Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T15:37:49.887Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Women Leaders and Policy Compliance during a Public Health Crisis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 August 2020

Nichole M. Bauer
Affiliation:
Louisiana State University
Jeong Hyun Kim
Affiliation:
Louisiana State University
Yesola Kweon
Affiliation:
Utah State University
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

How does the gender of a political leader affect policy compliance of the public during a public health crisis? State and national leaders have taken a variety of policy measures to combat the COVID-19 pandemic, with varying levels of success. While many female leaders have been credited with containing the spread of COVID-19, often through implementing strict policy measures, there is little understanding of how individuals respond to public health policy recommendations made by female and male leaders. This article investigates whether citizens are more willing to comply with strict policy recommendations about a public health issue when those recommendations are made by a female leader rather than a male leader. Using a survey experiment with American citizens, we compare individuals’ willingness to comply with policy along three dimensions: social distancing, face covering, and contact tracing. Our findings show that a leader's gender has little impact on policy compliance in general during the pandemic. These findings carry important implications for successful crisis management as well as understanding how a crisis in a nonmasculine issue context influences the effectiveness of a leader's ability to implement measures to mitigate the crisis.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Women and Politics Research Section of theAmerican Political Science Association

How does the gender of a political leader affect policy compliance of the public during a public health crisis? Prior research suggests that voters prefer strong and aggressive leaders during crises (Gadarian Reference Gadarian2010). While most studies focus on the effects of a leader's gender during crises that fit into “masculine” issue areas such as national security (Gadarian Reference Gadarian2010; Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2016; Simas Reference Simas2020), there is little understanding of how crises in “feminine” issue areas such as public health affect evaluations of leaders. This knowledge gap affects our understanding of how individuals respond to female and male leaders working to mitigate a crisis. The results of this study are particularly important as a number of news outlets note that women lead the countries that have been most successful at limiting the spread of COVID-19 (Anderson and Auxier Reference Anderson and Auxier2020; Henley and Roy Reference Henley and Roy2020).

Using a survey experiment of Americans, we compare individuals’ willingness to comply with the COVID-19 policy recommendations regarding social distancing, face coverings, and contact tracing made by female leaders relative to those by male leaders. We examine how policy compliance varies based on the gender of the leader as well as the leader's partisanship.

Our findings show that a leader's gender has little impact on policy compliance in general during the pandemic. As the policy recommendations become more personally invasive, such as engaging in contact tracing, compliance weakly increases when the recommendation comes from a copartisan female leader, while a message from a copartisan male leader has no effect. Our findings that voters will comply with female leadership during a health crisis stand in contrast to previous findings showing that voters have more favorable attitudes toward strong male leadership during a crisis.

GENDERED LEADERSHIP AND POLICY COMPLIANCE DURING A PUBLIC HEALTH CRISIS

In times of crisis, threatened individuals seek out strong, charismatic, and hawkish leadership (Gadarian Reference Gadarian2010; Merolla, Ramos, and Zechmeister Reference Merolla, Ramos and Zechmeister2007). This tendency implies that voters are less likely to view women as competent at dealing with a national security crisis compared with men (Holman et al. Reference Holman, Merolla, Zechmeister and Wang2019), and thus they more likely to comply with policy guidelines from male leaders.Footnote 1

While the literature mostly focuses on crises in masculine issue areas, public health issues are typically considered to be feminine issue areas (Kaufman and Petrocik Reference Kaufman and Petrocik1999; Schneider Reference Schneider2014). For typical women's issues, voters tend to rate female politicians’ competency higher than male politicians (Herrnson, Lay, and Stokes Reference Herrnson, Lay and Stokes2003; Huddy and Terkildsen Reference Huddy and Terkildsen1993). A key difference between the crisis spurred by the pandemic and other crises, such as a national security crisis, is that the former calls for leaders to display feminine traits, such as compassion, rather than masculine traits, such as strength (Johnson and Williams, Reference Johnson and Williamsforthcoming).

Perceived competency of a female leader during the current pandemic could enhance the credibility of policy recommendations made by a female leader. This leads to an expectation that citizens’ willingness to comply with the policy measures that most public health experts say are critical to limiting the spread of the virus will increase when they are made by a female leader relative to a male leader.

However, there is ample evidence that voters are more likely to exhibit gender-motivated biases toward female political leaders when they belong to the other political party (Bauer Reference Bauer2017; Ditonto Reference Ditonto2017; Krupnikov and Bauer Reference Krupnikov and Bauer2014). Individuals may be most resistant to following the policy guidelines of female leaders when those leaders belong to the opposing political party.

RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHODS

Our experiment used a 2 × 2 × 2 design in which we vary (1) the governor's gender (female/male), (2) policy recommendations (strong/no recommendations), and (3) relative partisanship (in-partisan/out-partisan). We conducted our study through Lucid, a survey sampling company that used a national convenience sample of adults in the United States, with a total size of N = 811. We include descriptive data on our sample in Table A1 in Appendix C in the supplementary materials online and more detailed explanations of the survey experiment in Appendix A. A total of 697 respondents (86%) passed the manipulation check.Footnote 2 See Appendix B2 for the manipulation question wording. The balance test shows that the data are well balanced across key individual attributes (Appendix D).

