as attentional avoidance. One study suggested that restrained eaters were distracted by high-energy food when focusing on low-energy food while being hungry( Reference Forestell, Lau and Gyurovski 60 ). Another study suggested that restrained eaters prioritise processing of food cues more than unrestrained eaters( Reference Neimeijer, de Jong and Roefs 61 ). However, the majority of the published studies (n 5) observed no significant differences between restrained and unrestrained eaters in attention processing of food cues( Reference Veenstra, de Jong and Koster 62 Reference Boon, Vogelzang and Jansen 65 , Reference Werthmann, Roefs and Nederkoorn 68 ). Three of these five studies suggested that attention is biased towards food v. non-food cues in all tested participants, irrespective of their restraint status ( Reference Wilson and Wallis 63 , Reference Ahern, Field and Yokum 64 , Reference Werthmann, Roefs and Nederkoorn 68 ) (Boon et al.( Reference Boon, Vogelzang and Jansen 65 ) did not test explicitly whether attention bias for food cues v. non-food cues was increased (or decreased) in all participants.) and one study suggested that all participants avoided looking at food( Reference Veenstra, de Jong and Koster 62 ). Together, these results might suggest that food is a highly relevant stimulus in the environment in general, but not only for restrained eaters. Moreover, the results highlight the need to account for restraint and weight status when measuring attention bias for food.

Attention bias for food and food intake: a causal relationship?

Recently, five studies have been published using an attention bias modification to test if an attention bias for food leads to increased intake of that food; see Table 4 for an overview of studies. Usually, attention bias modification entails that the contingencies of a visual probe task are manipulated in a fashion that the probe either replaces the food stimulus in all (or most) trials (thereby modifying attention towards food cues) or that the probe replaces the contrast category in all (or most) trials (thereby modifying attention away from food cues).

Table 4. A summary of attention bias modification studies for food-related attentional bias and food intake (2013–2014)

mVP, modified visual probe task; mAST, modified anti-saccade task; RT, response latencies; AB, attention bias for the respective food cues (e.g. chocolate, cake, healthy food); EAH, eating in the absence of hunger.

Note that a marginal significant interaction of time and condition on attention bias scores was observed.

Note that the amount of healthy snack food consumed was compared between conditions as a proportion of total (including healthy and unhealthy) snack food.

§ In Expt 1 and in Expt 2.

|| In Expt 2 only.

Note that a significant interaction of accuracy and condition on intake was observed.

Hardman et al. ( Reference Hardman, Rogers and Etchells 69 ) used a visual probe task with high-fat cake and non-food pictures to modify attention bias. They included a no-bias-induction control condition, (i.e. the probe replaced food cues in 50 % of trials and non-food cues in 50 % of trials), an ‘Attend cake’ condition (i.e. the probe always replaced the high-cake picture) and an ‘Avoid cake’ condition (i.e. the probe always replaced the non-food picture). Results indicated a marginal significant change from pre- to post-training in attention bias for cake: participants in the ‘Attend cake’ condition had a stronger attention bias for cake after the attention modification, in comparison to the other conditions. However, no significant differences in intake (of cake and other food items) were observed between conditions. The present study thus indicates that attention bias for a certain food might not change easily due to a modification task, and that marginal changes in attention bias do not influence subsequent intake.

Kemps et al. ( Reference Kemps, Tiggemann and Orr 70 ) conducted two experiments, also applying a manipulated variant of the visual probe task, with chocolate-food and non-chocolate food stimuli. Neither study included a no-bias-induction control group. Participants were either trained to attend to chocolate stimuli or to avoid looking at chocolate stimuli (to look towards non-chocolate food). Their findings indicated that in both experiments attention bias for chocolate v. non-chocolate food changed with the training: participants in the ‘Avoid chocolate’ condition had significantly less attention bias for chocolate than participants in the ‘Attend chocolate’ condition after the training. Moreover, these changes translated to subsequent food intake: participants in the ‘Avoid chocolate’ condition ate significantly less chocolate muffins during a taste test and a similar amount of blueberry muffins when compared to participants in the ‘Attend to chocolate’ condition (Expt 1). In the second experiment, participants in the ‘Avoid chocolate’ condition again ate significantly less chocolate muffins but significantly more blueberry muffins than participants who had to attend to chocolate stimuli. Thus, these experiments provide evidence that experimentally changing attention bias towards or away from a certain food influences subsequent food intake. However, because no control group was employed, it is impossible to determine whether modifying attention away from chocolate decreased chocolate intake or whether modifying attention towards chocolate increased chocolate intake in the respective conditions.

