Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-rwnhh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-26T15:15:35.725Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mayor Pete is Smart and Elizabeth Warren is Unlikable? Coverage of Warmth and Competence Traits in the 2020 Democratic Presidential Primary

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 February 2025

Meredith Conroy*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, California State University, San Bernardino, San Bernardino, CA, USA
Erin Cassese
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of Delaware, Newark, DE, USA
Dhrumil Mehta
Affiliation:
Columbia Journalism School, Columbia University; New York City, NY, USA
Ciera Hammond
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
Linda Beail
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Point Loma Nazarene University, San Diego, CA, USA
Al Johri
Affiliation:
Independent Researcher, New York City, NY, USA
Sean Long
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Oregon State University, Beaverton, OR, USA
Dominik Stecula
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, and Department of Communication, Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, USA
*
Corresponding author: Meredith Conroy; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Past work on media coverage of candidates for political office has explored gender differences in quantity, substance, and tone with mixed results depending on the office, race, and context. We draw on the stereotype content model (SCM) to examine gendered patterns of media coverage of candidates on the trait dimensions of warmth and competence in the 2020 U.S. Democratic presidential primary. Combining Natural Language Processing and manual analysis of news, we find that female candidates receive more negative than positive warmth coverage, while male candidates receive more tonally balanced warmth coverage, which suggests that female leaders are penalized on the dimension of warmth. Additionally, white women received more warmth coverage than women of color and women of color receive more competence coverage than white women. The findings suggest news media may portray white women and women of color candidates as lacking gender congruent traits like warmth but may portray white women as possessing role congruent traits like competence.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Women, Gender, and Politics Research Section of the American Political Science Association

Past research on political campaigns in the U.S. has considered gender bias in media coverage, including studies that look at gender differences in the quantity of coverage received, differences in the framing of male and female candidates, and whether coverage reinforces gender stereotypes, traits, and issues (See Van der Pas and Aaldering Reference Van der Pas and Aaldering2020 for a meta-analysis). Evidence of gender bias in media coverage is mixed and varies across political contexts by office level (federal, state; legislative, executive) and election type (general election, primary election). While some studies point to an attenuation of gender bias in media coverage over time in the U.S. (e.g., Hayes and Lawless Reference Hayes and Lawless2016; Lawrence Reference Lawrence and Ridout2018), other recent studies find evidence of ongoing gender bias (e.g., Bauer and Taylor Reference Bauer and Taylor2023; Lay et al. Reference Lay, Holman, Bos, Greenlee, Oxley and Buffett2021). Studies that find less hostile media environments for women over time have largely focused on legislative races; therefore, it is unclear whether reduced gender bias tracks for executive offices (and their primaries), given the paucity of cases to draw on, and the degree to which analysis of Hillary Clinton dominates studies of women who run for the U.S. presidency (Lawrence and Rose Reference Lawrence and Rose2008, Reference Lawrence and Rose2011; Heldman et al. Reference Heldman, Conroy and Ackerman2018; Uscinski and Goren Reference Uscinski and Goren2009; but see Falk Reference Falk2010 for a review of women’s historical presidential bids).

In this study, we look for evidence of gender bias in candidates’ media coverage by applying the stereotype content model (SCM) framework, which argues that people are judged along two dimensions – warmth and competence. We apply this framework to a single election – the 2020 U.S. Democratic Party’s presidential primary. This race was unprecedented among American presidential primaries in terms of the gender and racial diversity of the candidate pool. A U.S. primary race is also optimal for analysis of gender differences in media coverage because party identification is not a useful heuristic in these races, given that all candidates are members of the same party (Hayes Reference Hayes2011). This means that the media must search for frames that do not rely on party differences when covering the contest, and other characteristics like race and gender may rise to the fore (Farnsworth & Lichter Reference Farnsworth, Lichter and Benoit2016). We analyze media coverage of the candidates during the “invisible primary” period of the 2020 Democratic presidential primary race, which refers to the period before voting when candidates compete for endorsements, media attention, and financial support (Cohen et al. Reference Cohen, Karol, Noel and Zaller2008). This time period is when the race is most crowded, because candidates haven’t yet withdrawn from the race, as they do once sequential voting begins. Therefore, it is the point in the campaign where the media is covering the largest and most diverse set of candidates and focuses on their viability and personal character.

We also consider how gender intersects with candidates’ race to jointly shape their coverage during presidential primaries, given the racial diversity of the 2020 Democratic primary. Existing studies of leadership perceptions find white women, white men, women of color, and men of color often face distinct stereotypic profiles on the dimensions of warmth and competence (Rosette et al. Reference Rosette, Koval, Ma and Livingston2016; Livingston et al. Reference Livingston, Rosette and Washington2012; Dupree Reference Dupree2024). Therefore, this study also compares the coverage of white male candidates to male candidates of color, and white female candidates to female candidates of color.

Our analysis finds that mainstream media covered male and female candidates differently in the 2020 Democratic presidential primary. While competence dominated trait coverage for both men and women relative to warmth coverage, women candidates faced significantly more critical warmth coverage than male candidates. But trait coverage also varied across female candidates in meaningful ways. Female candidates of color received significantly more negative competence coverage compared to white female candidates, while both white women and women of color received more negative warmth coverage, on balance. Male candidates of color, however, did not face this coverage deficit, relative to their white male counterparts. We conclude by discussing the potential consequences of gendered trait coverage on perceptions of candidate viability, which takes on new importance in light of Kamala Harris’s ascension to the top of the Democratic presidential ticket in 2024 as the second female nominee, and first women of color nominee, for a major political party in the U.S.

Executive Offices and Gendered Media Coverage

Studies of gender differences in coverage of candidates at the executive level (gubernatorial and presidential races) suggest media coverage tends to emphasize masculine gendered traits and masculine gendered issues by focusing on masculine bona fides or issues stereotypically associated with men, therefore disadvantaging women (e.g., Chang et al. Reference Chang, Brichta, Ahn and de Vight2023; Carlin and Winfrey Reference Carlin and Winfrey2009; Conroy Reference Conroy2015; Duerst-Lahti Reference Duerst-Lahti2008; Heldman et al. Reference Heldman, Carroll and Olson2005; Bystrom et al. Reference Bystrom, Robertson and Banwart2001; Kahn Reference Kahn1994; Gidengil and Everitt Reference Gidengil and Everitt2003). However, given the limited number of women running for president in U.S. history, our understanding of gendered media bias for this office is slim. That said, since 2018 in the U.S., more women have been running for office, including gubernatorial races and presidential primaries, which provides new opportunities to study gender bias in media coverage at the executive level.

