Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T15:09:37.604Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mainstream popular music research: a musical update

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 March 2022

Bernhard Steinbrecher*
Affiliation:
Department of Music, University of Innsbruck, Austria
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This article reviews studies that examine internationally circulating music which has reached the upper echelon of all-genre single charts in the 21st century. The examinations will be used as examples for the analysis of sonic aesthetics that are embedded in a particular frame of cultural debate, which the article conceptualises as ‘mainstream popular music’. The research field is then mapped and discussed with regard to prevailing objectives, methods and findings, including elaborations on how future research might advance understanding of the aesthetics within the discourses of mainstream popular music and, thus, of contemporary culture at large. The literature review focuses on music- and listener-based analytical directions and critically reflects on the frequent absence of theoretically well-founded and empirically underpinned, context-sensitive music examinations, particularly with regard to quantitative and audience research. The concluding section calls for a more integrated perspective on mainstream popular music as a discourse and praxis formation.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Mainstream notions play an important role in music-cultural analysis. But instead of being deployed as a particular lens through which to advance understanding of contemporary music's characteristics, meanings and functions, the concept of the mainstream has often only been used to demarcate, rather vaguely, the binary other to the authentic, honest, subversive, and creative (Huber Reference Huber, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013, p. 8). As Alison Huber puts it, it ‘has been working quietly behind the scenes, upstaged by the romantic bravado of concepts such as “subculture” and “resistance”’ (Huber Reference Huber, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013, p. 4; see also, e.g. Thornton Reference Thornton1995; Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Baker, Bennett, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013; Jost Reference Jost2016). Difficulties arise already when trying to clearly define, in connection with music, the term mainstream, with its ‘significant cultural value’ (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Baker, Bennett, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013, p. ix), and thus to go beyond its often (apparently) commonsensical and ‘haphazard’ application (Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Baker, Bennett, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013, p. viii).

In the past 20 years, some attempts have actually been made to frame the term theoretically (e.g. Toynbee Reference Toynbee, Hesmondhalgh and Negus2002; Martel Reference Martel, Ranke and Schäfer2011; Huber Reference Huber, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013; Weisbard Reference Weisbard2014; Jost Reference Jost2016). However, the different ways in which it is being defined and what side of it is brought to the fore depend strongly on the particular scholar's perspective and intention, so there is still not really a consensus ‘on what its use describes’ (Huber Reference Huber, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013, p. 11). In the opening section of my article, I do not strive to stipulate what the mainstream is in musical or other aesthetic terms, i.e. what type of music the term suggests (see Toynbee Reference Toynbee, Hesmondhalgh and Negus2002, p. 149), but will conceptualise the implications that go along with when something or someone is/becomes ascribed to mainstream popular music.

Mapping the concept

Most basically, notions of the mainstream are considered in this article in the context of popular music, i.e. as music-related phenomena attached to internationally circulating styles such as pop, rock, hip-hop or EDM. The general relationship between popular music and its mainstream might be defined in such a way that the latter is the superlative or pinnacle version of the former: mainstream popular music is very popular popular music, striving for maximum popularity with the largest part of the listening audience and to succeed in top positions of the charts, which ‘define the most popular of popular music, the goal, the pinnacle of success’ (Parker Reference Parker1991, p. 205).Footnote 1

Somewhat similar to popular music though, mainstream popular music does not refer to a coherent musical category in the form of, for example, a defined genre which can be characterised by distinct formal, technical, semiotic, social or other ‘rules’ (Fabbri Reference Fabbri, Horn and Tagg1982). Instead, the term mainstream mostly functions additionally, as an attribution to more specific, and diverse, kinds of popular music. In relevant contemporary literature, for instance, one can find (albeit mostly vaguely defined) terms such as ‘mainstream hip-hop’ (Hansen and Hawkins Reference Hansen and Hawkins2018, p. 168; Duinker Reference Duinker2019a, p. 430), ‘mainstream rap’ (Singh and Tracy Reference Singh and Tracy2015, p. 98), ‘mainstream 1980s music’ (Traut Reference Traut2005, p. 57), ‘mainstream rock’ (Christenson et al. Reference Christenson, de Haan-Rietdijk and Roberts2018, p. 3), ‘mainstream contemporary R&B’ (Danielsen Reference Danielsen2017, p. 12), ‘mainstream EDM-pop’ (James Reference James2015) and ‘mainstream pop’ (Bradby Reference Bradby and Hawkins2017, p. 35; Brøvig-Hanssen and Danielsen Reference Brøvig-Hanssen and Danielsen2016, p. 56).Footnote 2

Seen from this perspective, common metaphorical ideas of the mainstream as representing the principal stream of a river claiming any nearby water (Huber Reference Huber, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013, p. 10), a ‘central subfield’ around which the field of pop-rock is scattered (Regev Reference Regev2013, p. 83) or a ‘stylistic middle ground’ (Toynbee Reference Toynbee, Hesmondhalgh and Negus2002, p. 150) might just be as misleading as relating mainstream to terms like ‘normal’ (cf. Huber Reference Huber, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013, p. 4) or ‘normalising’ (Weisbard Reference Weisbard2014, pp. 263–5). Because in the examples given, the norm or centre is probably rather to be found at hip-hop, rock or R'n’B themselves than in these genres’ stylistically subordinated mainstream offshoots, which evolved – similar to their alternative offshoots – out of them (and not necessarily vice versa; see also Middleton Reference Middleton, Born and Hesmondhalgh2000, p. 62).Footnote 3

A more operative definition of the mainstream attribution might read as follows: it is an evaluative term with strong adherence to economic aspects that refers to a process. Other than the (mostly non-judgementally used) terms popular music or pop music, mainstream is a description and an evaluation simultaneously (see also Jost Reference Jost2019, 1:45–1:55). Hans Otto Huegel notes that the term mainstream very often is used ex ante to devalue the aesthetics of the popular as cheap, trivial and trashy, with the consequence that its structures and qualities are being flagged as something not worth discussing (Huegel Reference Huegel2007, p. 10). Mainstream music is music that is discredited as being ‘banal, homogeneous, unsophisticated, undiscerning, uncultured, low, inauthentic, fake, commercial, conservative, unimaginative, conformist or just plain stupid’ (Huber Reference Huber, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013, p. 8). Something might sound ‘(too) mainstream’ (Jost Reference Jost2019), listeners define themselves ‘against the “mainstream” of commercial taste, wherever that might lie’ (Frith Reference Frith1998, p. 66) and artists are loath to create anything viewed as mainstream.

Hence, when examining sonic, visual or other objects by the concept of mainstream popular music, analysis can interpret them as to their embedment in a specific cultural debate (Wicke Reference Wicke2016, c. 1696) that entails particular discursive instruments for negotiating musical value (see Jost Reference Jost2016, p. 167). On that account, I would like to build up on Christofer Jost's mainstream considerations (2016, 2019). Drawing on Erving Goffman (Reference Goffman1977), he conceptualises the mainstream as a frame, i.e. ‘a scheme of interpretation that arises from collectively communicated experiences’ (Jost Reference Jost2019, 21:40–21:50). Against the backdrop of the cultural knowledge circulating at any given time, mainstream popular music is a particular praxis formation of producing and perceiving music that incorporates specific ways of collective thinking and action as well as underlying logics (Jost Reference Jost2019, 20:40–20:50). It is a specific ‘discursive framework’ in Lee Marshall's terms (Reference Marshall2011), giving listeners ‘the tools to make sense of the sounds they hear’ and to create musical meaning through their position within it (p. 159).

An essential instrument in this mainstream popular music frame concerns economic notions or notions of commerciality. Not only did cultural studies scholars such as Dick Hebdige (Reference Hebdige1979) consolidate the view that mainstream culture's principal objective is profit-maximising reproduction (and the strengthening of hegemonic power relations; see Taylor et al. Reference Taylor, Baker, Bennett, Taylor, Baker and Bennett2013), but it is particularly the term commercial music that becomes central in this regard. The term often appears either as an equivalent to the term mainstream music, or else both terms are considered as an entity in that the adjective commercial is put in front of the word mainstream (see, for example, the quote above from Frith). However, the respective use of this adjective, particularly in everyday language, is seldom neutral in terms of a description of state, i.e. of music generating (maximum) revenue, being marketed to a broad (as large as possible) audience and ‘addressing a large market which traverses geographical and social space and can be sustained over time; precisely a mainstream in other words’ (Toynbee Reference Toynbee, Hesmondhalgh and Negus2002, p. 160). Instead, the term commercial music most often functions as an ascribed intent associated with the music's creators. Drawing on Michael Parzer's content-analytical study on musical taste (2011), mainstream popular music is understood not merely as a commercially successful product, but also one that raises suspicions that it was created solely out of economic interests, i.e. exclusively with the intention of gaining (the highest-possible) financial profit rather than for its own aesthetic sake and, thus, lacking authenticity, artistry and creativity (Parzer Reference Parzer2011, pp. 160–8; see also Ackermann Reference Ackermann2019, pp. 188–91).

Thus, when popular music becomes interwoven into the mainstream debate, evaluations happen against the backdrop of an imaginary conception of particular intentions and production practices within capitalistic conditions, triggering specific beliefs as to why and how certain music is made. Basically, respective value judgements tend to refer more to the (commercial) process of development than to the aesthetics of the final (commercial) product. This becomes apparent, for example, when considering that mainstream popular music often carries the image of being polished, pre-processed, cut-and-dried, overproduced and removed of any edge whatsoever (Steinbrecher and Pichler Reference Steinbrecher and Pichler2021). Such attributions relate more strongly to how the songs were produced than to the music's inherent aesthetic qualities (such as its alleged catchinessFootnote 4).

In this context, ideas of professionalism together with technological changes play a peculiar role. While professional musicianship is usually positively charged at least in terms of an appreciation of musical skills,Footnote 5 the artistic value of popular chart tunes is questioned precisely because they are written and produced by professionals, i.e. by people who can make a living out of music, follow a pre-defined plan (rather than act intuitively) and use state-of-the art tools and equipment. The latter becomes relevant especially in the age of digital music production, where the skills that are required to work on a professional level are shifting more and more towards technological skills, towards creating and manipulating sounds in digital audio workstations. As a result, professionally made very popular popular music is also strongly involved in (re-)current discourses about the decline of musical quality in connection to the degrading relevance of conventional playing and singing skills.

What makes the negotiation of mainstream notions in relation to popular music also somewhat exceptional is that there is a concrete benchmark rather than an abstract one, namely high chart placements, to evaluate, for better or worse, whether these professionals succeeded in gearing musical aesthetics towards their (true or imputed) intentions. However, this confirmation (of aims or beliefs) is based only upon a momentary snapshot that needs to be taken at weekly, or today even shorter, intervals. On that account, I want to elaborate in more detail on my above-indicated idea of an inherently processual nature of musical mainstream(-ness): something or someone crosses over into the mainstream, becomes mainstream, is brought to the mainstream or slips out of the mainstream.