In a similar experiment, Kakoschke et al. ( Reference Kakoschke, Kemps and Tiggemann 71 ) trained participants to attend healthy food (and look away from unhealthy food) or to attend unhealthy food (and look away from healthy food). Again, a no-bias-induction control group was missing. The results concurred with those of Kemps et al.( Reference Kemps, Tiggemann and Orr 70 ), in that attention bias towards healthy food was modifiable. Prior to the attention modification, all the participants had an attention bias towards unhealthy food. This attention bias for unhealthy food did not further increase in the unhealthy training condition (maybe due to a ceiling effect). However, participants in the healthy condition significantly increased their attention bias towards healthy food. Results also yielded that participants in the ‘healthy’ condition ate a significant greater proportion of healthy food than unhealthy food during a taste test offering both kinds of foods compared to participants in the ‘unhealthy’ condition.

Whereas the previous studies tested attention bias modification within non-clinical (student) samples, Boutelle et al. ( Reference Boutelle, Kuckertz and Carlson 72 ) studied attention bias modification for food within a clinical sample of overweight and obese children. A modified visual probe task with food words and non-food words was used to modify attention away from food in the training condition, whereas in the control condition contingencies in this task remained unaltered. Prior to and after the attention modification, children's intake of snack food in the absence of hunger was assessed. Results yielded that attention bias for food words remained the same in the training group, yet the control group showed marginally increased attentional bias towards food words after the control task. This finding translated to food intake: whereas children in the training condition ate a similar amount of food before and after the training task, the control group significantly increased their intake after the (control) task. The authors suggested that training to look away from food words helped children to maintain a similar level of attention bias for food and similar amount of intake, whereas looking in 50 % of trials towards food might have increased attentional bias and subsequent intake in the control group.

The previously discussed studies relied on a pre- and post-assessment of attentional bias to account for changes in attentional processing of food cues, yet one study integrated a measure for attentional allocation during the attention modification paradigm( Reference Werthmann, Field and Roefs 73 ). An anti-saccade task was applied to modify attention bias for chocolate. Participants either had to saccade quickly towards chocolate and away from shoe cues (‘Attend chocolate’ condition), or had to look quickly towards shoes and away from chocolate (‘Attend shoes’ condition). To account for the accuracy with which participants followed these instructions, participants’ eye-movements were recorded during the task. Results showed that accuracy significantly moderated the outcome: participants with higher accuracy ate more chocolate when they had to attend to chocolate, and ate less chocolate when they had to attend to shoes. However, the results were reversed for participants with lower accuracy. Even though this interaction strongly suggests that attention for food is related to subsequent food intake, the question of causality remains in this study because it was unclear what caused the differences in accuracy (and thus influenced attention processing) during the modification training.

To summarise, results so far suggest that experimentally induced change in attention bias for food relates to changes in intake. Most of the currently published studies showed that attention bias modification towards (or away from) certain food increases (or decreases) intake of this food( Reference Kemps, Tiggemann and Orr 70 , Reference Kakoschke, Kemps and Tiggemann 71 , Reference Werthmann, Field and Roefs 73 ). Unfortunately, in most studies, a no-bias-induction control group was missing and hence it remains unclear whether an increase in attention bias leads to increased food intake, or whether a decrease in attention bias for food leads to a decrease in food intake. To sum up, at the present state of research, results are considerably promising that attention bias modification influences food intake and thus suggest that an attention bias for food is causally related to intake.