Evidence from studies of media coverage of women heads of state in democratic governments outside the U.S., where there are more cases to draw from, such as in Canada, Australia, Latin America, and Germany, shows that women are often covered differently by news media, such as more coverage about their clothing (Thomas et al. Reference Thomas, Harell, Rijkhoff and Gosselin2021). Even when coverage for male and female candidates is similar, it tends to focus on topics that favor men by emphasizing masculine issues and experiences (Wagner et al. Reference Wagner, Trimble, Curtin, Auer and Woodman2022). For an example of the latter point, Gerris et al. (Reference Garrits, Trimble, Wagner, Raphael and Sampert2017) found that three women running for prime minister in Canada were subject to gendered mediation (which is the use of gendered metaphors in campaign coverage) that favored male candidates. For an example of the former point, Trimble (Reference Trimble2017) analyzes media coverage of four female prime ministers across three countries to discuss how coverage delegitimizes female leaders. Taken together, there is good reason to expect that gender biases in media coverage of heads of state observed in non-U.S. countries will emerge in the coverage of 2020 Democratic presidential primary candidates. Heads of state, regardless of the country, are expected to demonstrate leadership that is usually defined by norms traditionally associated with masculinity (Smith Reference Smith2017; Garrits et al. Reference Garrits, Trimble, Wagner, Raphael and Sampert2017). In addition, the U.S. is one of just a few industrialized democracies that has never elected a female head of state (Thames and Williams Reference Thames and Williams2013), and as such, female candidates’ media coverage is likely to differ from that of male candidates. That said, studies of Latin America find the context of the race to matter. For example, where a history of corruption is salient, female candidates can have an advantage in terms of congruency with leadership perceptions, due to the stereotype that women are more honest (Le Foulon and Reyes-Householder Reference Le Foulon and Reyes-Housholder2021; Funk et al. Reference Funk, Hinojosa and Piscopo2021; Lucciola Reference Lucciola2023).

Anecdotal accounts of the 2020 Democratic presidential primary suggest gender bias was present in campaign coverage (see Siddiqui [Reference Siddiqui2019] for overview), and journalists wrote about whether some of their peers’ emphasis on female candidates’ “likability” was sexist (Lerer Reference Lerer2019). Stories like this suggest there is more awareness about the gendered double standards in campaign coverage at the presidential level among some in the press, but that it persists. However, whether gendered coverage broadly permeated the 2020 Democratic primary race is an open empirical question.

In the next section, we review existing approaches to studying media coverage of presidential candidates’ character and traits through a gendered lens and discuss how these approaches apply to contests where men and women are competing against each other. We also elaborate on our approach for categorizing descriptions of presidential character and traits — the stereotype content model (SCM) — and how it applies at the intersection of race and gender.

Gendered Trait Coverage in Political Campaigns

Analyses of media coverage of presidential character and traits have employed numerous frameworks. For instance, some studies have employed Kinder et al.’s (Reference Kinder, Peters, Abelson and Fiske1980) approach, which argues for four basic presidential trait dimensions: leadership, competence, integrity, and compassion (Conroy Reference Conroy2015). There is some overlap between these traits and those explored in research on gender stereotypes. For instance, compassion is a stereotypically feminine trait, whereas competence and leadership are stereotypically associated with masculinity (Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2014). Other studies that have focused on women running for the presidency have looked explicitly at feminine and masculine attributes (Falk Reference Falk2010; Meeks Reference Meeks2013). Each of these frameworks is useful for conceptually consolidating descriptions of presidential character to draw conclusions about gendered trends in media coverage. In this study we apply a traits approach known as the stereotype content model.

In psychology, researchers often employ a two-dimensional framework called the stereotype content model (SCM), which maintains that intergroup attitudes and perceptions of individuals stem from evaluations of warmth and competence traits (Fiske et al. Reference Fiske, Xu, Cuddy and Glick1999, Reference Fiske, Cuddy, Glick and Xu2002; Cuddy et al. Reference Cuddy, Fiske and Glick2008). Some scholarship on voter decision making has utilized this framework, analyzing the relative influence of voters’ warmth and competence ratings of candidates on electoral outcomes, but the results have been mixed (Laustsen and Bor Reference Laustsen and Bor2017; McAllister Reference McAllister2016; McGraw Reference McGraw, Druckman, Green, Kuklinski and Lupia2011), or found that it varies on whether the candidate is an out- or in-partisan (Bor and Laustsen Reference Bor and Laustsen2022). This work has focused on the perceptions of individual voters and has not considered the relative prevalence of these traits in media coverage of male and female presidential primary candidates’ campaigns.

The SCM attributes of warmth and competence also overlap with the agency-communion framework (Abele et al. Reference Abele, Hauke, Peters, Louvet, Aleksandra and Yanping2016), which has been applied in political science research to understand political behaviors, evaluations, and goals (Conroy and Green Reference Conroy and Green2020; Liebenow et al. Reference Liebenow, Boucher and Cassidy2022; Bauer Reference Bauer2024). Like the SCM, the agency-communion framework centers on two higher order traits (agency and communion), which subsume competence (agency) and warmth (communion) (Abele and Wojciszke Reference Abele, Wojciszke, Abele and Wojciszke2018). Ultimately, the SCM is one of several frameworks for explaining social perception in terms of a limited set of trait dimensions that are considered to cover broad (but similar) elements of human perceptions and behaviors. Given the similarity in these frameworks, there has been an effort to consolidate these (and other related frameworks) into a distinction between vertical traits and horizontal traits, with vertical traits including characteristics like agency and competence and horizontal traits reflecting characteristics like warmth and communion. Said differently, vertical traits are oriented around “getting ahead,” whereas horizontal traits focus on “getting along” (Abele et al. Reference Abele, Ellemers, Fiske, Koch and Yzerbyt2021). Given that we developed our dictionaries with a focus on warmth and competence, drawing on prior scholarship on the SCM (especially the SCM dictionary created by Nicolas et al. Reference Nicolas, Bai and Fiske2020), we rely on the SCM framework language for outlining our expectations about trait coverage and discussing our results. However, these trait dimensions are subsumed within the broader integrated agency-communion framework described by Abele et al. (Reference Abele, Ellemers, Fiske, Koch and Yzerbyt2021).

The SCM has been used to explain gender bias against “career women,” broadly finding that career women as a group tend to be stereotyped as competent but not warm, while “housewives” are warm but not competent (Fiske et al. Reference Fiske, Xu, Cuddy and Glick1999). Warmth includes dimensions of friendliness and trustworthiness, while competence includes dimensions of capability and assertiveness. Capability competence traits are related to ability, such as “intelligent,” “effective,” or “smart,” while assertiveness competence traits are related to motivation, such as “ambitious,” and “self-confident.” Friendliness facets of warmth are related to getting along and maintaining social relationships, such as “kind,” and “compassionate,” while trustworthiness facets of warmth are related to morality and social values, such as “honest,” and “fair.”