To analyse popular music as to this in-and-out process means to sharpen the focus on those instants when particular aesthetics, practices and discourses intermingle in such a way that they successfully correspond to (inter-)nationally prevailing fashions and trends of the day. Music that gains such a ‘mainstream momentum’ enters the broad public sphere, becomes visible (and hearable) in far-reaching media channels and gets interwoven in public cultural commentary.Footnote 6 Hence, sovereignty in matters of interpretation is suddenly shared among many commentators, going beyond music-centred or scene-specific discourses, and the music takes the function of a more general cultural evaluation surface on which society negotiates its dominant conceptions, narratives and ideologies.

This doesn't necessarily mean, though, that music which has successfully run through the process of integration and assimilation into the broad public sphere must merely be, at any given time, an aesthetic reflection or aural mirror of dominant discourses or practices. It can also act contrarily, as an active agent of interrogation. Either way, mainstream popular music can be analysed as a society's loudest but most provisional stage on which to perform or contest normativity and to reproduce or rework hegemonic constructions of gender, sexuality, race, class or identity. Within this perspective, it is important to consider that such broadly mediatised musical objects, images and artist personae do not act unidirectionally but enter specific contexts of perception, in which a large number of listeners ‘from diverse social groups and across large geographical areas’ (Toynbee Reference Toynbee, Hesmondhalgh and Negus2002, p. 150) find favour with them and load them, together at the same (short) time, with their own cultural meanings, values and experiences.

Hence, mainstream popular music provides a multiple casement window through which it is possible to identify and critically understand, comparatively, the mechanisms of cultural globalisation influence and the individual and collectively shared realities of meaningful experience (Jost Reference Jost2016, p. 164) that the music is capable of bringing about at particular moments in time (see also Steinbrecher forthcoming). As David Riesman remarked in 1950 in ‘Listening to Popular Music’, ‘we must go out and talk to various sorts of people in various moods to get at them. It may then appear that it is the audience which manipulates the product (and hence the producer), no less than the other way around’ (Riesman Reference Riesman1950, p. 361).

Mapping the field

In the following sections, a mapping of relevant literature aims to advance understanding of the sonic aesthetics that have been negotiated within the mainstream popular music frame in the last two decades. In the course of this review, research gaps and potentials for future research are discussed. In the light of my considerations so far, I operationalise the term mainstream popular music for capturing music which achieved commercial success as measured by its appearance in top regions of main single-charts and, thus, got potentially interwoven with the evaluative interrelationships above conceptualised.

Structurally, the literature review starts with the mapping of research that directly takes the musical characteristics of 21st century hit records as a starting point for interpretation (which I broadly categorise as music-based analysis). What sounds have entered (and left) the mainstream frame of popular cultural debate? What do respective analyses tell us about zeitgeist matters and professional creators’ production practices driven by commercial interest? The section starts with a relatively lengthy, critical reflection of quantitative big data examinations and continues with a discussion of music-theoretical perspectives, including an elaboration on production-focussed analysis.

The second section emphasises research in which the audiences of post-2000 mainstream popular music is the focus (listener-based analysis). It will ask specifically whether these kinds of studies also incorporate closer examinations of the music's sonic aspects and, thus, enhance knowledge of how international hits’ sounds are experienced and adapted by its listeners in the current millennium, why they like or dislike it and what kinds of social, psychological and aesthetic functions it fulfils.

Owing to limited space, the mapping of literature does not consider, for the most part, studies that are primarily concerned with 21st century mainstream popular music's artist personae and use musical analysis as one method for interpreting, for example, performative aspects and identity notions (approaches of this kind might be categorised as artist-based analysis).

My review includes peer-reviewed journal articles, monographs, edited volumes and PhD dissertations in the fields of popular music studies, music theory, music information retrieval (MIR), music psychology and sociology, ethnomusicology and media and celebrity studies, as well as, when appropriate, semi- or non-academic sources. Most of the review had to be done manually by scanning through titles and texts in search of matching research objects, because the search string ‘mainstream’ applied to various scholarly databases did not lead to satisfactory results (which, actually, underlines the aforementioned observation that the concept is seldom addressed explicitly).

Music-based analysis

Drawing on the assumption that the sounds within the mainstream popular music frame adhere to the music-cultural zeitgeist of any given time, it becomes a reasonable approach to examine charts quantitatively to show historical transitions, depict trends or, as in the case of so-called ‘Hit Song Science’ (Dhanaraj and Logan Reference Dhanaraj and Logan2005; Pachet and Roy Reference Pachet and Roy2008; Pachet Reference Pachet, Tao, Ogihara and Tzanetakis2012), discuss success predictability. Relevant studies can be grouped loosely in the MIR field using digitised, music-related, big data collections that are, to a certain extent, publicly available and/or originating on the web.Footnote 7 The favourite object of study in this regard is the US Billboard Top 100 charts, which have been analysed frequently in recent years, with the UK charts already also having been a focus.

To start this review section, I critically reflect on the findings, methods and data sources of such statistics-oriented approaches, particularly of those that retrace developments in the charts until at least the late 2000s. I consider a rather detailed discussion of big data examinations as important insofar as they are, at the moment, probably the most prevalent in media representation of popular music research and have thus become themselves a key driver of the mainstream popular music debate. However, what do they tell us about hit-songs’ ever-changing sonic aesthetics? Two major emphases are aspects of emotion and homogeneity.

According to Schellenberg and von Scheve's (Reference Schellenberg and von Scheve2012) findings, there was an increasing emergence of minor mode and slower tempos during the 1965–2009 period on Billboard's Top 40 charts, indicating that popular recordings ‘became sadder-sounding and more emotionally ambiguous since the 1960s’ (von Scheve 2012, p. 200). Interiano et al. (Reference Interiano, Kazemi, Wang, Yang, Yu and Komarova2018) similarly observed increasing negativity in the UK, noting that from 1985 to 2015, according to semantic descriptors, most songs experienced a ‘decline in happiness and brightness and an increase in sadness’ (p. 8). However, successful songs seem to defy this downward trend in mood, as they display a tendency to be happier, more party-like and danceable, less sad, more feminine and possess a brighter timbre than songs that fail to enter the charts (pp. 3–4 and 8).

Askin and Mauskapf (Reference Askin and Mauskapf2017) find, regarding, for instance, what they call ‘objective musical features’ (p. 17) of tempo, mode and key, that the most popular songs on the Billboard charts between 1958 and 2016 employ the ‘similarity–differentiation (or familiarity–novelty) tradeoff’ (p. 34) most successfully; that is, they do not sound too similar or too novel in relation to other successful songs from their eras. Mauch et al. (Reference Mauch, MacCallum, Levy and Leroi2015) claim that ‘contrary to current theories of musical evolution’ (p. 5), they have found no evidence that music on the charts between 1960 and 2010 is becoming progressively homogenous, as to matters of harmony and timbre. Regarding the relation between a song's popularity and the number and types of instruments, Nunes and Ordanini (Reference Nunes and Ordanini2014) detect some sort of homogeneity, in that two standard configurations have been used repeatedly between 1958 and 2012, but they assume that adding a distinct, standout instrument on top increases the chances of succeeding on the charts (p. 406). Serrà et al. (Reference Serrà, Corral, Boguña, Haro and Arcos2012) recognise certain general trends in the evolution of Western popular music between 1955 and 2010, noting that pitch transitions tend to have less variety, the timbral palette is getting more homogenous and the volume dynamics are becoming poorer in connection with more overall loudness (p. 5). Regarding song lyrics, studies by Nunes et al. (Reference Nunes, Ordanini and Valsesia2015) and Morris (Reference Morris2017) indicate that frequent repetition helps attain success on the US Billboard charts.

On first sight, statistical approaches such as the ones I have spotlighted here might act as a helpful orientation guide for pinpointing some aesthetic changes or continuities within the mainstream popular music frame. They might be of use in situating a song or artist musically in a certain period of time and in examining whether specific musical or lyrical aesthetics correspond with or deviate from some of the ‘most common’ characteristics. However, from a critical perspective, in my opinion, two principal limitations exist that need to be discussed.

First, these big data analyses do little to support our knowledge of why certain trends and transitions have happened and how they connected to broader contexts. Owing to the studies’ methodological orientation towards large-scale data and representativity, they are hardly able to explain the cultural, psychological, media-related and other reasons for why so many people bought and streamed, at a certain time and place, particular songs with particular sounds. Yet many of the authors seem to be aware of this fact.Footnote 8

The second problematic issue regarding these quantitative accounts, particularly from a musicologist's perspective, is the authors’ tendency to generalise and/or simplify certain findings and deduce from them some sort of universal validity,Footnote 9 as well as their reliance on often questionable information provided by external databases. Statements about the increase or decrease in musical heterogeneity, or in emotional states transferred through certain musical particularities, are received gratefully in the media, but are standing on shaky scientific ground. For example, Schellenberg and von Scheve's assumption of the existence of widespread and stable stimulus–response schemata in music, such as minor mode and slow tempo providing reliable cues on sadness in Western culture, remains dubious, even though they consider it ‘well established that […] sad-sounding music tends to be slow and minor’ (p. 1). Not only are there plenty of other musicalFootnote 10 and non-musicalFootnote 11 factors that may induce sad emotions, but popular music also can be rather ambiguous regarding tonal centres/tonic chords and modes (e.g. Capuzzo Reference Capuzzo2009; Moore Reference Moore2012; Everett Reference Everett, Helms and Phleps2012; Welch Reference Welch2015; Peres Reference Peres2016; Spicer Reference Spicer2017; Richards Reference Richards2017; Sterbenz Reference Sterbenz2018). Moreover, I question whether the tonic chord is psychologically always so important that it kind of superimposes over all of the other chords (p. 199) which, of course, all can be major chords. The global 2017 hit ‘Despacito’ is not necessarily a sad song, nor is LMFAO's ‘Party Rock Anthem’ (2011).Footnote 12

Many of the studies must also be scrutinised insofar as their findings are based, to a large extent, either on quite arbitrary-seeming, machine-generated annotations provided by databases such as acousticbrainz.com (e.g. Interiano et al. Reference Interiano, Kazemi, Wang, Yang, Yu and Komarova2018), or in the case of the timbre classifications from Mauch et al. (Reference Mauch, MacCallum, Levy and Leroi2015), on human ‘expert’ annotations to concatenated sounds that are, in my opinion at least, not immediately comprehensible.Footnote 13 Mauch et al.'s data source, Echo Nest, provides no information on how attributes like ‘danceability’ are ascribed any further than: ‘how suitable a track is for dancing. This measure includes tempo, regularity of beat and beat strength’. The definition of ‘energy’ is similarly insufficient: ‘A perceptual measure of intensity throughout the track. Think fast, loud and noisy (i.e. hard rock) more than dance tracks’ (Askin and Mauskapf, Reference Askin and Mauskapf2017, p. 10). ‘Danceability’ is also one of the high-level information criteria on acousticbrainz.com, which is 100% sure that Calvin Harris’ tropical house hit ‘My Way’ is ‘not danceable’ and ‘not party’,Footnote 14 and that Drake's ‘God's Plan’ is ‘atonal’ (probability 96.4%).Footnote 15

Concerning such classifications, we must take into account that in-depth research on the correlation between certain musical dimensions in actual songs and listeners’ movements remains in its early stages (e.g. Iyer Reference Iyer2002; Eitan and Granot Reference Eitan and Granot2006; Kozak Reference Kozak2015; Godøy et al. Reference Godøy, Song, Nymoen, Haugen and Jensenius2016), that related notions of groove are a multidimensional field of research (Middleton Reference Middleton, Horner and Swiss1999; Pressing Reference Pressing2002; Danielsen Reference Danielsen2006; Klingmann Reference Klingmann2010; Pfleiderer Reference Pfleiderer2010; Gerischer Reference Gerischer2010) and that not that many analytical approaches towards energy and intensity processes exist that go beyond tonal function (e.g. Berry Reference Berry1987; Huron Reference Huron2006; Butler Reference Butler2006; Steinbrecher Reference Steinbrecher2016; Peres Reference Peres2016). Altogether, to make a reasonable music-based interpretation of 21st century mainstream popular music possible through the lens of statistical feature-extraction methods, it seems necessary to first take a few steps back towards potential fundamentals of contemporary hit-records’ musical language and see what current music theory has to say about it.