The treatment used a press release explaining that the governor was moving to Phase 2 of reopening the economy during the pandemic. In the press release, the governor makes a strong policy recommendation that residents wear face coverings and social distance in public and says that the state will implement a contact tracing program. The second press release includes no policy recommendation from the governor. We framed the press release around the process of reopening the economy and included no explicit information about the spread of COVID-19 infection in that particular state. Including the no policy recommendation is theoretically important because it lets us gauge people's willingness to participate in mitigation efforts without direct guidance from a political official. We developed the treatment by reviewing the press releases from governors, at the time we developed the treatment in early June 2020, in states such as California, Louisiana, Michigan, and Ohio. See Appendix B1 for the full text of the treatment.

The key outcome variable, compliance willingness, is measured as respondents’ willingness to follow three policy measures with varying levels of restrictiveness on a 4-point scale. While the use of a hypothetical governor in a state where the respondent does not live could limit our ability to make inferences about people's actual behaviors, we prompt individuals to think about their own willingness to wear a face covering, social distance, and participate in contact tracing in response to the treatment they read; this is a standard protocol and design in survey experiments (Mutz Reference Mutz2011). See Appendix B3 for the outcome questions. For our empirical strategy, we use one-sided independent samples t-tests examining differences in compliance willingness among four groups.

FINDINGS

We compare policy compliance across the strict policy treatment condition first among in-partisans and then among out-partisans. Then, we conduct comparisons separately for the female governor and the male governor from the no policy condition to the strict policy condition for the in-partisans and for the out-partisans. Together, these sets of comparisons offer the most rigorous tests for whether citizens are willing to listen to female leaders or reject their advice during a major national crisis.

Figure 1 displays the mean differences in compliance willingness between female and male leaders who issue strict recommendations along with 95% confidence intervals. We display the results separately by in-partisan (a) and out-partisan groups (b). The higher value indicates more responsiveness to a female governor's recommendation.

Figure 1. Gender difference in responsiveness to strict policy recommendations. In each panel, solid lines with error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

The results suggest that there are no statistically significant gender differences in responsiveness to wearing a face covering and social distancing between in-partisans and out-partisans. For contact tracing, however, we find that respondents were more responsive to a female governor's recommendation compared with a male governor's. This difference was statistically significant at p < .10.

Although contact tracing plays a vital role in curbing the spread of the virus (Klinkenberg, Fraser, and Heesterbeek Reference Klinkenberg, Fraser and Heesterbeek2006), American citizens are wary of the government's use of tracing tools. In a recent survey in the United States, 48% of the respondents said it is unacceptable for the government to track people through their cell phones (Anderson and Auxier Reference Anderson and Auxier2020). Since voters tend to view female officeholders as more ethical and more principled than male officeholders (Barnes and Beaulieu Reference Barnes and Beaulieu2014; Dolan Reference Dolan2018), a female governor's recommendation to engage in contact tracing could mitigate people's concerns that the government might misuse its tracing tools, promoting willingness to comply with the policy.

Next, we investigate the differences in policy compliance between strict and no policy recommendations by a leader of the same gender and between female and male leadership. This analysis enables us to evaluate (1) how a governor's strong recommendation influences policy compliance willingness and (2) how that effect might differ by the leader's gender.

In Figure 2, we report the differences in means between the strict and no strict policy recommendation conditions, disaggregated by governor gender. Our findings show some limited evidence of gendered effects. As shown in the left panel, strict policy recommendations by a female in-partisan leader have little impact on policy compliance. Respondents who received a strict recommendation from an in-partisan male governor displayed more willingness to maintain social distancing than those who received no recommendation by the same male governor. The effect size was roughly 24% of a standard deviation of the outcome measure. Similarly, a male governor's strict recommendation increased policy compliance with face-covering in public spaces, though the difference was only significant at the p < 0.1 level.

Figure 2. Gendered difference in responsiveness to policy recommendations. In each panel, solid lines with error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

The right panel suggests that a governor's strong policy recommendations do not have a big impact on out-partisan voters’ compliance willingness. One exception was the effect on social distancing: a female governor's strict recommendations increased willingness to maintain social distancing, while a male governor's recommendations did not have the same effect.

IMPLICATIONS

Our results show that citizens express policy compliance similarly toward the recommendations made by a female governor and a male governor during a pandemic. One may be concerned that conducting a study about COVID-19 in the middle of the pandemic makes it possible that preexisting attitudes about wearing a mask or social distancing affected how people responded to our treatments. Nevertheless, we find these results encouraging, especially considering a popular media narrative suggesting more resistance to female governors during the COVID-19 crisis. This study opens up the door for future work on the gendered nature of political leadership during a pandemic, such as whether women receive different types of evaluations on other leadership metrics, such as favorability, compared to men when they put in place policies to mitigate the crisis. Moreover, women may receive more gendered forms of criticism compared to men even if individuals are just as willing to follow mask recommendations. Our study suggests that the majority of citizens are willing to follow the advice and recommendations of female and male leaders.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1743923X20000604

Footnotes

This research was funded through the Remal Das and Lachmi Devi Bhatia Memorial Professorship of the Manship School of Mass Communication, Louisiana State University.