Although women in the abstract are perceived as warm, there are limitations to this association. First, research on perceptions of professional women and “feminists” found that their stereotype profile diverges from that of “women,” in that they are competent, but not warm. In short, stereotypes about women broadly in the abstract do not overlap with stereotypes about women in positions of power, or working women; moreover, perceptions of the warmth and friendliness of women in positions of power are largely negative (Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2014; Cuddy et al. Reference Cuddy, Fiske and Glick2004; Fiske et al. Reference Fiske, Xu, Cuddy and Glick1999). These patterns also reflect the “double-bind” (Tolley, Lawlor, and Fortier-Chouinard Reference Tolley, Lawlor and Fortier-Chouinard2023). The double bind is the phenomenon where female candidates who conform with stereotypes about what it means to be a “good woman” are characterized as weak and insufficient for politics; on the other hand, if they conform with stereotypes about what it means to be a good political leader, female politicians can be characterized as insufficiently feminine, resulting in negative evaluations either way (Eagly and Karau Reference Eagly and Karau2002; Jamieson Reference Jamieson1995; Ritter and Yoder Reference Ritter and Yoder2004). Additionally, Teele, Kalla, and Rosenbluth (Reference Teele, Kalla and Rosenbluth2018) find that women who fit traditional notions of womanhood (married with children) are perceived as better candidates than women who do not, which they characterize as a double bind because familial roles are a larger hurdle for women than men, when it comes to running for office. These studies are examples of how external groups penalize female candidates when they fail to uphold gender stereotypes, but also when they fail to uphold leadership stereotypes. Political campaigns and political strategists are aware of the double bind (Dittmar Reference Dittmar2015) and encourage female candidates to follow a “balancing strategy,” by which they work to portray the appropriate balance of warmth and competent traits, so as to not alienate voters who subscribe to traditional gender norms, and to avoid priming sexist attitudes about women in power (Bauer Reference Bauer2020; Bauer and Santia Reference Bauer and Santia2022).

Gender also intersects with other social categories, such as race and ethnicity, in ways that moderate the application of gender stereotypic traits linked to warmth and competence by audiences or potential voters (Fiske et al. Reference Fiske, Cuddy, Glick and Xu2002; Rosette et al. Reference Rosette, Koval, Ma and Livingston2016; Gershon and Lavariega Monforti Reference Gershon and Lavariega Monforti2019; Cargile Reference Cargile2023; Dowe Reference Dowe2020; Pao and Rajan Reference Pao, Akhil, Brown and Gershon2023; Bejarano et al. Reference Bejarano, Brown, Gershon and Montoya2021). For instance, compared to women of color, white women are more commonly described in gender stereotypic terms, emphasizing warmth (Coles and Pasek Reference Coles and Pasek2020; Donovan Reference Donovan2011; Purdie-Vaughns and Eibach Reference Purdie-Vaughns and Eibach2008). By contrast, Black and Asian women are often perceived as less warm than white women, leading to descriptions that prioritize agentic qualities linked to competence, such as dominance (for Black women) and expertise (for Asian women) (Galinsky et al. Reference Galinsky, Hall and Cuddy2013; Lin et al. Reference Lin, Kwan, Cheung and Fiske2005). Some studies suggest the more agentic stereotype profile for women of color in leadership roles may be due to their anticipation of greater scrutiny of their qualifications, so they work harder to shore up doubts about their competence by emphasizing experience and credibility (Dittmar Reference Dittmar2015; Garcia Bedolla Reference Bedolla2007; Koenig and Eagly Reference Koenig and Eagly2014). Either way, there is evidence that white women and women of color experience different warmth and competence trait expectations (Bauer and Santia Reference Bauer and Santia2022; Dupree Reference Dupree2024).

Men of color in leadership roles also face stereotypes on the dimensions of warmth and competence that are distinct from white men. Broadly, men of color in leadership roles, like women of color, anticipate heightened scrutiny of their competence (Rosette et al. Reference Rosette, Leonardelli and Phillips2008). Stereotypes that Black men are dominant and aggressive limit perceptions that they are warm, while also being perceived as less competent than white men (Walzer and Czopp Reference Walzer and Czopp2011). Asian and Latino men are often presumed more warm or communal than white men, which can contribute to the belief that they are not assertive or dominant enough for leadership roles (Rosette and Tost Reference Rosette and Tost2013). However, Asian men are often perceived as more competent in certain professions, such as engineering (Sy et al. Reference Sy, Shore, Strauss, Shore, Tram, Whiteley and Ikeda-Muromachi2010).

Given the racial and gender diversity in the 2020 Democratic primary candidate pool for president, and past work suggesting these factors jointly influence candidate evaluations, we also consider the intersection of gender and race when interpreting the analysis of trait coverage in 2020. While grouping all women of color and all men of color together for the analysis is not optimal, for identifying whether white women and white men are distinct from women of color and men of color, this will allow us to observe broad patterns in how race and gender jointly shape candidate characterizations in media. We recognize this approach may mask important racial group differences but adopt it here to capture potential disparities at a high level. As more candidates of color run for president, there should be further disaggregation in analysis at the intersection of race and ethnicity.

In the analysis that follows, we consider the relative balance of warmth and competence coverage during the 2020 invisible primary period for male and female candidates overall, and at the intersection of race. Our goal is to describe the nature of this coverage and determine whether patterns are apparent when taking into consideration candidates’ gender and racial identification. We are working from the perspective that balanced coverage of warmth and competence is preferred for white women and people of color, given prior work establishing the importance of a balancing strategy for candidates who are not white or men, because reception from audiences may be more sensitive to leadership trait deficits for white women and people of color (Bauer Reference Bauer2020).

Hypotheses

Based on existing research that analyzes candidate gender and media coverage, we expect to see that coverage of female candidates stresses different personal traits compared to male candidates along the lines of warmth and competence. Specifically, we expect that overall, female primary candidates’ trait coverage is more focused on warmth than competence, compared with male primary candidates, whose trait coverage will be more focused on competence than warmth. (Hypothesis 1). This pattern of coverage is expected to disadvantage women, because warmth in isolation (e.g., without competence coverage, and therefore “balance”) is incongruent with political leadership, and coverage that invokes warmth, even if positive, can harm perceptions that the candidate is a viable presidential nominee.

Beyond this, we expect that the valence of warmth and competence descriptions to also be gendered. For instance, “likable” would be an example of warm coverage that is positively valenced, whereas “unlikeable” would be an example of warmth coverage that is negatively valenced, or critical. We expect that female primary candidates’ warmth coverage will be more expressly negative in tone than male primary candidates’ warmth coverage. We expect warmth coverage for female primary candidates will more often be invoked to criticize their apparent lack of this trait. Similarly, we expect that female primary candidates’ competence coverage will be more critical than male primary candidates’ competence coverage, raising doubts about women’s qualifications or readiness for office more so than their male counterparts. In short, we hypothesize that the valence of warmth and competence coverage for female candidates is significantly more negative than for male candidates (Hypothesis 2).

Finally, we expect the balance of warmth and competence traits in terms of valence to vary at the intersection of gender and race, given prior work on intersectional stereotyping (for a review, see Cassese Reference Cassese, Redlawsk and Oxley2019). This scholarship suggests that the conventional female stereotype profile of high warmth and low competence applies not to all women but specifically to white women. By contrast, women of color are more commonly stereotyped as low on warmth and high on competence and therefore may receive less negative warmth coverage than white women, due to this lowered expectation of warmth for women of color (Livingston et. al 2012). Therefore, we expect white women to receive more warmth coverage relative to their competence coverage than women of color (Hypothesis 3). There is also some evidence of stereotype inversion for men of color, though this may stem in part from intentional self-presentation strategies aimed at softening group-based stereotypes about violence and aggression (Entman and Rojecki Reference Entman and Rojecki2000; Stephens-Dougan Reference Stephens-Dougan2020). We anticipate men of color will receive more warmth coverage relative to competence coverage, compared with white men (Hypothesis 4).