Stressing the metaphor of centre and periphery, it cannot be said that international top hits of the current millennium are in the centre of ‘popmusicology's’ (Bielefeldt et al., Reference Bielefeldt, Dahmen, Großmann, Bielefeldt, Dahmen and Großmann2008, p. 7) analytical canon. Asaf Peres remarks that:

Although the body of popular music scholarship in music theory has grown substantially in the last ten to fifteen years, 21st century commercial pop music is still little represented as a research subject in this field. […] This is a significant gap in the literature, considering not only the cultural impact of this music, but also the technological and artistic revolution that it represents. (Peres Reference Peres2016, pp. 4–5)

The scope of popular music analysis has clearly expanded beyond its traditional focus, which used to be primarily the sounds of (classic and progressive) rock music and the Beatles, but ‘analyses of top 40 pop, hip-hop or electronic dance music […] are still hard to find’, as von Appen et al. note (Reference von Appen, Doehring, Helms, Moore, von Appen, Doehring, Helms and Moore2015a, p. 2; see also de Clercq Reference de Clercq2020). In recent collections of popular music analysis, they also play a rather subordinate role (e.g. Scotto et al. Reference Scotto, Smith and Brackett2018; Julien and Levaux Reference Julien and Levaux2018). However, a closer look at current literature reveals that music theoretical approaches towards post-2000 hit records are slowly becoming more numerous. Broadly speaking, there are two ways in which the sounds are viewed in these studies.

One the one hand, rather conventional methods of harmony and form analysis are applied to a broad repertoire of Billboard Hot 100 songs. For example, Mark Richards (Reference Richards2017) shows, on the basis of an analysis of 2,715 songs (1990–2016), how diverse and tonally ambiguous the harmonic ‘axis progressions’ of Am–F–C–G (and its transpositions) can be, which ‘have become staples of mainstream popular songs from the last decades of the twentieth century and the first decades of the twenty-first’ (para. 1). Harald Scholz similarly concludes that there is variety within simplicity regarding the harmonic layout of the ‘musical mainstream’ from 2001 to 2010, after analysing 326 US Top 10 R&B and hip-hop songs (Scholz Reference Scholz2014, p. 316). New song forms in the ‘current pop mainstream’ featuring single-loop repetitions, post-choruses and ‘pop drops’ are shown in an ongoing corpus analysis project that examines the Billboard Year End Top 10 from 2006 to 2016 (von Appen and Frei-Hauenschild Reference von Appen and Frei-Hauenschild2018).

On the other hand, authors take hit songs from the 21st century as representative examples to discuss new analytical conceptualisations regarding particular features of popular music. For instance, Robin Attas’ consideration of the buildup introduction process in popular music (Reference Attas2015) includes an analysis of Nelly Furtado's ‘Say It Right’ (2006). Attas hears the song's beginning, in which the stepwise addition of instruments increases the textural and metric density, as a typical example of a so-called ‘projection shift’; that is, an in-time musical process that offers listeners different metric interpretation possibilities depending upon their choice of focus (pp. 282–3). Mark Spicer, in his text about ‘the sometimes tricky question of tonality in pop and rock songs’ (2017, abst.), picks out Daft Punk's ‘Get Lucky’ (2013) as a ‘textbook example’ of an ‘autotelic groove’ (para. 25). Owing to the (rather fragile) harmonic–functional make-up and the anacrustic bass line, the first measure of the song's fundamental four-bar loop can be perceived, according to Spicer, both as a beginning and an ending.

Somewhat similarly to Attas and Spicer, Ben Duinker (Reference Duinker2019b), Kyle Adams (Reference Adams2019) and David Forrest (Reference Forrest2017) identify ambiguities in the sounds of post-millennial hit records. Duinker illustrates hybrid tonics and plateau loops within the Chainsmokers’ ‘Something Just Like This’ (2017) and Sam Smith's ‘Dancing with a Stranger’ (2019), among others. Adams shows, with the examples of Flo Rida's ‘Cake’ (2017) and David Guetta's ‘Don't Leave Me Alone’ (2018), notions of formal instability in connection with timbral aspects and subtle changes in texture. A ‘perceptual paradox’ is observed by Forrest, who analyses Twenty One Pilots’ ‘Heathens’ (2016) as one example in which minor-mode melodic half-step motions against harmonic major-third progressions evoke an uncanny experience and invite the listener ‘to question the line that divides consonance and dissonance into two mutually exclusive categories’ (para. 2). In one of my own articles (Steinbrecher Reference Steinbrecher2021), I provide a very nuanced view, literally, on hit songs of the 2010s. I discuss and conceptualise how microrhythmic and microtonal nuances potentially affect – for example, through tiny offset-anticipations, subtly nested asymmetrical rhythms or articulatory vocal variations – the aesthetic experience of tensity and motion within the hooks of broadly commercialised songs such as Lukas Graham's ‘7 Years’ (2015), Calvin Harris feat. Rihanna's ‘This Is What You Came For’ (2016), Post Malone's ‘rockstar’ (2017) and others.

However, as to the process of building musical theory concerning sounds that entered the mainstream popular music frame recently, more comprehensive examinations of a broad variety of songs are certainly required to be able to explain whether the musical features identified in these case analyses are integral parts of international hit singles of a certain period, or rather exceptions to the norm. In the context of phrase analysis, Robin Attas remarks that:

Finally, it might be objected that to study phrase rhythm in such mainstream artists as Sarah McLachlan is to miss the truly interesting manipulations of phrase and phrase rhythm that exist in popular music. I would respond by citing the work of Philip Tagg (Reference Tagg1987), who has argued that the basic musical codes, the general ways in which popular music communicates to the masses, need to be examined before the exceptional effects of alternative practices can be fully understood (284). (Attas Reference Attas2011, para. 38)

PhD dissertations such as those by Peres (Reference Peres2016) and David Geary (Reference Geary2019) provide important contributions to enhancing understanding of these ‘general ways’, because the scholars illustrate their expanded analytical models – on sonic energy (Peres) and the rhetorical function of drum patterns (Geary) – on a broad range of contemporary Top 40 music examples, including songs from artists such as Ariana Grande, Major Lazer, Zayn Malik, Cardi B, Ed Sheeran, DJ Khalid and others. Peres finds that mainstream 21st century pop music uses timbre, gesture and spatialisation ‘in a highly systematized and categorizable way’ (2016, p. 184) and he illuminates a central sonic syntax consisting of setup, buildup and peak as its three major functions (p. 183). According to Geary's Billboard charts analysis, the ‘top fifty mainstream popular songs of 2018’ have an average number of approximately eight drum pattern changes per song and, thus, an emphasis on drum pattern variety that ‘often outweighs the variability of other musical elements such as harmonic progressions and melodic lines’ (2019, p. 119).

Viewed from a more specific perspective, the outlined music-theoretical approaches actually reveal a significant blind spot regarding a central aspect of mainstream popular music discourse, i.e. they hardly touch on production and technology topics. A lack of production-based theoretical analysis that considers in detail ‘the technical aspects of a song's production’ (de Clercq Reference de Clercq2016 para. 8) is certainly an issue in popular music analysis in general. Bates and Bennett (Reference Bates, Bennett, Bennett and Bates2018) indicate awareness of this, stating in the introduction of their co-edited companion Critical Approaches to the Production of Music and Sound that ‘analytical work such as that carried out by [Philip] Tagg and [Allan F.] Moore is notably absent from the discourse’ (p. 12; see also de Clercq Reference de Clercq2016). Brøvig-Hanssen and Danielsen (Reference Brøvig-Hanssen and Danielsen2016) also note the importance of generating more music analysis of technological aspects (p. 4) in their book about digitisation's impact on the aesthetics of popular music. Ben Duinker raises even broader criticism of the lack of music theoretical work that takes ‘as a starting orientation the practices of composition and performance’ (2019b, para. 7.4).

Generally, 21st century hit-records’ music often is given only short shrift in recent publications on sound and recording issues in popular culture. For example, it is neither in the focus of the critical ‘phonomusicological’ (p. 1) approaches in the aforementioned companion edited by Bennett and Bates (Reference Bennett and Bates2018), nor is it explicitly addressed in Schulze (Reference Schulze2015), Mazierska et al. (Reference Mazierska, Gillon and Rigg2018) and in the Journal on the Art of Record Production. One small spotlight can be found in the IASPM Journal special issue, ‘Perspectives on Popular Music and Sound Recording’ (Bennett and Klein Reference Bennett and Klein2016), in which the recording studio processing of Miley Cyrus’ highly audible breath sounds in ‘Wrecking Ball’ (2013) is analysed (Weinstein Reference Weinstein2016). Weinstein finds that through this artificial construction of humanness, a ‘mismatched woman’ is mediated to ‘assure[s] listeners of the authenticity and wholeness of the woman's musicking body’ (p. 131). A somewhat contrary use of technology is noted by Brøvig-Hanssen and Danielsen (Reference Brøvig-Hanssen and Danielsen2016), who analyse the autotuned voice of Lady Gaga in the song ‘Starstruck’ (2008) as an explicit withdrawal ‘from the authentic expression generally implied by (and expected of) the pop singer’ (p. 130).Footnote 17

Examinations of sound effects and individual sounds may turn out to be one essential key to closing the gap between production-oriented approaches and music-theoretical approaches, focussing more strongly on aesthetic matters. For example, Immanuel Brockhaus (Reference Brockhaus2017) analyses prototypical, ‘iconic’ sounds in music from the Billboard charts (1966–2014) – such as the hand-clap sound or sidechain compression – aesthetically, technologically and sociologically in his book Kultsounds.Footnote 18 A broader production-focussed perspective is provided by Simon Zagorski-Thomas, whose musical analysis of Kings of Leon's 2008 international hit ‘Sex on Fire’ discusses how the production techniques foster specific perceptions of narrative structure, gestural activity and participants’ interaction (Reference Zagorski-Thomas, von Appen, Doehring, Helms and Moore2015). In the same edited volume in which Zagorski-Thomas’ text was published, one can also find a rather experimental text from Ralf von Appen, who fictionally renarrates, ‘as a fly on the wall’ (Reference von Appen, von Appen, Doehring, Helms and Moore2015, p. 46), how Ke$ha's ‘Tik Tok’ (2010) came into being and subsequently reflects on how the song fulfils functions such as, for instance, to ‘hijack listeners from their daily routines’, ‘positively influence the listeners’ mood’ and ‘reflect current developments in society’ (pp. 47–8).Footnote 19