1. Some research suggests that the threat of a security crisis can lead to an increase in preferences for female leaders (Brown, Diekman, and Schneider Reference Brown, Diekman and Schneider2011).

2. The reported analysis focuses on respondents who passed the manipulation check.

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, Monica, and Auxier, Brooke. 2020. “Most Americans Don't Think Cellphone Tracking Will Help Limit COVID-19, Are Divided on Whether It's Acceptable.” Pew Research Center, April 16. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/04/16/most-americans-dont-think-cellphone-tracking-will-help-limit-covid-19-are-divided/-on-whether-its-acceptable/ (accessed August 27, 2020).Google Scholar
Barnes, Tiffany D., and Beaulieu, Emily. 2014. “Gender Stereotypes and Corruption: How Candidates Affect Perceptions of Election Fraud.” Politics & Gender 10 (3): 365–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nichole M. 2017. “The Effects of Counterstereotypic Gender Strategies on Candidate Evaluations.” Political Psychology 38 (2): 279–95.Google Scholar
Brown, Elizabeth R., Diekman, Amanda B., and Schneider, Monica C.. 2011. “A Change Will Do Us Good: Threats Diminish Typical Preferences for Male Leaders.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37 (7): 930–41.Google ScholarPubMed
Ditonto, Tessa. 2017. “A High Bar or a Double Standard? Gender, Competence, and Information in Political Campaigns.” Political Behavior 39 (2): 301–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, Kathy. 2018. Voting for Women: How the Public Evaluates Women Candidates. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gadarian, Shana Kushner. 2010. “Foreign Policy at the Ballot Box: How Citizens Use Foreign Policy to Judge and Choose Candidates.” Journal of Politics 72 (4): 1046–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, John, and Roy, Eleanor Ainge. 2020. “Are Female Leaders More Successful at Managing the Coronavirus Crisis?” The Guardian, April 25. https://www.theguardian.com/world/2020/apr/25/why-do-female-leaders-seem-to-be-more-successful-at-managing-the-coronavirus-crisis (accessed August 27, 2020).Google Scholar
Herrnson, Paul S., Lay, J. Celeste, and Stokes, Atiya Kai. 2003. “Women Running ‘as Women’: Candidate Gender, Campaign Issues, and Voter-Targeting Strategies.” Journal of Politics 65 (1): 244–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holman, Mirya R., Merolla, Jennifer L., and Zechmeister, Elizabeth J.. 2016. “Terrorist Threat, Male Stereotypes, and Candidate Evaluations.” Political Research Quarterly 69 (1): 134–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holman, Mirya R., Merolla, Jennifer L., Zechmeister, Elizabeth J., and Wang, Ding. 2019. “Terrorism, Gender, and the 2016 U.S. Presidential Election.” Electoral Studies 61: 18.Google Scholar
Huddy, Leonie, and Terkildsen, Nayda. 1993. “Gender Stereotypes and the Perception of Male and Female Candidates.” American Journal of Political Science 37 (1): 119–47.Google Scholar
Johnson, Carol, and Williams, Blair. Forthcoming. “Gender and Political Leadership in a Time of COVID.” Politics & Gender. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1743923X2000029X.Google Scholar
Kaufman, Karen M., and Petrocik, John R.. 1999. “The Changing Politics of American Men: Understanding the Sources of the Gender Gap.” American Journal of Political Science 43 (3): 864–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klinkenberg, Don, Fraser, Christophe, and Heesterbeek, Hans. 2006. “The Effectiveness of Contact Tracing in Emerging Epidemics.” PloS one 1(1).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krupnikov, Yanna, and Bauer, Nichole M.. 2014. “The Relationship between Campaign Negativity, Gender, and Campaign Context.” Political Behavior 36 (1): 167–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merolla, Jennifer L., Ramos, Jennifer M., and Zechmeister, Elizabeth J.. 2007. “Crisis, Charisma, and Consequences: Evidence from the 2004 US Presidential Election.” Journal of Politics 69 (1): 3042.Google Scholar
Mutz, Diana C. 2011. Population-Based Survey Experiments. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Schneider, Monica C. 2014. “The Effects of Gender-Bending on Candidate Evaluations.” Journal of Women, Politics & Policy 35 (1): 5677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simas, Elizabeth N. 2020. “But Can She Make America Great Again? Threat, Stability, and Support for Female Candidates in the United States.” Political Behavior. Published online April 3. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-020-09607-4.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Gender difference in responsiveness to strict policy recommendations. In each panel, solid lines with error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Gendered difference in responsiveness to policy recommendations. In each panel, solid lines with error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Supplementary material: PDF

Bauer et al. supplementary material

Online Appendix

Download Bauer et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 3.4 MB