Attention bias for food and craving

We also briefly reviewed findings on the relationship of craving or hunger and attention bias for food, based on the included studies. Most of these studies measured craving or hunger but did not examine directly (e.g. by testing correlations) if craving and/or hunger is associated with attention biases for food. Of the thirty reviewed studies, eleven measured craving( Reference Schmitz, Naumann and Trentowska 46 , Reference Graham, Hoover and Ceballos 48 , Reference Werthmann, Roefs and Nederkoorn 50 , Reference Loeber, Grosshans and Korucuoglu 51 , Reference Meule, Vögele and Kübler 59 , Reference Neimeijer, de Jong and Roefs 61 , Reference Veenstra, de Jong and Koster 62 , Reference Werthmann, Roefs and Nederkoorn 68 , Reference Boutelle, Kuckertz and Carlson 72 Reference Kemps and Tiggemann 74 ) and seventeen assessed hunger( Reference Nijs, Muris and Euser 19 , Reference Castellanos, Charboneau and Dietrich 44 , Reference Nummenmaa, Hietanen and Calvo 47 Reference Loeber, Grosshans and Korucuoglu 51 , Reference Giel, Friederich and Teufel 54 , Reference Hollitt, Kemps and Tiggemann 58 , Reference Forestell, Lau and Gyurovski 60 Reference Wilson and Wallis 63 , Reference Boon, Vogelzang and Jansen 65 , Reference Werthmann, Roefs and Nederkoorn 68 , Reference Hardman, Rogers and Etchells 69 , Reference Boutelle, Kuckertz and Carlson 72 ) (with an overlap in seven studies assessing both). Eleven of these studies tested the relation of attention bias and craving or hunger statistically. Most studies (n 6) reported a positive association of an early attention process (e.g. direction bias) with self-reported craving or hunger( Reference Nijs, Muris and Euser 19 , Reference Castellanos, Charboneau and Dietrich 44 , Reference Schmitz, Naumann and Trentowska 46 , Reference Graham, Hoover and Ceballos 48 Reference Werthmann, Roefs and Nederkoorn 50 ). Only two studies reported null findings for correlations of an early attention component and craving( Reference Loeber, Grosshans and Korucuoglu 51 ) and/or hunger( Reference Nummenmaa, Hietanen and Calvo 47 , Reference Loeber, Grosshans and Korucuoglu 51 ). Findings seem less consistent for a later attention component (e.g. dwell-time bias), as one study found a positive correlation of attention maintenance and hunger( Reference Castellanos, Charboneau and Dietrich 44 ), whereas another study yielded that hunger negatively related to attention maintenance on (fried) food( Reference Gearhardt, Treat and Hollingworth 49 ). No other study reported significant findings on the relation of a later attention component and hunger or craving. Hence, these results suggest that specifically the early (more automatic) attention component is related to subjective (self-reported) experiences of hunger or craving. Interestingly, whereas most studies( Reference Hardman, Rogers and Etchells 69 , Reference Kemps, Tiggemann and Orr 70 , Reference Boutelle, Kuckertz and Carlson 72 , Reference Werthmann, Field and Roefs 73 ) on attention bias modification assessed craving or hunger, only Kemps et al. ( Reference Kemps, Tiggemann and Orr 70 ) observed a change in craving for chocolate in line with change in attention bias for chocolate. The other studies did not find an effect of attention bias (modification) on self-reported craving. This suggests that the effect of attention bias modification on food intake might not necessarily transfer to the subjectively experienced (explicit) reports of craving.

Conclusion and implications

The aim of this selective review was to summarise studies testing if an attention bias for food reflects appetitive motivation or worry about food intake, and if attention bias for food is causally related to food intake. The present state of research provides no consistent empirical evidence on reliable individual differences in attentional bias for food, depending on weight status or eating concern. Evidence for an increased attention bias for (high-energy) food in obese and overweight participants in comparison with healthy-weight participants is conflicting. Similarly inconsistent results were obtained for eating-disorder patients in comparison to non-clinical groups. The present research on attention biases and restrained eating is also equivocal, but seems less contradictory with a majority of published studies reporting no differences in an attention bias for food between restrained and unrestrained eaters. Interestingly, there is also empirical evidence showing that healthy-weight, non-eating disordered and unrestrained participants have food-related attention biases, suggesting that everyone might have an attention bias for food.