Compared with purely scholarly discourse, there seems to be more interest in how mainstream popular music's sounds are made on the edge of academic and journalistic work. Worth mentioning are, for example, podcasts like Switched On Pop, ‘about the making and meaning of popular music’ (Sloan and Harding Reference Sloan and Harding2019; see also the book by Sloan and Harding Reference Sloan and Harding2020) and ‘And The Writer Is …’, with Ross Golan, which features songwriters and producers of Top 40 songs. An extensive source for hit-songs’ analysis is the platform songlexikon.de, which is published by the University of Freiburg (Fischer et al. Reference Fischer, Hörner and Jost2019). At the moment, it contains more than 220 short song examinations of ‘All-Time Greatest Hits’ (38 of them released after the year 2000), providing information about the songs’ musicians and origins, as well as their production context, content and reception.Footnote 20

Moreover, plenty of YouTube channels provide detailed tutorials and song breakdowns, often from the composers and producers themselves.Footnote 21 They can be found on channels such as Deconstructed (Genius), The Breakdown (Pyramind), Internet Money, Dave Pensado, imamusicmogul, Diary of a Song (New York Times), Future Music magazine and others. Although the motivation for these kinds of examinations is often not primarily scholarly, but rather educational and/or marketing-driven, they offer valuable musicological insights into current production techniques. From the perspective of knowledge transfer mechanisms, such production-related videos generally are becoming increasingly important to the whole popular music creation process. This becomes apparent when commercially successful current artists, particularly in the field of chart-oriented hip-hop and EDM, openly speak about how they learned and professionalised their skills from YouTube tutorials.Footnote 22

A systematic analysis of these sources – which, to my knowledge, has yet to be conducted – could reveal superordinate aesthetic production schemata (Diaz-Bone Reference Diaz-Bone2002) and, thus, support the understanding of the mainstream popular music frame and the aesthetic (sonic) objects within. Topics that could be examined are, for instance, the ubiquitous principle of trial and error, the self-evident handling of DAW presets, the seamless crossing-over between idea-sketching (on a smartphone) and recording, repeatedly stressed instinctiveness notions, (fast) production pace and adding ‘live(li)ness’ or ‘human elements’ to the digital texture, or, more generally, the technology-related change from auditory-based to visually/computer-screen-based creation and recording.

Listener-based analysis

Titling this second section of my literature review ‘Listener-based analysis’ might seem a rather odd choice at first because most – but actually not all – of the aforementioned hit-record examinations are based on the analysing person's listening impressions. Certainly, there has been a rising awareness in popular music analysis of the reflection of the analyst's subjectivity in hearing, experiencing and interpreting music (e.g. Moore Reference Moore2012, pp. 329–30; Doehring Reference Doehring, Helms and Phleps2012, pp. 37–8; von Appen et al. Reference von Appen, Doehring, Helms, Moore, von Appen, Doehring, Helms and Moore2015a, p. 1). It is somewhat remarkable, though, that in recent analyses particularly of mainstream popular music, some scholars take their personal aversions as a starting point for song examinations (of course, without making general value judgements; Helms Reference Helms, von Appen, Doehring, Helms and Moore2015; Doehring Reference Doehring, Helms and Phleps2015; von Appen Reference von Appen, Ahlers and Jacke2017). However, considering that popular music musicologists are usually not among those who make mainstream popular music an essential part of their cultural practices and aesthetic experiences (or at least listen and dance to it frequentlyFootnote 23), their elaborations do not really enhance understanding of what heavy Top 40 music listeners exactly do with, and how they make sense of, ‘their’ music and why they like or dislike certain sonic constellations.

Thus, this section's attention is directed towards empirically underpinned audience studies in connection with 21st century mainstream popular music and the question of how music analysis is interwoven into these approaches. The necessity of such entangled research has already been tied to MIR approaches, and it is noted further by von Appen, who remarks that:

A vocal phrase or a drum pattern is intersubjectively perceived as ‘cool’ [‘lässig’] or ‘pushing’ [‘drängend’] – but what kind of compositional processes in connection with micro-rhythm and -intonation cause that perception? How do you accomplish maximum hardness? What has to be given so that a bass pattern is described as ‘funky’? […] Producers of huge chart hits seemingly know by trial and error how danceability is achieved, how a chorus takes off and how to make the catchiest possible melody. But wouldn't it be important to understand such processes also scholarly? (von Appen Reference von Appen, Auhagen, Bullerjahn and Höge2012, p. 15, transl. BSt.)

Given that ‘hooking’ the listener is obviously a fundamental aspect of hit tunes (see also Steinbrecher Reference Steinbrecher2021), a potentially fruitful resource of listener-based research is one that focusses on notions of catchiness and song memorability. Surprisingly, however, the cognitive psychology research field concerned with involuntary music imagery has very little to say about the musical properties of actual earworms, such as Lady Gaga's 2009 hit ‘Bad Romance’ (Jakubowski et al. Reference Jakubowski, Stewart, Finkel and Müllensiefen2017). Liikkanen and Jakubowski (Reference Liikkanen and Jakubowski2020), in their review of empirical research in involuntary music imagery, state that ‘research on the importance of musical characteristics is still in its infancy’ (p. 15). Some insight has been provided by Jakubowski et al. (Reference Jakubowski, Stewart, Finkel and Müllensiefen2017), who indicate, on the basis of a mixed-methods approach using online questionnaires and melodic-structural feature extraction from commercial MIDI files, that fast-tempo and arch-shaped melodic phrases are helpful in generating earworms (p. 130). The actual shortcomings of applying this kind of automated music analysis is noted by the scholars themselves, who remark that their data analysis is only symbolic because they do not measure song recordings’ actual audio features, ‘such as loudness, timbral content, rhythmic clarity and so forth’ (Jakubowski et al. Reference Jakubowski, Stewart, Finkel and Müllensiefen2017, p. 133).

By and large, also in the listener- and listening-function-focussed area of mainstream popular music analysis, one can observe a trend towards the use of quantitative (computerised) measurement instruments.Footnote 24 However, recent studies concerned with a vast amount of songs from the UK or US charts in connection with notions of emotion and mood, for example, North et al. (Reference North, Krause, Sheridan and Ritchie2018; Reference North, Krause, Sheridan and Ritchie2019a, Reference North, Krause, Sheridan and Ritchieb) and Krause and North (Reference Krause and North2020), skip the step of asking listeners directly for their experiences in favour of artificial intelligence processing. Some other quantitative studies utilising machine learning in the field of music emotion recognition integrate popular music listeners’ perspectives to some extent in that they combine audio feature extraction with the retrieval of social, tag-based emotion annotations from services such as Last.fm (Song et al. Reference Song, Dixon and Pearce2012; Jamdar et al. Reference Jamdar, Abraham, Khanna and Dubey2015; Aljanaki et al. Reference Aljanaki, Yang and Soleymani2017). In its current state, though, this research field provides hardly any concrete or differentiated information about emotional responses to popular music in general and mainstream popular music in particular (see also Beveridge and Knox Reference Beveridge and Knox2018, p. 412), as it seems to be concerned mostly with methodological issues in connection with dataset choices or classification processes. Markus Schedl sees one fundamental reason for relevant scientific work remaining in a ‘fledgling state’ in that ‘involving users, which is an obvious necessity to build user-aware approaches, is time-consuming and hardly feasible on a large scale – at least not in academia’ (Schedl Reference Schedl2016, p. 2).Footnote 25

Mainstream popular music analysis and music-oriented emotion studies generally turn out to be a rather bad match. Eerola and Vuoskoski (Reference Eerola and Vuoskoski2013), in their music and emotion studies literature review, state that ‘it is somewhat surprising that many popular genres such as R&B/soul, dance or chart pop are featured rarely or not at all in the studies on music and emotion’ (2013, p. 320) and that music-specific emotion models overall are ‘still relatively rarely used’ (p. 324). For example, a 2018 Beveridge and Knox study uses semi-prepared popular music tracks in ‘a style similar to UK chart-orientated music of the last decade’ (p. 412) as stimuli in listening tests and concludes that rhythm and articulation are the most important factors in expressing emotion through vocal melody (p. 420).

The latter study is noteworthy insofar as the scholars played (at least somewhat) actual songs for participants to gain differentiated insights into how listeners perceive and make psychological use of mainstream popular music. Particularly in research using experimental methods, ‘there exists a general aversion to the study of emotion in pop music from the listener's perspective’ (2013, p. 17), as Emery Schubert remarks. In his own study, Schubert examines emotional responses to participants’ freely self-selected music, finding that songs such as those from Justin Bieber, Lady Gaga and Bruno Mars ‘evoke and express emotions that are equally powerful to high-art music’ (Schubert Reference Schubert2013, para. 46). As for the question ‘What do young people hear when listening to music?’ Herbert and Dibben (Reference Herbert and Dibben2018) show, by conducting an online listening study, that 10- to 18-year-old listeners are likely to claim ownership of a ‘four-chord pop’ song like Kelly Clarkson's 2004 hit ‘Behind These Hazel Eyes’, as they link extracts of it to personal everyday functions (p. 384).

In my opinion, it would make sense to connect music-analytically to approaches like those of Schubert and of Herbert and Dibben to better understand, for instance, which ‘extracts’ the listeners gravitate towards precisely and why they pick them. In other words, focus even more strongly and ‘simultaneously on the intra- and extra-musical meanings young people attach to music when listening’ (Herbert and Dibben Reference Herbert and Dibben2018, p. 388). Greb et al. (Reference Greb, Schlotz and Steffens2018) also point to the need for a stronger entanglement of music analytical methods in the study of listening functions, admitting that in their own examination of individual and situational variables in everyday-life music interactions, they did not address the question of ‘which music with specific musical characteristics people are listening to in order to fulfil the various functions of music listening’ (p. 790).

As for uses of mainstream popular music in particular, concrete music-analytical connection points are difficult to find – either in Greb et al.'s or in similar survey- and interview-based studies. Rather than being addressed explicitly as their own category or phenomenon in their own rights, mainstream notions are included in seemingly self-explanatory hybrid proxy genres like ‘pop’ or ‘pop/rock’. Given that ‘pop’ is, according to some studies, ‘upbeat and conventional’ (Vella and Mills Reference Vella and Mills2017), ‘rhythmic and intense’ (George et al. Reference George, Stickle, Rachid and Wopnford2007) and ‘percussive, electric, energetic and not sad’ (Rentfrow Reference Rentfrow2012, p. 408) music that is preferred by people who are ‘high in extraversion, value social recognition, endorse more gender stereotypes, have more permissive attitudes about sex and consider themselves physically attractive’ (Rentfrow Reference Rentfrow2012), then it would be interesting to see what listeners think about and how they relate to pop's pinnacle mainstream version. At least regarding listeners’ negotiation of musical preferences, explorative studies such as those from Parzer (Reference Parzer2011), Taren Ackermann (Reference Ackermann2019) and Steinbrecher and Pichler (Reference Steinbrecher and Pichler2021) have already indicated mainstream notions’ important role.