We also consider the role of trait valence in candidate coverage at the intersection of race and gender. We expect that women of color will receive more positive competence coverage than white women (Hypothesis 5) due to compensating strategies, and stereotypes about women of color in leadership roles (as we noted elsewhere). Expectations regarding warmth for white women and women of color are less clear. White women who run for office violate stereotypic expectations about hypothetical women and warmth, and this expectation is reduced for women of color, who (as we note elsewhere) are associated with a distinctive stereotype profile, where they are perceived as lacking warmth. Therefore, we expect warmth coverage will be on balance more negative than positive for both white women and women of color (Hypothesis 6). For male candidates, we expect men of color will receive more positive warmth trait coverage relative to white men due to compensating strategies by the candidates (Hypothesis 7). Yet, we expect men of color will receive more negative competence coverage than white male candidates (Hypothesis 8) due to the persistence of racial stereotypes that question their qualifications and leadership abilities, as well as implicit biases that associate white men with authority and competence more readily than men of color. In addition to testing these hypotheses, we also present an analysis of the specific warmth and competence traits that are more likely to be used to describe female candidates and male candidates to offer deeper insights into the application of warmth and competence in candidate coverage.

The 2020 Democratic Primary

The 2020 Democratic presidential primary was a highly contested race that featured a large field of viable candidates competing for the nomination to challenge the incumbent President, Donald Trump. Senator Elizabeth Warren of Massachusetts declared her candidacy on February 9, 2019, and Senator Bernie Sanders of Vermont announced ten days later. Former Vice President Joe Biden did not officially announce that he was running until April 2019. Other Democrats who entered the race included Kamala Harris (California Senator), Cory Booker (New Jersey Senator), Amy Klobuchar (Minnesota Senator), Pete Buttigieg (mayor of South Bend, IN), Julian Castro (former Secretary of Housing and Urban Development), Kirsten Gillibrand (New York Senator), Tulsi Gabbard (Hawaii Representative), Michael Bennett (Colorado Senator), Steve Bullock (Montana Governor), Bill de Blasio (New York City mayor), John Hickenlooper (Colorado Senator), and Beto O’Rourke (former Texas Representative). Michael Bloomberg, former New York City mayor, entered the race eventually, too, but not until November 2019. Several candidates without political experience also ran, such as Tom Steyer, Marianne Williamson, and Andrew Yang. All Democratic candidates who appeared at least once on a Democratic primary debate stage are included in this analysis.Footnote 1

One major wedge issue in the primary was support for “Medicare for All,” which was championed most aggressively by Sanders. Issues of racial justice, issues related to the #MeToo movement, and the climate crisis were also top of the Democratic primary agenda. However, most Democratic primary voters prioritized beating Trump over their issue priorities, according to polls (Santhanam Reference Santhanam2019). During the period of our data collection, there were numerous televised debates, and some movement in the polls as a result (Skelley Reference Skelley2019); however, Biden was the most consistent leader in the polls during the invisible primary period. At various moments in the campaign, Warren and Harris were competitive, but Sanders was the most consistently in reach of Biden. Altogether, the field was large, diverse, and competitive enough to warrant dynamic media coverage the year before the first primary contest in Iowa.

Data Collection and Processing

The data for our analysis were collected from the LexisNexis BulkAPI, accessed through the University of Pennsylvania. We considered print articles from January 1, 2019 through January 31, 2020, mentioning a Democratic primary candidate’s name one or more times, from various national and regional sources. Each article is a “document.” The LexisNexis BulkAPI included a broad range of sources, from which we selected articles (or “documents”) from major national newspapers (e.g., The New York Times, The Washington Post), newswires (Associated Press), large online news sites (e.g., Slate, Politico), major tabloids (e.g., The New York Post, Daily News), regional newspapers (e.g., LA Times, Tampa Bay Times), and some international publications that cover American politics (e.g., The Guardian, The Globe and Mail) (see Table A1, Appendix). Our goal was to capture a comprehensive mix of mainstream political news outlets, including a balance of partisan and nonpartisan sources.

Given the polarized U.S. political context during the campaign period covered, we recognize that media coverage could vary widely across outlets, potentially introducing bias into our sample. However, given that all candidates are running as Democrats, we were less concerned that our selection of outlets would favor a particular candidate’s ideology over another candidate’s ideology. We selected sources that mostly skew left or are nonpartisan to reflect the most relevant audience. This approach allows us to capture a representative view of media discourse surrounding the Democratic primary. In Table A1 in the Appendix, we indicate which sources are partisan (and in which ideological direction), according to the Ad Fondes Media Bias Chart.

To analyze warmth and competence coverage of the 2020 Democratic presidential primary candidates, we identify relevant snippets for analysis from all documents using the following preprocessing steps, which we elaborate on below:

  1. 1. Remove duplicate documents

  2. 2. Tokenize documents

  3. 3. Tag adjectives in documents

  4. 4. Carryout dependency parsing

  5. 5. Identify candidates in documents by name

  6. 6. Identify candidates in documents by their pronouns

  7. 7. Resolve adjectives to the candidates within the same documents

For step 1, we removed duplicate articlesFootnote 2 and were left with 34,530 documents. In step 2, the documents are “tokenized” in order to be tagged. In step 3, all adjectives are flagged using spaCy’s part of speech tagging. In step 4, we apply spaCy’s dependency parsing tool.Footnote 3 Dependency parsing identifies the structure of sentences based on dependencies, which are relationships between words. This is a necessary step to link the adjectives to the candidates in the documents. In step 5, we use spaCy’s named entity recognition system to identify the candidates in the articles (e.g., “Bernie Sanders”). In step 6, we use HuggingFace’s NeuralCoref (coreference resolution) to account for references to the candidates by pronouns (e.g., he/she/him/her).Footnote 4 And in step 7, we applied an algorithm to navigate the dependency tree starting with each adjective. If the adjective had a “child node” in the dependency tree that was identified as a noun or pronoun, we identified that noun or pronoun as a referent of the adjective; otherwise, we navigated to the “parent node” of the adjective and ran the algorithm again to search for the nearest “child node” that was a noun or pronoun. If the noun or pronoun that was identified by this algorithm had already been resolved to a candidate (“entity”), we marked that candidate as the referent of the adjective. We refer to the outcome of this process as “referent-adjective pairings.”

Next, we manually checked each referent-adjective pairing to correct any instances where the algorithm erroneously identified the wrong candidate (and corrected it), or to remove referent-adjective pairings that were irrelevant or otherwise inaccurate. In total, 25% were removed due to this process, which left us with 4,735 snippets. Because we rely on the pipeline outlined above to identify snippets containing adjective-referent pairs, we cannot explicitly account for adjective-referent pairings that are missing, but our manual process ensures that those that are included are accurate.