Methodological differences in the reviewed studies might explain the divergent results: the reviewed studies were inconsistent with regard to the assessment of attention bias (direct assessment via eye-tracking v. indirect assessment of response latencies), the temporal components of attention bias (early v. later attention processes) and specific characteristics of heterogeneous (sub)samples (e.g. in eating-disorder research combining groups of anorexic, bulimic and other eating-disorder patients). Moreover, different choices in stimuli sets could have contributed to mixed findings, because it is possible that the contrast category influences the context in which the relevant stimuli are automatically evaluated (see for a similar argumentation when using implicit measures ( Reference Houben, Roefs and Jansen 75 Reference Roefs, Quaedackers and Werrij 77 )). For example, by presenting high-energy food together with low-energy food, participants might be primed with the concept of ‘health’ whereas a combination of high-energy with neutral non-food stimuli renders the activation of this association less likely. These methodological considerations highlight not only the need for refined and valid methods to assess attention bias in an eating context, but also call for replication of previous studies to test how reliable the applied methods are.

Overall, this selective review of existing literature cannot provide a definite answer on the question if attention bias for food reflects worry about intake or craving. However, based on the relatively consistent findings on attention bias modification more evidence speaks for an addiction account: an attention bias for food leads to increased intake. Similarly, even though there is a paucity of studies, positive results for the relation of attention bias for food and craving were obtained. However, it is to note that (experimental) research testing if an attention bias can reflect worry, especially within overweight or restrained samples, is relatively scarce. Overall, research on an attention bias for food seems to corroborate with an addiction account of the role of attention bias for food( Reference Field, Munafó and Franken 43 ) suggesting that an attention bias towards food is the expression of increased hedonic motivation for food and could even causally contribute to overeating.

This knowledge could be useful for future research: an experimental modification of attention biases could help to understand the working mechanisms of attentional processes and can inform on effective treatment options, such as incorporating an attention bias modification training in obesity treatments or as (part of) a relapse prevention programme. On a societal level, another implication could imply targeting the visibility of (high-energy) food temptations in our surroundings to prevent susceptible individuals from being lured into craving and overeating by their attentional bias for food.

Conflicts of Interest

None.

Authorship

J. W. drafted this manuscript. All authors contributed and/or commented on an earlier and the final version of this review.