Ultimately, by placing more emphasis on mainstream popular music listeners, thereby supporting a culturally relativistic stance towards the music's analysis more aggressively (Mendivil Reference Mendivil, van Dyck-Hemming and Hemming2019, p. 123), more comprehensive qualitative audience research focussing on everyday listening contexts seems to be required. Naturalistic data-collection approaches, such as the experience-sampling method (e.g. Greasley and Lamont Reference Greasley and Lamont2011; Krause et al. Reference Krause, North and Hewitt2015), could be supportive in this regard, as can ethnographic methods. For example, Pruett (Reference Pruett2011) states that ‘the need for an ethnographic approach to mainstream popular music in the United States is particularly strong, but examples are few’ (Pruett Reference Pruett2011, p. 3), and Eliot Bates notes, in his Reference Bates2013 analysis of popular music studies journals, that ‘few ethnographic articles concern the music of Western Europe, Canada and the US’ (p. 19). Remarkably, none of the studies in his review examines mainstream currents. After all, referring to the argument at the very beginning of this section, a listener-aware understanding of mainstream popular music's sounds might already be enhanced by a shared approach challenging scholars’ individual readings, for example, in the form of the group analysis method proposed by von Appen et al. (Reference von Appen, Doehring, Helms and Moore2015b, pp. 3–6 and pp. 175–276).

Conclusion

The different approaches that I have outlined in this article reveal – or potentially might reveal – various facets of 21st century mainstream popular music's sounds from diverse perspectives. However, owing to these (and other) approaches’ diversity, the research field presents itself as quite fragmented, raising the question of how a more integrated perspective could be shaped, one that more strongly considers the interrelations between the aesthetics, acts and actors within the frame of cultural debate that I conceptualised, drawing on Christofer Jost, as mainstream popular music.

Jost (Reference Jost2016, p. 170) also suggests a possibly fruitful conceptual grasp of the field's research goals. He directs the main focus of mainstream popular music analysis towards the rhetoric of success, enabling a retracing of the structures of need and experience within a society. Simply put, how does something (musical) materialise that comparatively many people like, and in which circumstances do these people get to be sympathetic to it? The duality of presentation and production is a pivotal aspect in examining this formation's characteristics, requiring a cross-disciplinary, praxeological approach that integrates both production- and reception-oriented methods (p. 171).

Along these lines, context-sensitive music analysis equipped with an extended repertoire of music-theoretical concepts is, essentially, the key method for examining the role of the sounds within the actions and logics of the mainstream popular music frame, that is, for evaluating how sonic aesthetics contribute to the mainstream momentum of a particular song or artist in certain – musical and non-musical – contexts. In this regard, extensive qualitative case studies – comprising a combination of music, technology, media and discourse analysis with ethnographic and experimental methods – are certainly better suited to enhancing understanding of mainstream popular music than quantitative-only approaches. However, computed analyses from the fields of MIR or music emotion recognition potentially also might be useful in drawing a bigger picture, given that they add to qualitative research and, thus, focus more specifically on the music of particular artists in particular time periods and regions.

Of course, an essential part of such an integrated perspective on contemporary culture must concern mainstream's artist personae and the question of how they use the far-reaching mainstream stage to perpetuate or interrogate dominant conceptions and ideologies through their musical performance. The fact that I have generally left out artist-based analytical directions in my literature review doesn't mean that there is not already a growing body of studies to inform future research in this regard. Relevant analyses focussing, for instance, on gender-related aspects provide some fruitful indication about the range of possibilities (and limitations) for artists to successfully negotiating, by musical means, identity formations within the mainstream popular music frame(s) of the 21st century.Footnote 26

Important connection points for the analysis of these interrelations might also be found in research areas dealing with the construction, identification and functioning of stardom and celebrity culture.Footnote 27 Given that contemporary stars are an ‘individualised, personalised reflection of broader social or cultural meanings’ (Loy et al. Reference Loy, Rickwood, Bennett, Loy, Rickwood and Bennett2018, p. 1), then these meanings are potentially reflected in the stars’ current musical output, too. By focussing critically on the musical-aesthetic processes interrelated with an artist's star status, which has rarely been done on a theoretical level yet (Menrath Reference Menrath2019, p. 36),Footnote 28 it is possible to advance understanding of the myriad dimensions interwoven through a media persona which is appealing to a global audience and is integrated strongly into broad public (mainstream) discourse.

Footnotes

1 I have presented this definitional ‘superlative’ notion already elsewhere briefly, as well as my thoughts about uses of the term commercial music, in connection with musical nuances in popular music (Steinbrecher Reference Steinbrecher2021, pp. 113–14). Of course, popular music is itself a complex and discursive term, and popular music studies have struggled since their early days with a narrow framing of their core topic, by means of, e.g. quantitative, economic-technological, sociological or music-related criteria (Steinbrecher Reference Steinbrecher2016, pp. 18–26). There is at least some agreement on the common concern of the popular music research field, according to Hesmondhalgh and Negus, which is – and for me, this is also true regarding mainstream popular music – ‘questions about the relationship between music meaning, social power and cultural value’ (Reference Hesmondhalgh, Negus, Hesmondhalgh and Negus2002, p. 2).

2 A simple google search shows that word combinations the other way round, such as ‘pop mainstream’ or ‘rock mainstream’, are way less common.

3 Today, the rather paradoxical term alternative mainstream has become a label on its own, used, for example, in the musical self-description of the popular Austrian public radio station FM4 (https://docplayer.org/26470765-Presseinfos-zu-der-jugendkultursender-des-orf-fm4-homepage-fm4-orf-at-mail.html; accessed 30 September 2020). For further elaborations on the alternative mainstream see Keunert (Reference Keunert2015); for the concept of a ‘mainstream of minorities’ see Holert and Terkessidis (Reference Holert and Terkessidis1996).

4 If pop songs are defined through their focus on catchy hooks (Shepherd Reference Shepherd, Horn and Shepherd2003, p. 508) as well as their simplicity regarding beat, melody, harmonic accompaniment (Kramarz Reference Kramarz2006, p. 61) and structure (Everett Reference Everett and Everett2000, p. 272), then mainstream pop songs are potentially the epitome of catchiness and simplicity – ‘the most popular blend of different types of catchy, easy-on-the-ears, literally “popular” music’ (Gillet Reference Gillet1970 in ter Bogt et al. Reference ter Bogt, Delsing, van Zalk, Christenson and Meeus2011, p. 299).

5 In some case, like in punk, professional musicianship might also be deliberately neglected for reasons of attitude.

6 According to a study of Steinbrecher and Pichler (Reference Steinbrecher and Pichler2021), the term mainstream evokes strong associations with music that is played on the radio. For some adolescents, music becomes mainstream as soon as it is played on the radio, and when it already is mainstream, it ‘[could] simply be played on the radio and it would be absolutely OK’ (p. 8). Viewing big, public and commercial radio stations as one main platform for mediating mainstream popular music's sounds opens up an additional perspective on mainstream-related evaluations: they relate to music where the traditional manner of one-to-many broadcasting remains highly important. Thus, mainstream popular music might somewhat be already an antiquated term, because of its strong adherence to an old-fashioned concept of music distribution and, more generally, against the backdrop of fragmented media consumption, genre fluidity and (probably) outdated ideas of subculture.

7 For example, Million Song Dataset, Million Musical Tweets Dataset, wasabi.i3s.unice.fr, musicbrainz.org, acousticbrainz.org, the.echonest.com, info.geerdes.com, last.fm, libre.fm, discogs.com, genius.com, soundfacts.org or Shazam, which offer metadata about artists, songs, albums and production, as well as audio descriptions, acoustic features, MIDI transcriptions and information about lyrics. Moreover, they open up possibilities to examine user behaviours by making query data and geo-temporal data accessible.

8 ‘At the present time, we can only speculate about the causes and mechanisms driving this change, and it is unlikely that a single causal explanation would suffice’ (Schellenberg and von Scheve Reference Schellenberg and von Scheve2012, pp. 201–2); ‘We have not addressed the causes of the dynamics that we detect. Like any cultural artefact – and any living organism – music is the result of a variational-selection process’ (Mauch et al. Reference Mauch, MacCallum, Levy and Leroi2015, pp. 16–19); ‘We do not want to overstate the implications of our findings. Certainly marketing, artist preference, genre, catchy hooks, and other factors play a role in determining a song's relative success. For example, the star power of Rihanna may overcome any effect of instrumentation’ (Nunes and Ordanini Reference Nunes and Ordanini2014, p. 401); ‘We also recognize many factors unrelated to the music itself often are involved in determining what songs are popular, which the current studies did not (and could not) control for’ (Nunes and Ordanini Reference Nunes and Ordanini2014, p. 406); ‘We encourage the development of further historical databases to be able to quantify the major transitions in the history of music, and to start looking at more subtle evolving characteristics of particular genres or artists, without forgetting the whole wealth of cultures and music styles present in the world’ (Serrà et al. Reference Serrà, Corral, Boguña, Haro and Arcos2012, pp. 5–6).

9 ‘Those who wish to make claims about how and when popular music changed can no longer appeal to anecdote, connoisseurship and theory unadorned by data’ (Mauch et al. Reference Mauch, MacCallum, Levy and Leroi2015, p. 9)

10 ‘Low pitch, narrow pitch range, falling pitch contour, minor mode, slow tone attacks, slow tempo, dull and dark timbres, low sound level, little sound level variability, little highfrequency energy, microstructural irregularity and legato articulation (Huron Reference Huron2008; Juslin and Laukka Reference Juslin and Laukka2003; Post and Huron Reference Post and Huron2009; Turner and Huron Reference Huron2008)’ (Taruffi Reference Taruffi2016, p. 3).

11 Brain-stem response, evaluative conditioning, emotional contagion, visual imagery, episodic memory, music expectancy and cognitive appraisal (Juslin et al. Reference Juslin, Liljeström, Västfjäll, Lundqvist, Juslin and Sloboda2010, pp. 620–3).

12 Krumhansl (Reference Krumhansl2017) also is sceptical: ‘Schellenberg and von Scheve (Reference Schellenberg and von Scheve2012) hypothesised that this shift to minor mode and slower tempo would make more recent songs sound sadder, although they did not test this empirically’ (p. 1).

13 The 35 classes used in their T-Lexicon can be heard at Soundcloud, for instance the topic ‘female voice, melodic, vocal’: https://soundcloud.com/descent-of-pop/sets/t-topic-08-female-voice (accessed 28 January 2019).