Adjectives in the adjective-referent pairings were then categorized as warmth attributes (positive or negative), competence attributes (positive or negative), or neither, in order to apply the stereotype content model to the candidates’ coverage. For this process, we first classified the traits based on existing dictionaries developed to capture the dimensions of warmth and competence dimensions of the SCM (Nicolas et al Reference Nicolas, Bai and Fiske2020). Two of the authors then independently categorized all the adjectives according to valence, so that each fell within one of the following five categories: positive warmth (e.g., “approachable”), positive competence (e.g., “intelligent”), negative warmth (e.g., “cold”), negative competence (e.g., “inarticulate”), or none of these. For all traits where there was disagreement, the authors met to come to a consensus to determine which category the adjective belongs to. This process produced the list of traits in the Appendix (Table A2).

Together, this process identified all sentences (which we refer to as “snippets”) that mention a warmth or competence trait (positive or negative) that is associated with a candidate, by their name or their pronouns (the adjective-referent pairings). For example, some warmth snippets identified within the media sample by our process include “In our short time watching, she was affable and at ease,” where “she” is Klobuchar, and “affable” is the warmth trait. Another example is, “It’s part of who he is and it’s why people think he’s accessible or approachable,” where “he” is Biden, and “accessible” and “approachable” are warmth attributes. Another example is, “One of the main drivers of Buttigieg’s appeal is how authentic he appears in word and manner,” where “authentic” is a warmth trait describing Buttigieg. Some competence snippets identified in the media sampled by our process include, “She is competitive. She is a fighter,” a description of Gillibrand, and “On the campaign trail, Mr. Castro appears confident,” a description of Castro. Our processes identified 747 positive warmth, 2,255 positive competence, 649 negative warmth, and 1,084 negative competence snippets for a total of 4,735.

After a training session detailing the content analysis process, four of the authors were assigned a subset of all snippets for an interrater reliability assessment. A sample size calculator was used to determine an appropriate sample size (95% confidence with +/- 3% margin of error), yielding a sample of 644 snippets. Given that a snippet could refer to an adjective as a negation (e.g., “Klobuchar lacked warmth”), we assess interrater reliability for referent-adjective pairing matches, and referent-adjective pairing negations. For the former, interrater reliability was .73, and for the latter, it was .78 (Fleiss’s kappa), which meets the conventional standard for “substantial agreement” (Landis and Koch Reference Landis and Koch1977).

The hand-coding team read every snippet to verify that the adjective referenced in a snippet was in fact describing the identified candidate and referred to their personal character. If the adjective in the snippet did not describe the identified candidate (due to error or context), it was removed. Therefore, the analysis does not assume the referent-adjective pairings identified by the automated process are correct; hand-coding affirms the accuracy of the referent-adjective pairings and the referent-adjective descriptions.

Having achieved substantial interrater agreement, the full set of snippets was divided among the team of four experts who then carried out the process of identifying whether the adjective in the snippets applied to the identified candidate, and if so, whether the application was positive (“Biden’s speech was warm”), or negative (“Biden’s speech was not warm”). Any snippets that seemed ambivalent (e.g., “Some people thought Biden’s speech was warm but other people said it was not warm”), were marked as “other” and read by a committee of two researchers to determine if the description fit into the positive or negative categories. Capturing trait valence in this way was necessary to effectively evaluate critical candidate coverage. Not all warmth coverage, for instance, characterized a candidate as warm. Instead, some commented on a candidate’s apparent lack of warmth. As noted above, we anticipated that this phenomenon was related to candidate gender, with women more likely to be characterized as “unlikable” (a negatively valenced warmth trait) compared to men, and by coding valence it allows us to evaluate this possibility. Through the process outlined here, we built a database of candidate media representations through an SCM lens.

Findings

Balance of Warmth and Competence Coverage for Male and Female Candidates

To assess our first hypothesis, we analyze the proportion of warmth and competence traits for all men and all women running in the 2020 Democratic primary in Figure 1. Raw counts are also included for context, because they provide a sense of the volume of coverage on each of the trait dimensions. Notably, trait coverage in 2020 revolves around competence more than warmth. Of the 4,735 trait mentions in our dataset, 70.5% were competence traits. Recall that our first hypothesis laid out an expectation that women would receive coverage that was more focused on warmth and less focused on competence relative to male candidates. Both women and men received less warmth coverage compared to competence coverage. However, the share of the traits associated with warmth as opposed to competence was slightly higher for women compared with men and the difference is statistically significant, (χ2(1) = 7.63, p = .006); therefore Hypothesis 1 is confirmed. This finding might reflect a slight advantage for male candidates, in that coverage of the presidential leadership contest favors a trait that is more often attributed to men. However, to better understand this balance, the tone of that coverage needs to be assessed.

Figure 1. Warmth and competence traits in media coverage of male and female presidential candidates

Our second hypothesis posits that gender differences in trait coverage may manifest in the positive and negative valence of coverage and not merely the relative proportion of warmth and competence traits attributed to the candidates. If women receive more critical warmth and competence coverage, they may face an electoral disadvantage. Figure 2 displays the proportion of positive and negative trait coverage for competence and warmth separately, with negative coverage rendered in black and positive coverage rendered in gray. The results qualify our previous finding in an important way. Though men received slightly but significantly more competence coverage than women, the competence coverage women do receive is, on balance, more positive than competence coverage for men. About 77.2% of competence descriptions ascribed to women were positive while only about 63.7% of those ascribed to men were positive. A chi-squared test indicates that this difference in proportions is statistically significant (χ2(1) = 55.68, p< .001).

Figure 2. Warmth and competence trait coverage by valence and candidate gender

This result runs counter to our Hypothesis 2 expectations about competence coverage and suggests mixed results when it comes to gender and competence traits, given that the slight disadvantage in competence coverage quantity for women may be offset by the more positive valence of this coverage. Existing studies suggest this could be a reflection of supply-side factors related to female candidate emergence; for instance, a qualifications gap in which the women who run for office tend to be more qualified than men who run (Fulton Reference Fulton2012; Fulton & Dhima Reference Fulton and Dhima2021; Anzia and Berry Reference Anzia and Berry2011; Bauer Reference Bauer2020), especially at higher levels such as the presidency. Indeed, a common theme in 2020 coverage was that many of the women on the debate stage had never lost an election.

For warmth descriptions, unlike competence descriptions, the gender differences laid out in Hypothesis 2 do emerge. Over half of the descriptions of female candidates that invoke warmth are negative (53.7%), while descriptions of male candidates that invoke warmth are positive on balance (56.9%). This gender difference is statistically significant (χ2(1) = 13.95, p< .001). This result suggests that female candidates’ warmth is drawn into question or criticized more often and tracks with prior scholarship that finds female leaders and professional women to be perceived as lacking warmth (Cuddy et al. Reference Cuddy, Fiske and Glick2004, Reference Cuddy, Glick and Beinger2011; Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2014).