References

1. Cali, AM & Caprio, S (2008) Obesity in children and adolescents. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 93, 11 Suppl 1, 2008–1363.Google Scholar
2. Harriger, JA & Thompson, JK (2012) Psychological consequences of obesity: weight bias and body image in overweight and obese youth. Int Rev Psychiatry 24, 247253.Google Scholar
3. Hu, FB (2003). Overweight and obesity in women: health risks and consequences. J Women's Health 12, 163172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4. Kalarchian, MA & Marcus, MD (2012) Psychiatric comorbidity of childhood obesity. Int Rev Psychiatry 24, 241246.Google Scholar
5. Mokdad, AH, Marks, JS, Stroup, DF et al. (2004) Actual causes of death in the United States, 2000. J Am Med Assoc 291, 12381245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Pulgaron, ER (2013) Childhood obesity: a review of increased risk for physical and psychological comorbidities. Clin Ther 35, 014.Google Scholar
7. World Health Organization (2013) Obesity and Overweight. Fact Sheet 311. 2013; available at http://amro.who.int/common/Display.asp?Lang (accessed 02.07.2013)Google Scholar
8. World Health Organization (1998) Obesity: Preventing and Managing the Global Epidemic. Geneva: World Health Organization.Google Scholar
9. Hill, JO, Catenacci, V & Wyatt, HR (2005) Obesity: overview of an epidemic. Psychiatr Clin North Am 28, 123.Google Scholar
10. Van Zant, RS (1992) Influence of diet and exercise on energy expenditure–a review. Int J Sport Nutr 2, 119.Google Scholar
11. Hill, JO & Peters, JC (1998) Environmental contributions to the obesity epidemic. Science 280, 13711374.Google Scholar
12. Wadden, TA, Brownell, KD & Foster, GD (2002) Obesity: responding to the global epidemic. J Consult Clin Psychol 70, 510525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13. Lowe, MR & Butryn, ML (2007) Hedonic hunger: a new dimension of appetite? Physiol Behav 91, 432439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14. Harrar, V, Toepel, U, Murray, MM et al. (2011) Food's visually perceived fat content affects discrimination speed in an orthogonal spatial task. Exp Brain Res 214, 351356.Google Scholar
15. Berridge, KC (1996) Food reward: Brain substrates of wanting and liking. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 20, 125.Google Scholar
16. Berridge, KC (2009) ‘Liking’ and ‘wanting’ food rewards: brain substrates and roles in eating disorders. Physiol Behav 97, 537550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Robinson, TE & Berridge, KC (1993) The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res Brain Res Rev 18, 247291.Google Scholar
18. Nijs, IMT & Franken, IHA (2012). Attentional processing of food cues in overweight and obese individuals. Curr Obes Rep 1, 106113.Google Scholar
19. Nijs, IMT, Muris, P, Euser, AS et al. (2010). Differences in attention to food and food intake between overweight/obese and normal-weight females under conditions of hunger and satiety. Appetite 54, 243254.Google Scholar
20. Smeets, E, Roefs, A & Jansen, A (2009). Experimentally induced chocolate craving leads to an attentional bias in increased distraction but not in speeded detection. Appetite 53, 370375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
21. Fairburn, CG (1997) Eating disorders. In Cognitive Behaviour Therapy: Science and Practice, pp. 209241 [Clark, DM and Fairburn, CG, editors]. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
22. Lee, M & Shafran, R (2004) Information processing biases in eating disorders. Clin Psychol Rev 24, 215238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23. Williamson, DA, White, MA, York-Crowe, E et al. (2004) Cognitive-behavioral theories of eating disorders. Behav Modif 28, 711738.Google Scholar
24. Andreyeva, T, Long, MW, Henderson, KE et al. (2010). Trying to lose weight: diet strategies among Americans with overweight or obesity in 1996 and 2003. J Am Diet Assoc 110, 535542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25. Jeffery, RW, Folsom, AR, Luepker, RV et al. (1984) Prevalence of overweight and weight loss behavior in a metropolitan adult population: the Minnesota Heart Survey experience. Am J Public Health 74, 349352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
26. Williamson, DF, Serdula, MK, Anda, RF et al. (1992) Weight loss attempts in adults: goals, duration, and rate of weight loss. Am J Public Health 82, 1251.Google Scholar
27. Legenbauer, T, Vocks, S, Betz, S et al. (2011) Differences in the nature of body image disturbances between female obese individuals with versus without a comorbid binge eating disorder: an exploratory study including static and dynamic aspects of body image. Behav Modif 35, 162186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28. Knudsen, EI (2007) Fundamental components of attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 30, 5778.Google Scholar