17 Similar interpretations of mainstream pop singers’ manipulated voice, with a main focus on aspects of gender identity, can be found in James (Reference James2008), Mueller (Reference Mueller2018) and Hansen (Reference Hansen2018).

18 Discussions on the autotune effect can also be found, for instance, in Bonz (Reference Bonz2015), Marshall (Reference Marshall2017) and Reynolds (Reference Reynolds2018).

19 The edited volume Song Interpretation in 21st-Century Pop Music puts an emphasis, already conceptually, on 21st century popular music. It aims ‘to shed light on how popular tunes that many listen and dance to work musically’, also ‘highlighting mainstream pop (Lady Gaga, Ke$ha, Lucenzo, Amy MacDonald)’ (von Appen et al. Reference von Appen, Doehring, Helms, Moore, von Appen, Doehring, Helms and Moore2015a, p. 2).

21 For example, see the Chainsmokers on making their song ‘Roses’ (https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=HlUGRbPLn-4), singer/songwriter Mike Posner on the podcast Switched On Pop (http://www.switchedonpop.com/entering-beard-phase-with-mike-posner/) or producers Martin Garrix and Charlie Puth talking about the creation of their songs ‘Animals’ and ‘Attention’ (https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=CfCmoEixxro; https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=IU8BEMi8UyM).

23 See Elijah Wald (Reference Wald, Helms and Phleps2014, p. 29).

24 North et al. (Reference North, Krause and Ritchie2021) find that two out of the three themes which have emerged to date in ‘corpus-level work concerning music aesthetics’ (p. 736) are concerned with either psychological features or with the ‘relationship between particularly pop music and various social psychological and socioeconomic indicators’ (North et al. Reference North, Krause and Ritchie2021).

25 An example for another listener-based, quantitative approach towards hit-records’ sounds is Kaneshiro et al. (Reference Kaneshiro, Ruan, Baker and Berger2017), who examine the influence of musical features on Top 5 songs’ discovery via Shazam and find that ‘the onset of vocals and the first occurrence of the chorus in particular drive an increase in queries’ (p. 5).

26 For example, Djupvik (Reference Djupvik2014), Bradby (Reference Bradby and Hawkins2017), Hansen (Reference Hansen2018) and Hansen and Hawkins (Reference Hansen and Hawkins2018).

27 See, for example, Celebrity Studies (2019), Loy et al. (Reference Loy, Rickwood, Bennett, Loy, Rickwood and Bennett2018) and Baade et al. (Reference Baade, McGee and Smith2019).

28 A recent example of such music-oriented deliberations can be found in a Journal of Popular Music Studies special issue edited by Vernallis et al. (Reference Vernallis2018) in which 10 authors wrote a 60 page analysis of the Carters’ ‘Apes**t’ (2018).