What are the consequences of differential warmth coverage? Existing studies find a lack of warmth does not diminish male leaders’ appeal or the perception of their capacity for leadership, whereas it does diminish female leaders’ appeal (e.g., Fiske et al. Reference Fiske, Cuddy, Glick and Xu2002; Cuddy et al. Reference Cuddy, Fiske and Glick2008), and therefore the negative warmth portrayals for women may have disproportionately more negative effects on their candidacies. Additionally, our finding that women face more negative warmth coverage is especially interesting given scholarship that finds warmth evaluations to have mental primacy (Wojciszke and Abele Reference Wojciszke and Abele2008), and warmth to be critical to examinations of candidate character and leadership (e.g., Laustsen and Bor Reference Laustsen and Bor2017). This suggests an election environment that reinforces the double bind, where women are expected to uphold existing gender norms, wherein they are warm in scenarios where warmth may be more difficult to project, such as a political contest. For women who do not fit into a “warm” (feminine) mold (such as women running for high political office) the media may be (inadvertently or not) telling a story about how women fall short of upholding trait expectations associated with their gender, while being successful at upholding trait expectations associated with the role, given the competence findings.

Trait Coverage by Candidate Gender and Race

In addition to average gender differences in the type and valence of trait coverage, we also considered whether candidates’ gender intersects with their racial identification to shape coverage, by comparing white candidates to candidates of color. Our third hypothesis laid out an expectation that female candidates of color would receive less warmth coverage, and more competence coverage compared to white female candidates, and our fourth hypothesis indicated that male candidates of color would receive more warmth coverage and less competence coverage than white men. Although stereotypes about Black men suggest they lack warmth, and stereotypes about Asian men suggest they are exceptionally competent, our hypotheses reflect an expectation that white men will have an advantage in election coverage, where competence is typically privileged to warmth. To evaluate these claims, we reported the proportion of warmth and competence traits for each candidate group in Figure 3. Consistent with our expectations, the media coverage of white women and women of color differs significantly in how it emphasizes warmth and competence (χ2(1) = 31.53, p<.001). White women candidates are more likely to receive warmth coverage (36.9%) than women of color (21.2%), while women of color are more likely to receive competence coverage (71.9%) than white women (63.1%). These differences reflect distinct patterns of representation that align with racialized gender stereotypes about women in the abstract. This finding squares with prior research discussed above about a warmth deficit facing women of color that white women in the abstract do not face. Hypothesis 4, however, was not supported. Male candidates of color did receive slightly more warmth relative to competence coverage, compared with white men, but this difference was not statistically significant (χ2(1) = 0.45, p < .4). Collectively, this analysis suggests that race moderates trait coverage for female candidates, but significant differences did not emerge for the male candidates in the 2020 Democratic primary.

Figure 3. Quantity of warmth and competence coverage by candidate gender and race

Trait valence based on candidates’ race and gender identifications is depicted in Figure 4. Here, it’s evident that while women of color received more competence coverage overall than white woman (Figure 3), this coverage was significantly more negative for women of color compared to white women (31.0% negative compared with 18.6% negative) (χ2(1) = 17.87, p = <.001). This finding runs counter the expectations laid out in Hypothesis 5 and suggests competence discussions were not a boon to female candidates of color. In addition to this, we expected white women and women of color candidates would receive more negative than positive warmth coverage, albeit due to different stereotypic profiles (Hypothesis 6). We find that the tone of the warmth coverage didn’t vary significantly by race among women (χ2(1) = .16, p = .69), but was more negative in tone for both white women and women of color, confirming Hypothesis 6.

Figure 4. Valence of warmth and competence coverage by candidate gender and race

Turning to male candidates, the analysis found white men received significantly more negative coverage relative to male candidates of color, both in terms of warmth (χ2(1) = 5.38, p < .01) and competence (χ2(1) = 24.87, p < .001). This comports with our expectation regarding warmth traits (Hypothesis 7), based on prior research about strategic self-presentation aimed at disrupting stereotypes about men of color and aggression (Entman and Rojecki Reference Entman and Rojecki2000; Stephens-Dougan Reference Stephens-Dougan2020). However, we also hypothesized that white men would receive more positive competence coverage than men of color but did not find support for this expectation (Hypothesis 8).

Thus, the results suggest men of color face a different pattern of trait coverage relative to women of color. Rather than facing an obstacle of negative trait coverage, they are advantaged in terms of trait coverage relative to white men in the race on both trait dimensions. It’s worth noting that male candidates of color were not particularly competitive in this race, and that much of the critical competence coverage observed for women of color may have been driven by Harris, the most competitive of the candidates of color in 2020. In the next section, we dig deeper into candidate-level trait coverage.

Candidate-Level Trait Coverage Analysis

The analysis so far suggests that media coverage emphasizes certain traits differently based on candidates’ gender and race. One limitation of this analysis is that the data is drawn from a small set of candidates, meaning its possible distinctive coverage for any one candidate might exert a disproportionate influence on trait coverage for any of the race-gender groups. Disaggregating by candidate allows us further insight into the nature of candidate coverage. Though this analysis is exploratory and not hypothesis driven, it offers more context to the group differences reported in the previous section and allows us to identify outlier candidates in terms of trait coverage.

Figure 5 presents the balance of warmth and competence coverage for each candidate, excluding candidates with fewer than 20 trait mentions total. Looking first at competence coverage for female candidates, one can see that on balance, competence coverage was positive for most female candidates, but the proportion of positive coverage did vary across candidates. The range was from 53% positive for Marianne Williamson to 85% positive for Elizabeth Warren. Of the three female candidates receiving the most coverage (Warren, Harris, and Klobuchar), a factor likely related to their greater competitiveness in the race, Harris received markedly more negative coverage – 31% compared to 16% for Elizabeth Warren and 21% for Amy Klobuchar. Maryanne Williamson received a higher proportion of negative competence coverage compared to the other women in the race (47% negative), but because of her lack of competitiveness, she received very little coverage overall. Tulsi Gabbard’s competence coverage was 69% positive, while Kirsten Gillibrand’s competence coverage was 62% positive.

Figure 5. Warmth and competence traits disaggregated by candidate

In terms of female candidates’ warmth, of the three most covered (Warren, Harris, and Klobuchar), Warren and Harris received the most negative warmth coverage – 65% of Warren’s warmth descriptions and 50% of Harris’s warmth descriptions were negative. Just 19% of Klobuchar’s warmth coverage was negative. Among the other women running, Gabbard’s warmth coverage was also mostly negative (63%), but overall, her personal character received very little attention in the corpus of campaign coverage. For the remaining women, warmth coverage was more positive than negative: only 38% of Gillibrand’s and 18% of Williamson’s warmth descriptions were negative. Of the women in the race, Warren and Harris arguably were the most competitive, which explains their greater media attention, and possibly why their personal warmth received more scrutiny. Both women of color in the race – Harris and Gabbard – are also largely on the losing end of warmth descriptions, and this is consistent with studies that show women of color in leadership face additional hurdles when it comes to perceptions of warmth (Dupree Reference Dupree2024; Cooley et al. Reference Cooley, Winslow, Vojt, Shein and Ho2018).