29. LaBerge, D (2002) Attentional control: brief and prolonged. Psychol Res 66, 220233.Google Scholar
30. Overduin, J, Jansen, A & Louwerse, E (1995) Stroop interference and food intake. Int J Eat Disord 18, 277285.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31. Williams, JM, Mathews, A & MacLeod, C (1996) The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol Bull 120, 324.Google Scholar
32. Field, M & Cox, WM (2008) Attentional bias in addictive behaviors: a review of its development, causes, and consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend 97, 120.Google Scholar
33. Cox, WM, Fadardi, JS & Klinger, E (2006) Motivational processes underlying implicit cognition in addiction. In Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction, pp. 253266 [Wiers, RW and Stacy, AW, editors]. Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
34. Phaf, RH & Kan, KJ (2007) The automaticity of emotional Stroop: a meta-analysis. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 38, 184199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35. MacLeod, C, Mathews, A & Tata, P (1986) Attentional bias in emotional disorders. J Abnorm Psychol 95, 1520.Google Scholar
36. Mogg, K, Bradley, BP, De Bono, J et al. (1997) Time course of attentional bias for threat information in non-clinical anxiety. Behav Res Ther 35, 297303.Google Scholar
37. Mogg, K, Bradley, BP, Miles, F et al. (2004) Time course of attentional bias for threat scenes: testing the vigilance-avoidance hypothesis. Cognit Emotion 18, 689700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
38. Koster, EH, Crombez, G, Verschuere, B et al. (2006) Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behav Res Ther 44, 17571771.Google Scholar
39. Koster, EH, Crombez, G, Verschuere, B et al. (2007) A time-course analysis of attentional cueing by threatening scenes. Exp Psychol 54, 161171.Google Scholar
40. Rinck, M, Becker, ES, Kellermann, J et al. (2003) Selective attention in anxiety: distraction and enhancement in visual search. Depress Anxiety 18, 1828.Google Scholar
41. Smeets, E, Roefs, A, van Furth, E et al. (2008) Attentional bias for body and food in eating disorders: increased distraction, speeded detection, or both? Behav Res Ther 46, 229238.Google Scholar
42. Field, M & Christiansen, P (2012) Commentary on, ‘Internal reliability of measures of substance-related cognitive bias’. Drug Alcohol Depend 124, 189190.Google Scholar
43. Field, M, Munafó, MR & Franken, IHA (2009) A meta-analytic investigation of the relationship between attentional bias and subjective craving in substance abuse. Psychol Bull 135, 589607.Google Scholar
44. Castellanos, EH, Charboneau, E, Dietrich, MS et al. (2009) Obese adults have visual attention bias for food cue images: evidence for altered reward system function. Int J Obes 33, 10631073.Google Scholar
45. Nathan, PJ, O'Neill, BV, Mogg, K et al. (2012) The effects of the dopamine D3 receptor antagonist GSK598809 on attentional bias to palatable food cues in overweight and obese subjects. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 15, 149161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46. Schmitz, F, Naumann, E, Trentowska, M et al. (2014) Attentional bias for food cues in binge eating disorder. Appetite 80, 7080.Google Scholar
47. Nummenmaa, L, Hietanen, JK, Calvo, MG et al. (2011) Food catches the eye but not for everyone: a BMI-contingent attentional bias in rapid detection of nutriments. PLoS ONE 6, 16.Google Scholar
48. Graham, R, Hoover, A, Ceballos, NA et al. (2011) Body mass index moderates gaze orienting biases and pupil diameter to high and low calorie food images. Appetite 56, 577586.Google Scholar
49. Gearhardt, AN, Treat, TA, Hollingworth, A et al. (2012) The relationship between eating-related individual differences and visual attention to foods high in added fat and sugar. Eat Behav 13, 371374.Google Scholar
50. Werthmann, J, Roefs, A, Nederkoorn, C et al. (2011) Can(not) take my eyes off it: Attention bias for food in overweight participants. Health Psychol 30, 561569.Google Scholar
51. Loeber, S, Grosshans, M, Korucuoglu, O et al. (2012) Impairment of inhibitory control in response to food-associated cues and attentional bias of obese participants and normal-weight controls. Int J Obes 36, 13341339.Google Scholar
52. Pothos, EM, Tapper, K & Calitri, R (2009) Cognitive and behavioral correlates of BMI among male and female undergraduate students. Appetite 52, 797800.Google Scholar
53. Calitri, R, Pothos, EM, Tapper, K et al. (2010). Cognitive biases to healthy and unhealthy food words predict change in BMI. Obesity 18, 22822287.Google Scholar