References

Ackermann, T. 2019. Disliked Music. Merkmale, Gründe und Funktionen abgelehnter Musik PhD thesis (Kassel, Kassel University Press)Google Scholar
Adams, K. 2019. ‘Musical texture and formal instability in post-millenial popular music: two case studies’, Intégral, 33, pp. 3345Google Scholar
Aljanaki, A., Yang, Y., and Soleymani, M. 2017. ‘Developing a benchmark for emotional analysis of music’, PLoS ONE, 12/3, e0173392. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0173392CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Askin, N., and Mauskapf, M. 2017. ‘What makes popular culture popular?: product features and optimal differentiation in music’, American Sociological Review, 82/4. doi:10.1177/0003122417728662Google Scholar
Attas, R. 2011. ‘Sarah setting the terms: defining phrase in popular Music’, Music Theory Online, 17/3, https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.11.17.3/mto.11.17.3.attas.phpCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Attas, R. 2015. ‘Form as process: the buildup introduction in popular music’, Music Theory Spectrum, 37, pp. 275–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baade, C., McGee, K., and Smith, M.R. (eds) 2019. ‘Special issue on Beyoncé’, Popular Music and Society, 42/1 (Special Issue)Google Scholar
Baker, S. 2013. ‘Teenybop and the extraordinary particularities of mainstream practice’, in Redefining Mainstream Popular Music, ed. Taylor, J., Baker, S. and Bennett, A. (London, Routledge), pp. 1424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. 2013. ‘Music studies and the problems of sound, society and method’, IASPM Journal, 3/2, pp. 1532CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., and Bennett, S. 2018. ‘The production of music and sound: a multidisciplinary critique’, in Critical Approaches to the Production of Music and Sound, ed. Bennett, S. and Bates, E. (New York, Bloomsbury Academic)Google Scholar
Bennett, S., and Bates, E. (eds) 2018. Critical Approaches to the Production of Music and Sound (New York, Bloomsbury Academic)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, S., and Klein, E. (eds) 2016. Perspectives on Popular Music and Sound Recording, IASPM Journal, 6/2. http://www.iaspmjournal.net/index.php/IASPM_Journal/issue/view/61Google Scholar
Berry, W. 1987. Structural Functions in Music, 2nd edn (Englewood Cliffs, NJ, Prentice-Hall)Google Scholar
Beveridge, S., and Knox, D. 2018. ‘Popular music and the role of vocal melody in perceived emotion’, Psychology of Music, 46/3, pp. 411–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bielefeldt, C., Dahmen, U., and Großmann, R. 2008. ‘Einleitung’, in Popmusicology. Perspektiven der Popmusikwissenschaft, ed. Bielefeldt, C., Dahmen, U. and Großmann, R. (Bielefeld, transcript), pp. 716CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonz, J. 2015. Alltagsklänge – Einsätze einer Kulturanthropologie des Hörens (Wiesbaden, Springer)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradby, B. 2017. ‘Growing up to be a rapper? Justin Bieber's duet with Ludacris’, in The Routledge Research Companion to Popular Music and Gender, ed. Hawkins, S. (London, Routledge), pp. 3252Google Scholar
Brockhaus, I. 2017. Kultsounds. Die prägendsten Klänge der Popmusik 1960–2014 (Bielefeld, transcript)Google Scholar
Brøvig-Hanssen, R., and Danielsen, A. 2016. Digital Signatures. The Impact of Digitization on Popular Music Sound (Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, M.J. 2006. Unlocking the Groove: Rhythm, Meter, and Musical Design in Electronic Dance Music (Indiana, Indiana University Press)Google Scholar
Capuzzo, G. 2009. ‘Sectional tonality and sectional centricity in rock music’, Music Theory Spectrum, 31/1, pp. 157–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Celebrity Studies 2019. ‘Aims and scopes’. https://tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?show=aimsScope&journalCode=rcel20 (accessed 17 August 2019)Google Scholar
Christenson, P., de Haan-Rietdijk, S., Roberts, D. and ter Bogt, T. 2018. ‘What has America been singing about? Trends in themes in the U.S. top-40 songs: 1960–2010’, Psychology of Music, 47/2, pp.194212Google Scholar
Danielsen, A. 2006. Presence and Pleasure - the Funk Grooves of James Brown and Parliament (Middleton, CT, Wesleyan University Press)Google Scholar
Danielsen, A. 2017. ‘Music, media and technological creativity in the digital age’, Nordic Research in Music Education, Yearbook 18, pp. 922Google Scholar
de Clercq, T. 2016. ‘Review of Ralf von Appen, André Doehring, Dietrich Helms, and Allan Moore, eds Song Interpretation in 21st-Century Pop Music (Ashgate, 2015)’, Music Theory Online, 22/1, https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.16.22.1/mto.16.22.1.declercq.htmlGoogle Scholar
de Clercq, T. 2020. ‘Popular music analysis too often neglects the analysis of popular music’, Review article, Popular Music, 39/2, pp. 339–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Clercq, T., and Margulis, E. 2018. ‘A psychological perspective on repetition in popular music’, in Over and Over: Exploring Repetition in Popular Music, ed. Julien, O. and Levaux, C. (New York, Bloomsbury Press), pp. 147–62Google Scholar
de Clercq, T., and Temperley, D. 2011. ‘A corpus analysis of rock harmony’, Popular Music, 30/1, pp. 4770CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhanaraj, R., and Logan, B. 2005. ‘Automatic prediction of hit songs’, in Proceedings of the International Symposium on Music Information Retrieval, London, pp. 488–91Google Scholar
Diaz-Bone, R. 2002. Kulturwelt, Diskurs und Lebensstil. Eine diskurstheoretische Erweiterung der bourdieuschen Distinktionstheorie (Opladen, Leske + Budrich).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Djupvik, M.B. 2014. ‘Welcome to the candy shop! Conflicting representations of black masculinity’, Popular Music, 33/2. doi:10.1017/S0261143014000312Google Scholar
Doehring, A. 2012. ‘Probleme, Aufgaben und Ziele der Analyse Populärer Musik’, in Black Box Pop. Analysen Populärer Musik (Beiträge zur Popularmusikforschung 38), ed. Helms, D., Phleps, T. (Bielefeld, transcript), pp. 2343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doehring, A. 2015. ‘German modern talking vs. Iranian modern talking. Zur Anwendbarkeit der Korpus-Analyse als Mittel des Popmusikverstehens’, in Speaking in Tongues. Pop lokal global, ed. Helms, D. and Phleps, T. (Bielefeld, transcript), pp. 119–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duinker, B. 2019a. ‘Good things come in threes: triplet flow in recent hip-hop music’, Popular Music, 38/3, pp. 423–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duinker, B. 2019b. ‘Plateau loops and hybrid tonics in recent pop music’, Music Theory Online, 25/4. https://www.mtosmt.org/issues/mto.19.25.4/mto.19.25.4.duinker.htmlCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eerola, T., and Vuoskoski, J. 2013. ‘A review of music and emotion studies: approaches, emotion models, and stimuli’, Music Perception, 30/3, pp. 307–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eitan, Z., and Granot, R.Y. 2006. ‘How music moves: musical parameters and listeners’ images of motion’, Music Perception, 23/3, pp. 221–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, W. 2000. ‘Confessions form Blueberry Hell, or, pitch can be a sticky substance’, in Expression in Pop-Rock Music. A Collection of Critical and Analytical Essays, ed. Everett, W. (New York, Garland), pp. 269346Google Scholar
Everett, W. 2012. ‘The representation of meaning in post-millennial rock’, in Black Box Pop. Analysen Populärer Musik (Beiträge zur Popularmusikforschung 38), ed. Helms, D. and Phleps, T. (Bielefeld, transcript), pp. 149–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fabbri, F. 1982. ‘A theory of musical genres: two applications’, in Popular Music Perspectives, ed. Horn, D. and Tagg, P. (Göteborg/Exeter, IASPM), pp. 5281Google Scholar
Fischer, M., Hörner, F., and Jost, C. (eds) 2019. Songlexikon. Encyclopedia of Songs. http://www.songlexikon.deGoogle Scholar
Forrest, D. 2017. ‘PL voice leading and the uncanny in pop music’, Music Theory Online, 23/4, https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.17.23.4/mto.17.23.4.forrest.htmlCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, S. 1998. Performing Rites: On the Value of Popular Music (Oxford, Oxford University Press)Google Scholar
Geary, D. 2019. ‘Analyzing drums and other beats in twenty-first century popular music’, PhD thesis (Indiana University)Google Scholar
George, D., Stickle, K., Rachid, F., and Wopnford, A. 2007. ‘The association between types of music enjoyed and cognitive, behavioral, and personality factors of those who listen’, Psychomusicology, 19, pp. 3256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerischer, C. 2010. ‘Groove – magische Momente – Versuch einer rationalen Annäherung’, PopScriptum 11– The Groove Issue. https://www2.hu-berlin.de/fpm/popscrip/themen/pst11/pst11_gerischer.pdfGoogle Scholar
Gillet, C. 1970. The Sound of the City: The Rise of Rock and Roll (New York: Outerbridge & Dienstfrey)Google Scholar
Godøy, R., Song, M., Nymoen, K., Haugen, M., and Jensenius, A. 2016. ‘Exploring sound-motion similarity in musical experience’, Journal of New Music Research, 45/3, pp. 210–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E. 1977. Rahmen-Analyse. Ein Versuch über die Organisation von Alltagserfahrungen (Frankfurt/M., Suhrkamp).Google Scholar
Greasley, A., and Lamont, A. 2011. ‘Exploring engagement with music in everyday life using experience sampling methodology’, Musicæ Scientiæ, 15, pp. 4571CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greb, F., Schlotz, W., and Steffens, J. 2018. ‘Personal and situational influences on the functions of music listening’, Psychology of Music, 46/6, pp. 763–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, K.A. 2018. ‘Fashioning a post boy band masculinity: on the seductive dreamscape of Zayn's Pillowtalk’, Popular Music and Society, 41/2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, K.A., and Hawkins, S. 2018. ‘Azealia Banks: “Chasing Time”, erotics, and body politics’, Popular Music, 37/2, pp. 15774CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebdige, D. 1979. Subculture: The Meaning of Style (London, Methuen)Google Scholar
Helms, D. 2015. ‘Pragmatic “Poker Face”: Lady Gaga's song and Roman Jakobson's six functions of communication’, in Song Interpretation in 21st-Century Pop Music, ed. von Appen, R., Doehring, A., Helms, D. and Moore, A.F. (Farnham, Ashgate), pp. 7396Google Scholar
Herbert, R., and Dibben, N. 2018. ‘Making sense of music: meanings 10- to 18-year-olds attach to experimenter-selected musical materials’, Psychology of Music, 46/3, pp. 375–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hesmondhalgh, D., and Negus, K. 2002. ‘Introduction’, in Popular Music Studies, ed. Hesmondhalgh, D. and Negus, K. (London, Arnold), pp. 110Google Scholar
Holert, T., and Terkessidis, M. 1996. Mainstream der Minderheiten. Pop in der Kontrollgesellschaft (Berlin-Amsterdam, Edition ID-Archiv)Google Scholar
Huber, A. 2013. ‘Mainstream as metaphor: Imagining dominant culture’, in Redefining Mainstream Popular Music, ed. Taylor, J., Baker, S. and Bennett, A. (London, Routledge), pp. 313Google Scholar
Huron, D. 2006. Sweet Anticipation. Music and the Psychology of Expectation (Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huron, D. 2008. ‘A comparison of average pitch height and interval size in major- and minor-key themes: Evidence consistent with affect-related pitch prosody’, Empirical Musicology Review, 3/2, pp. 5963Google Scholar
Huegel, H.O. 2007. Lob des Mainstreams. Zu Begriff und Geschichte von Unterhaltung und populärer Kultur (Köln, Herbert von Halem Verlag)Google Scholar
Interiano, M., Kazemi, K., Wang, L., Yang, J., Yu, Z., and Komarova, N.L. 2018. ‘Musical trends and predictability of success in contemporary songs in and out of the top charts’, Royal Society, Open Science, 5, 171274CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iyer, V. 2002. ‘Embodied mind, situated cognition, and expressive microtiming in African-American music’, Music Perception, 19/3, pp. 387414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakubowski, K., Stewart, L., Finkel, S., and Müllensiefen, D. 2017. ‘Dissecting an earworm: melodic features and song popularity predict involuntary musical imagery’, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, pp. 122–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jamdar, A., Abraham, J., Khanna, K., and Dubey, R. 2015. ‘Emotion analysis of songs based on lyrical and audio features’, International Journal of Artificial Intelligence & Applications, 6/3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, R. 2008. ‘”Robo-Diva R&B”: aesthetics, politics, and black female robots in contemporary popular music’, Journal of Popular Music Studies, 20/4, pp. 402–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, R. 2015. Resilience & Melancholy. Pop Music, Feminism, Neoliberalism (Alresford, Zero Books)Google Scholar
Jost, C. 2016. ‘Musikalischer Mainstream. Aufgaben, Konzepte und Methoden zu seiner Erforschung’, POP. Kultur und Kritik, 8, pp. 152–72Google Scholar
Jost, C. 2019. ‘This sounds too mainstream! Locating record production and sonic experience in culturally dominant practices of popular music’, presentation held at the conference Crosstown Traffic: Popular Music Theory and Practice, University of Huddersfield, 3–5 September 2018. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=t_N-6qzTco4 (accessed 29 September 2020)Google Scholar
Julien, O., and Levaux, C. (eds) 2018. Over and Over: Exploring Repetition in Popular Music (New York, Bloomsbury Press)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juslin, P., and Laukka, P. 2003. ‘Communication of emotions in vocal expression and music performance: Different channels, same code?’, Psychological Bulletin, 129/5, pp. 770814CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juslin, P., Liljeström, S., Västfjäll, D., and Lundqvist, L.-O. 2010. ‘How does music evoke emotions? Exploring the underlying mechanisms’, in Handbook of Music and Emotion: Theory, Research, Applications, ed. Juslin, P. and Sloboda, J. (Oxford, Oxford University Press), pp. 605–42Google Scholar
Kaneshiro, B., Ruan, F., Baker, C.W., and Berger, J. 2017. ‘Characterizing listener engagement with popular songs using large-scale music discovery data’, Frontiers in Psychology, 8/416, pp. 115, https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00416/pdfCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keunert, G. 2015. Alternative Mainstream: Making Choices in Pop Music (Valiz, Antennae)Google Scholar
Klingmann, H. 2010. Groove – Kultur – Unterricht. Studien zur Erschließung einer musikkulturellen Praktik (Bielefeld, transcript)Google Scholar
Kozak, M. 2015. ‘Listeners’ bodies in music analysis: gestures, motor intentionality, and models’, Music Theory Online, 21/3. https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.15.21.3/mto.15.21.3.kozak.htmlCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramarz, V. 2006. Die Pop Formeln. Die Harmoniemodelle der Hitproduzenten (Bonn, Voggenreiter Verlag)Google Scholar