Turning to the male candidates, Biden’s competence descriptions are among the least positive of any candidate (54% positive, 46% negative). This was largely due to frequent attention in articles to Donald Trump’s characterization of Biden as “sleepy” and “weak,” which were coded as negative competence traits. Additionally, there were many news articles written about Biden’s performance in the first Democratic debate that questioned whether he was “prepared.” For example, “Certainly Biden didn’t seem entirely prepared to defend himself from what should have been an obvious line of criticism of his record, given recent news cycles…” and “Biden alternated between forceful defenses of his record and stumbling answers that suggested he wasn’t fully prepared for the intensity of the attacks.” Most of the other men in the race received more positive than negative competence descriptions: Booker (87% positive), Hickenlooper (83% positive), Yang (79% positive), Bullock (78% positive), Sanders (75% positive), Buttigieg (74% positive), Steyer and Castro (70% positive), and O’Rourke (61% positive). The male candidates with the least positive competence coverage were de Blasio and Bloomberg (52% and 51% positive). Recall from the analysis in the previous section that white men received significantly more negative competence coverage compared to men of color. Critical coverage of Biden, de Blasio, and Bloomberg likely contributed much to this difference.

On warmth, Bloomberg (25% positive) and de Blasio (33% positive) received the least positive descriptions. The candidates with the most positive warmth coverage are: Yang (88% positive), Bullock (75% positive), O’Rourke (74% positive), Castro (67% positive), Buttigieg (66% positive), and Booker (65% positive). Biden’s warmth descriptions were 62% positive, Sanders’s just 51% positive, and Hickenlooper’s were 57% positive. This finding that the men of color in the race are among the candidates with the most positive warmth coverage may be evidence that men of color lean into warmth to combat widely held negative stereotypes about racial minorities and violence, especially Black and Latino men (Entman and Rojecki Reference Entman and Rojecki2000), as a deracializing strategy (Stephens-Dougan Reference Stephens-Dougan2020); media coverage might reflect these self-presentation strategies. Collectively, this candidate-level analysis contributes a more nuanced understanding of trait coverage in the 2020 Democratic Presidential primary.

Unpacking the Warmth and Competence Trait Dimensions

To gain further insight into the nature of warmth and competence coverage, we present some additional descriptive analysis that disaggregates the individual facets composing the SCM’s two primary trait dimensions of warmth and competence. This analysis is exploratory and intended to add more granularity to our understanding of these higher order traits for social perception and how they relate to candidate gender. First, we calculated the relative probability each trait was used to describe each candidate – i.e., p(trait | candidate). We then averaged the probabilities for each trait for all male candidates and all female candidates. We took the difference of these averaged probabilities [i.e., p(trait | candidate)WOMEN - p(trait | candidate)MEN] and plotted the most distinctive traits, those with the largest differences. In Figure 6, we present the top ten facets of warmth and competence traits that were most likely to identify female candidates relative to male candidates. Figure 7 presents the top ten facets used to identify male candidates relative to female candidates. By contrast, terms that do not appear on these lists are applied to male and female candidates with more comparable frequency.

Figure 6. Distinctive trait coverage for female candidates

Figure 7. Distinctive trait coverage for male candidates

Facets of competence distinctively applied to female candidates in Figure 6 imply reliability (e.g., committed, dedicated) and physical stamina or dominance (e.g., tough, strong, stronger). Only one competence trait is explicitly about expertise (capable). These distinctive facets of competence are mostly positive, except for “embarrassed,” “nervous,” and “unqualified” are critical comments disproportionately applied to female candidates.

Facets of warmth distinctively applied to female candidates in Figure 6 invoke trustworthiness or the lack thereof (e.g., evasive, forthright), and friendliness (e.g., approachable). “Likable” is also on this list, which is more evidence that women, more so than men, face the likability question when running for office. This analysis suggests that feminine frames are also evoked for women, but not men, when the media is more likely to describe them as “grateful” and “peace-loving,” traits not usually expected or demanded of men with power or passion for a cause.

For traits that favor men in Figure 7, we see facets of competence that imply assertiveness and dominance (e.g., confident, bold, undeterred), and capability (e.g., successful, impressive, suited). Three facets of competence are explicitly about mental facilities, and one is negative (e.g., sensible, articulate, confused). By comparison, warmth coverage uniquely characterizing male candidates imply friendliness (e.g., affable, earnest, generous), and trustworthiness (e.g., transparency, trustworthy). “Emotional” is on this list, suggesting that men’s emotional states or reactions are more newsworthy than women’s, possibly because it reflects instances of gender stereotype violations. By comparison, women were more likely to be depicted as “embarrassed” or “nervous,” traits that only imply negative emotionality. The only negative trait with a gender gap for men was “unpopular,” an externalized description of others’ reaction to them, but not a projection of their own emotions or how the media thought they should feel.

Conclusion

The analysis reported here suggests media coverage of candidates running in the 2020 Democratic presidential primary was gendered and racialized in some respects, and dimensional social perception frameworks like the stereotype content model proved to be a useful tool for highlighting differences in trait coverage between male and female candidates (Abele et al. Reference Abele, Ellemers, Fiske, Koch and Yzerbyt2021). We found that, on the whole, the Democratic female candidates covered in 2020 overcame competence doubts that plagued women in earlier decades because they received more positively valenced competence, on average, compared to male candidates. However, our trait-level analysis showed large gender gaps in competence traits like “successful,” “suited,” and “influential” (more likely to be attributed to the men in the field), and the disproportionate use of these terms could imply women are working from a qualification (or preparedness) deficit (particularly given we find they are uniquely likely to be described using the term “unqualified”). In short, qualitative analysis of even positive competence traits may still reflect interesting and impactful gender differences.

Although women received favorable competence coverage on the whole, warmth coverage emerged as an important battleground. For male candidates, it was relatively straightforward and affirmative, while for the top-performing women in the race, a majority of this coverage (53.7%) was critical and characterized them as lacking in warmth. Moreover, while the analysis of distinctive traits describing female candidates found that words like “approachable” and “forthright” were used more often to describe women than men, consistent with prior research on voter attribution; even some of their positive qualities of warmth highlight differing gender expectations. Men are not described as “grateful” or “peace-loving,” but women are.

Additionally, we found that the women of color in the race faced a noticeable competence deficit, compared to the white women in the race. Although their competence coverage was overall more positive than negative, it was less positive than white women’s competence coverage. For both white women and women of color, their warmth coverage was more negative than positive, on balance.

This finding about the distinctive trait coverage facing women of color potentially situates us to understand media coverage of Kamala Harris’s 2024 presidential bid. At the very least, it provides an important point of comparison for future work investigating differences in primary and general election coverage, given that we will soon have a corpus of coverage for the same candidate in a primary and a general election.