54. Giel, KE, Friederich, HC, Teufel, M et al. (2011) Attentional processing of food pictures in individuals with anorexia nervosa–an eye-tracking study. Biol Psychiatry 69, 661667.Google Scholar
55. Shafran, R, Lee, M, Cooper, Z et al. (2007) Attentional bias in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 40, 369380.Google Scholar
56. Shafran, R, Lee, M, Cooper, Z et al. (2008) Effect of psychological treatment on attentional bias in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 41, 348354.Google Scholar
57. Veenstra, EM & de Jong, PJ (2012) Attentional bias in restrictive eating disorders. Stronger attentional avoidance of high-fat food compared to healthy controls? Appetite 58, 133140.Google Scholar
58. Hollitt, S, Kemps, E, Tiggemann, M et al. (2010) Components of attentional bias for food cues among restrained eaters. Appetite 54, 309313.Google Scholar
59. Meule, A, Vögele, C & Kübler, A (2012) Restrained eating is related to accelerated reaction to high caloric foods and cardiac autonomic dysregulation. Appetite 58, 638644.Google Scholar
60. Forestell, CA, Lau, P, Gyurovski, II et al. (2012) Attentional biases to foods: the effects of caloric content and cognitive restraint. Appetite 59, 748754.Google Scholar
61. Neimeijer, RAM, de Jong, PJ & Roefs, A (2013) Temporal attention for visual food stimuli in restrained eaters. Appetite 64, 511.Google Scholar
62. Veenstra, EM, de Jong, PJ, Koster, EHW et al. (2010) Attentional avoidance of high-fat food in unsuccessful dieters. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 41, 282288.Google Scholar
63. Wilson, C & Wallis, D (2013). Attentional bias and slowed disengagement from food and threat stimuli in restrained eaters using a modified stroop task. Cognit Therapy Res 37, 127138.Google Scholar
64. Ahern, AL, Field, M, Yokum, S et al. (2010) Relation of dietary restraint scores to cognitive biases and reward sensitivity. Appetite 55, 6168.Google Scholar
65. Boon, B, Vogelzang, L & Jansen, A (2000) Do restrained eaters show attention toward or away from food, shape and weight stimuli? Eur Eat Disord Rev 8, 5158.Google Scholar
66. Papies, EK, Stroebe, W & Aarts, H (2008) The allure of forbidden food: on the role of attention in self-regulation. J Exp Soc Psychol 44, 12831292.Google Scholar
67. Klesges, RC, Isbell, TR & Klesges, LM (1992) Relationship between dietary restraint, energy intake, physical activity, and body weight: a prospective analysis. J Abnorm Psychol 101, 668674.Google Scholar
68. Werthmann, J, Roefs, A, Nederkoorn, C et al. (2013) Attention bias for food is independent of restraint in healthy weight individuals—An eye tracking study. Eat Behav 14, 397400.Google Scholar
69. Hardman, CA, Rogers, PJ, Etchells, KA et al. (2013) The effects of food-related attentional bias training on appetite and food intake. Appetite 71, 295300.Google Scholar
70. Kemps, E, Tiggemann, M, Orr, J et al. (2014) Attentional retraining can reduce chocolate consumption. J Exp Psychol Appl 20, 94102.Google Scholar
71. Kakoschke, N, Kemps, E & Tiggemann, M (2014) Attentional bias modification encourages healthy eating. Eat Behav 15, 120124.Google Scholar
72. Boutelle, KN, Kuckertz, JM, Carlson, J et al. (2014) A pilot study evaluating a one-session attention modification training to decrease overeating in obese children. Appetite 8, 180185.Google Scholar
73. Werthmann, J, Field, M, Roefs, A et al. (2014) Attention bias for chocolate increases chocolate consumption—An attention bias modification study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 45, 136143.Google Scholar
74. Kemps, E & Tiggemann, M (2009) Attentional bias for craving-related (chocolate) food cues. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 17, 425433.Google Scholar
75. Houben, K, Roefs, A & Jansen, A (2010) Guilty pleasures: implicit preferences for high calorie food in restrained eating. Appetite 55, 1824.Google Scholar
76. Roefs, A, Huijding, J, Smulders, FT et al. (2011) Implicit measures of association in psychopathology research. Psychol Bull 137, 149193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
77. Roefs, A, Quaedackers, L, Werrij, MQ et al. (2006) The environment influences whether high-fat foods are associated with palatable or with unhealthy. Behav Res Ther 44, 715736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Overview of evidence on attentional processing of food cues in obese, overweight and healthy weight samples (2009–2014)

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary of food-related attention bias studies in ED and control participants (2007–2011)

Figure 2

Table 3. A summary of food-related attention bias studies in restrained and unrestrained eaters (2000–2013)

Figure 3

Table 4. A summary of attention bias modification studies for food-related attentional bias and food intake (2013–2014)