Krause, A., and North, A. 2020. ‘Collaboration, cognitive effort, and self-reference in United Kingdom top 5 pop music lyrics 1960–2015’, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 14/3, pp. 294300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krause, A., North, A., and Hewitt, L. 2015. ‘Music-listening in everyday life: devices and choice’, Psychology of Music, 43/3, pp. 155–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krumhansl, C.L. 2017. ‘Listening niches across a century of popular music’, Music. Frontiers of Psychology, 8/431, pp. 118, https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00431/pdfGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liikkanen, L., and Jakubowski, K. 2020. ‘Involuntary musical imagery as a component of ordinary music cognition: a review of empirical evidence’, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 27, pp. 1195–217CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loy, S., Rickwood, J., and Bennett, S. 2018. ‘Popular music, stars and stardom: definitions, discourses, interpretations’, in Popular Music, Stars and Stardom, ed. Loy, S., Rickwood, J. and Bennett, S. (Canberra, Australian National University Press), pp. 120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, L. 2011. ‘The sociology of popular music, interdisciplinarity and aesthetic autonomy’, The British Journal of Sociology, 62/1, pp. 154–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, W.O. 2017. Tuning in Situ: Articulations of Voice, Affect, and Artifact in the Recording Studio, PhD thesis (Ithaca, NY, University of Cornell)Google Scholar
Martel, F. 2011. Mainstream. Wie funktioniert, was allen gefällt (München, Albrecht Knaus Verlag, transl. by Ranke, E. and Schäfer, U.)Google Scholar
Mauch, M., MacCallum, R.M, Levy, M., and Leroi, A.M. 2015. ‘The evolution of popular music: USA 1960–2010’, Royal Society, Open Science, 2: 150081, pp. 110, https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/pdf/10.1098/rsos.150081Google ScholarPubMed
Mazierska, E., Gillon, L., and Rigg, T. 2018. Popular Music in the Post-digital Age. Politics, Economy, Culture and Technology (New York, Bloomsburg Academic)Google Scholar
Mendivil, J. 2019. ‘Über Musikethnologie und Popularmusikforschung’, in Beiträge zur Jahrestagung der Gesellschaft für Musikforschung in Kassel 2017: Das Populäre in der Musik und das Musikverlagswesen, ed. van Dyck-Hemming, A. and Hemming, J. (Wiesbaden, Springer), pp. 117–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menrath, S. 2019. Anonymity Performance in Electronic Pop Music (Bielefeld, transcript)Google Scholar
Middleton, R. 1999. ‘Form’, in Key Terms in Popular Music and Culture, ed. Horner, B. and Swiss, T. (Malden, MA, Blackwell), pp. 141–55Google Scholar
Middleton, R. 2000. ‘Musical belongings. Western music and its low-other’, in Western Music and its Others: Difference, Representation, and Appropriation in Music, ed. Born, G. and Hesmondhalgh, D. (Berkeley, CA, University of California Press), pp. 5985Google Scholar
Moore, A.F. 2012. Song Means: Analysing and Interpreting Recorded Popular Song (Farnham, Ashgate)Google Scholar
Morris, C. 2017. ‘Are pop lyrics getting more repetitive?’, The Pudding. https://pudding.cool/2017/05/song-repetition (accessed 29 April 2019)Google Scholar
Mueller, L.J. 2018. Sound und Sexismus - Geschlecht im Klang populärer Musik. Eine feministisch- musiktheoretische Annäherung (Hamburg, Marta Press)Google Scholar
North, A., Krause, A., Sheridan, L., and Ritchie, D. 2018. ‘Energy, popularity, and the circumplex: a computerized analysis of emotion in 143,353 musical pieces’, Empirical Studies of the Arts, 36/2, pp. 127–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
North, A., Krause, A., Sheridan, L., and Ritchie, D. 2019a. ‘Comparison of popular music in the United States and the United Kingdom: computerized analysis of 42,714 pieces’, Psychology of Music, 48/6, pp. 846–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
North, A., Krause, A., Sheridan, L., and Ritchie, D. 2019b. ‘Popularity, mood, energy, and typicality in music: a computerised analysis of 204,506 pieces’, Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 13, pp. 89109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
North, A., Krause, A., and Ritchie, D. 2021. ‘The relationship between pop music and lyrics: A computerized content analysis of the United Kingdom's weekly top five singles, 1999–2013’, Psychology of Music, 49/4, pp. 735758CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunes, J., and Ordanini, A. 2014. ‘I like the way it sounds: the influence of instrumentation on a pop song's place in the charts’, Musicae Scientiae, 18/4, pp. 392409CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nunes, J., Ordanini, A., and Valsesia, F. 2015. ‘The power of repetition: repetitive lyrics in a song increase processing fluency and drive market success’, Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25/2, pp. 187–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pachet, F. 2012. ‘Hit song science’, in Music Data Mining, ed. Tao, L., Ogihara, M. and Tzanetakis, G. (Boca Raton, FL, CRC Press), pp. 305–26Google Scholar
Pachet, F., and Roy, P. 2008. ‘Hit song science is not yet a science’, in Proceedings of the International Symposium on Music Information Retrieval, Philadelphia, pp. 355–60Google Scholar
Parker, M. 1991. ‘Reading the charts – making sense with the hit parade’. Popular Music, 10/2, pp. 205-17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parzer, M. 2011. Der gute Musikgeschmack. Zur sozialen Praxis ästhetischer Bewertung in der Popularkultur (Frankfurt am Main, Peter Lang)Google Scholar
Peres, A. 2016. ‘The sonic dimension as dramatic driver in 21st-century pop music’, PhD thesis (University of Michigan)Google Scholar
Pfleiderer, M. 2010. ‘Dimensionen der Groove-Erfahrung. Eine empirische Studie’, PopScriptum 11 – The Groove Issue. https://www2.hu-berlin.de/fpm/popscrip/themen/pst11/pst11_pfleiderer.pdf (accessed 7 September 2014)Google Scholar
Post, O., and Huron, D. 2009. ‘Music in minor modes is slower (except in the Romantic period)’, Empirical Musicology Review, 4/1, pp. 119CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pressing, J. 2002. ‘Black Atlantic rhythm: its computational and transcultural foundations’, Music Perception, 19/3, pp. 285310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruett, D. 2011. ‘When the tribe goes triple platinum: a case study toward an ethnomusicology of mainstream popular music in the U.S.’, Ethnomusicology, 55/1, pp. 130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Regev, M. 2013. Pop-Rock Music. Aesthetic Cosmopolitanism in Late Modernity (Cambridge, Polity Press)Google Scholar
Rentfrow, P.J. 2012. ‘The role of music in everyday life: current directions in the social psychology of music’, Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6/5, pp. 402–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, S. 2018. ‘How auto-tune revolutionized the sound of popular music’, Pitchfork, 17 September 2018. https://pitchfork.com/features/article/how-auto-tune-revolutionized-the-sound-of-popular-music (accessed 29 April 2019)Google Scholar
Richards, M. 2017. ‘Tonal ambiguity in popular music's axis progressions’, Music Theory Online, 23/3. https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.17.23.3/mto.17.23.3.richards.htmlCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riesman, D. 1950, ‘Listening to popular music’, American Quarterly, 2/4, pp. 359–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schedl, M. 2016. ‘The LFM-1b Dataset for Music Retrieval and Recommendation’, ICMR ’16: Proceedings of the 2016 ACM on International Conference on Multimedia Retrieval, New York, pp. 103–10Google Scholar
Schellenberg, E., and von Scheve, C. 2012. ‘Emotional cues in American popular music: five decades of the Top 40’, Psychology of Aesthetic, Creativity, and the Arts, 6/3, pp. 196203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholz, H. 2014. Harmonik im musikalischen Mainstream der Jahre 2001 bis 2010. Untersuchungen zu den Top-10-US-R'n’B-/HipHop-Songs mit Sprechgesang (Berlin, LIT Verlag)Google Scholar
Schubert, E. 2013. ‘Emotion in popular music: a psychological perspective’, Volume ! [Online], 10/1. https://journals.openedition.org/volume/3626Google Scholar
Schulze, H. (ed.) 2015. Sound Studies: Traditionen – Methoden – Desiderate. Eine Einführung (Bielefeld, transcript)Google Scholar
Scotto, C., Smith, K.M., and Brackett, J. (eds) 2018. The Routledge Companion to Popular Music Analysis (London, Routledge)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serrà, J., Corral, Á, Boguña, M., Haro, N., and Arcos, J. 2012. ‘Measuring the evolution of contemporary Western popular music’, Scientific Reports 2/521 pp. 1–6, https://www.nature.com/articles/srep00521.pdfCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shepherd, J. 2003. ‘Form’, in Continuum Encyclopedia of Popular Music of the World, Vol. 2: Performance and Production, ed. Horn, D. and Shepherd, J. (New York, Continuum), pp. 503–20.Google Scholar
Singh, K., and Tracy, D. 2015. ‘Assuming niceness: private and public relationships in Drake's Nothing Was the Same’, Popular Music, 34/1, pp. 94112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloan, N., and Harding, C. 2019. ‘About’, Switched On Pop. https://www.switchedonpop.com/about (accessed 29 April 2019)Google Scholar
Sloan, N., and Harding, C. 2020. Switched on Pop: How Popular Music Works, and Why It Matters (New York, Oxford University Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, Y., Dixon, S., and Pearce, M. 2012. ‘Evaluation of musical features for emotion classification’, Proceedings of the 13th International Society for Music Information Retrieval Conference (ISMIR) PortoGoogle Scholar
Spicer, M. 2017. ‘Fragile, emergent, and absent tonics in pop and rock songs’, Music Theory Online, 23/2. https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.17.23.2/mto.17.23.2.spicer.htmlCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinbrecher, B. 2016. Das Klanggeschehen in populärer Musik. Perspektiven einer systematischen Analyse und Interpretation (Wien, Böhlau)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinbrecher, B. 2021. ‘Musical nuances and the aesthetic experience of popular music hooks: theoretical considerations and analytical approaches’, El Oído Pensante, 9/1, pp. 111–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinbrecher, B. forthcoming. ‘”Austrian Popular Music Studies”. A critical assessment’, MusicologicaGoogle Scholar
Steinbrecher, B., and Pichler, M. 2021. ‘”That's the kind of music you hear on the subway without headphones!” Musical evaluation practices of adolescents in Vienna’, Popular Music and Society, pp. 131 doi:10.1080/03007766.2021.1925073Google Scholar
Sterbenz, M. 2018. ‘Ambiguity in “APES**T”’, Journal of Popular Music Studies, 30/4, pp. 1921Google Scholar
Tagg, P. 1987. ‘Musicology and the Semiotics of Popular Music’, Semiotica, 66/1–3, pp. 259–78Google Scholar
Taruffi, L. 2016. ‘Why we listen to sad music: effects on emotion and cognition’, PhD thesis (FU Berlin)Google Scholar
Taylor, J., Baker, S., and Bennett, A. 2013. ‘Preface’, in Redefining Mainstream Popular Music, ed. Taylor, J., Baker, S. and Bennett, A. (London, Routledge), pp. viiixivGoogle Scholar
ter Bogt, T., Delsing, M., van Zalk, M., Christenson, P., and Meeus, W. 2011. ‘Intergenerational continuity of taste: parental and adolescent music preferences’, Social Forces, 90/1, pp. 297319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, S. 1995. Club Cultures: Music, Media and Subcultural Capital (Oxford, Polity Press)Google Scholar
Toynbee, J. 2002. ‘Mainstreaming, from hegemonic centre to global networks’, in Popular Music Studies, ed. Hesmondhalgh, D. and Negus, K. (London, Hodder Arnold), pp. 149–63.Google Scholar
Traut, D. 2005. ‘”Simply irresistible”. Recurring accent patterns as hooks in mainstream 1980s music’, Popular Music, 24/1, pp. 5777CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, B., and Huron, D. 2008. A comparison of dynamics in major and minor key works. Empirical Musicology Review, 3/2, pp. 64–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vella, E.J., and Mills, G. 2017. ‘Personality, uses of music, and music preference: The influence of openness to experience and extraversion’, Psychology of Music, 45/3, pp. 338–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vernallis, C. et al. 2018. ‘Apes**t’, Journal of Popular Music Studies, 30/4, pp. 1170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Appen, R. 2012: ‘Populäre Musik als Herausforderung für die Musikpsychologie. Eine kritische Bilanz’, in Musikpsychologie – Populäre Musik, Band 22, ed. Auhagen, W., Bullerjahn, C. and Höge, H. (Goettingen, Hogrefe), pp. 726Google Scholar
von Appen, R. 2015: ‘Ear candy: what makes Ke$ha's “Tik Tok” tick?’, in Song Interpretation in 21st-Century Pop Music, ed. von Appen, R., Doehring, A., Helms, D. and Moore, A.F. (Farnham, Ashgate), pp. 2952Google Scholar
von Appen, R. 2017. ‘A re-encounter with the Scorpions’ “Wind Of Change”. Why i couldn't stand it then – and what i learn from analysing it now’, in Perspectives on German Popular Music, ed. by Ahlers, M. and Jacke, C. (London, Routledge), pp. 8893Google Scholar
von Appen, R., Doehring, A., Helms, D., and Moore, A.F. 2015a. ‘Introduction’, in Song Interpretation in 21st-century Pop Music, ed. von Appen, R., Doehring, A., Helms, D. and Moore, A.F. (Farnham, Ashgate), pp. 18Google Scholar
von Appen, R., Doehring, A., Helms, D., and Moore, A.F. (eds) 2015b. Song Interpretation in 21st-Century Pop Music (Farnham, Ashgate)Google Scholar
von Appen, R., and Frei-Hauenschild, M. 2018. ‘Theorising New Song Forms in Current Pop Mainstream (Part 1 & 2)’, paper presented to Future Present. Current Practices in Pop Music Studies, Uppsala, 19–20 June, viewed 20 June 2018, https://www.musik.uu.se/digitalAssets/681/c_681872-l_1-k_pop-conference-program.pdf (accessed 30 September 2020)Google Scholar
Wald, E. 2014. ‘Forbidden sounds: exploring the silences of music history’, in Geschichte wird gemacht. Zur Historiographie populärer Musik (= Beiträge zur Popularmusikforschung 40), ed. Helms, D. and Phleps, T. (Bielefeld, transcript), pp. 2539CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstein, G. 2016. ‘Air flows: breath, voice, and authenticity in three recordings’, IASPM Journal, 6/12 pp. 117–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisbard, E. 2014. Top 40 Democracy: The Rival Mainstreams of American Music (Chicago, IL, University of Chicago Press)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welch, N. 2015. ‘”All that noise, and all that sound.” Tonal ambiguity and melodic-harmonic disconnect in the music of Coldplay’, Master's thesis (Youngstown State University)Google Scholar
Wicke, P. 2016. ‘Populäre Musik’, MGG Online. https://www.mgg-online.com/mgg/stable/11557 (last modified November 2016)Google Scholar
Zagorski-Thomas, , 2015. ‘An analysis of space, gesture and interaction in Kings of Leon's “Sex On Fire”‘, in Song Interpretation in 21st-century Pop Music, ed. von Appen, R., Doehring, A., Helms, D. and Moore, A.F. (Farnham, Ashgate), pp. 115–32Google Scholar