For the men of color in the race, they were described in positive warmth terms more than negative, and largely more so than white men in the race on average, which further points to the need for an intersectional analysis of trait coverage in campaigns.

Whether these findings for male and female candidates, especially at the intersection of race, are generalizable to Democratic primaries more broadly, is an open question. While the 2020 Democratic primary was relatively diverse, the candidates of color in the race still saw a fraction of the coverage of white candidates, and thus additional research of diverse Democratic primaries should apply the SCM to examine whether the patterns observed in this study hold with other candidates. Such research could help determine if the stereotype content associated with male and female candidates of different racial backgrounds remains consistent across different electoral settings, or if these patterns vary depending on candidates themselves, or aspects of the specific contest. Expanding the application of the SCM in this way would contribute to a deeper understanding of gender, the presidency, and media coverage.

The study also delved into the individual profiles of male and female candidates, shedding light on specific patterns of coverage. Notably, highly competitive female candidates, like Warren and Harris, faced the most negative warmth descriptions, possibly due to increased scrutiny. This finding suggests a dichotomy for competitive women, where positive attributes in warmth or competence are emphasized, but rarely both, which aligns with broader discussions on gender biases in leadership perceptions, and the double bind.

Our results also raise directions for future research. First, our analysis aggregates across news sources with different ideological orientations. Closer attention to news sources might reveal further differences in candidate coverage, with certain outlets or sets of outlets with common political leanings covering candidates in more or less stereotypic terms. Such analysis would offer insights into the extent to which depictions of candidates are media-driven versus campaign-driven. Candidates’ efforts to control their images only go so far. Carefully crafted campaign communications interact with opponents’ messaging as well as media coverage of the race. As a result, candidates only have limited control over how their personal character and leadership potential is described. In some respects, this complicates the interpretation of gender and race-based differences in coverage as bias, given that candidates do differ in their experiences and personalities, and campaign strategies.

Second, our results raise questions about how voters weigh qualities like warmth and competence in their voting calculus. Some research suggests competence, more so than warmth, shapes electoral outcomes (i.e., McAllister Reference McAllister2016; McGraw Reference McGraw, Druckman, Green, Kuklinski and Lupia2011), while other work finds the centrality of warmth traits depend on partisanship (Bor and Laustsen Reference Bor and Laustsen2022); but this work does not evaluate whether the relative importance of these traits for voters varies by candidate gender, race, or the intersection of the two. Further research could also investigate double standards surrounding warmth, where certain actions and behaviors are interpreted differently when exhibited by women and men on the campaign trail. For example, if male and female candidates act similarly on the campaign trail but certain actions are viewed as uncharacteristic of women (such as lacking warmth), that can result in more attention to the negative behavior for women, because it is not seen as expected of men. Other cross-national work shows that a focus on communal traits, like warmth, in candidate coverage can disadvantage women candidates’ viability across settings (Rohrbach, Aaldering and Van der Pas Reference Rohrbach, Aaldering and Van der Pas2023). Voters (and journalists) may also have different traits and role expectations for different offices, such as a President or executive office versus a seat in a deliberative body (Huddy and Terkildsen Reference Huddy and Terkildsen1993). Further work probing gender stereotypes in media coverage of political campaigns across institutional settings and across political contexts is necessary to establish the generalizability of the patterns uncovered here, given that our analysis relies on a single American presidential primary. Ultimately, this is an area ripe for further investigation, and the approach developed here is one that can be adopted for various races, offices, and settings as a useful framework for understanding the prevalence of gendered trait coverage of candidates.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Cameron Espinoza and Jiwon Nam for their research assistance, and the editor and anonymous reviewers for their valuable feedback on earlier drafts of this article.

Appendix

Table A1. Media sources and number of documents

Table A2. Warmth and competence attributes in campaign coverage

Footnotes

1. For context, the white female candidates in the analysis include: Marianne Williamson, Kirsten Gillibrand, Amy Klobuchar, and Elizabeth Warren; female candidates of color include: Kamala Harris and Tulsi Gabbard; white male candidates include: Bernie Sanders, Joe Biden, Pete Buttigieg, Michael Bennet, Steve Bullock, Bill de Blasio, John Hickenlooper, Beto O’Rourke, Michael Bloomberg, and Tom Steyer; and male candidates of color include: Cory Booker, Julián Castro, and Andrew Yang.

2. For pre-processing, we used minhash and locality sensitive hashing with a jaccard similarity threshold of 0.75 and a locality sensitive hashing threshold of 0.5.

3. Documentation for spaCy’s Dependency Parser is here: https://spacy.io/api/dependencyparser

4. HuggingFace’s NeuralCoref is a Python library designed for coreference resolution, which is the task of determining when different expressions in a text refer to the same entity (e.g., recognizing that “Elizabeth” and “she” are the same person in a sentence). HuggingFace’s pre-trained NeuralCoref model is available on GitHub: https://github.com/huggingface/neuralcoref

References

Abele, Andrea E. and Wojciszke, Bogan. 2018. “The Big Two of Agency and Communion as an Overarching Framework in Psychology.” In Agency and Communion in Social Psychology eds. Abele, Andrea E., and Wojciszke, Bogan. London, UK: Routledge. 12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abele, Andrea E., Hauke, Nicole, Peters, Kim, Louvet, Eva, Aleksandra, Szymkow, and Yanping, Duan. 2016. Facets of the Fundamental Content Dimensions: Agency with Competence and Assertiveness – Communion with Warmth and Morality. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abele, Andrea E., Ellemers, Naomi, Fiske, Susan T., Koch, Alex, and Yzerbyt, Vincent. 2021. “Navigating the Social World: Toward an Integrated Framework for Evaluating Self, Individuals, and Groups.” Psychological Review 128 (2): 290314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anzia, Sarah F., and Berry, Christopher R.. 2011. “The Jackie (and Jill) Robinson Effect: Why Do Congresswomen Outperform Congressmen?American Journal of Political Science 55 (3): 478493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nicole M. 2024. “Gendered Self-promotion: Differences in How Voters Evaluate Women and Men who Highlight their Legislative Accomplishments.” Political Research Quarterly 77 (1): 344–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nicole M. 2020. The Qualification Gap: Why Women Must Be Better Than Men to Run for Political Office. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nicole M. and Santia, Martina. 2022. “Going Feminine: Identifying How and When Female Candidates Emphasize Feminine and Masculine Traits on the Campaign Trail.” Political Research Quarterly 75 (3): 691705. https://doi.org/10.1177/10659129211020257CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nicole M. and Taylor, Tatum. 2023. “Selling Them Short? Differences in News Coverage of Female and Male Candidate Qualifications.” Political Research Quarterly 76 (1): 308–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedolla, Lisa G. 2007. “Intersections of Inequality: Understanding Marginalization and Privilege In the Post-Civil Rights Era.” Politics & Gender 3 (20): 232–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bejarano, Christina, Brown, Nadia E., Gershon, Sarah A., and Montoya, Celeste 2021. “Shared Identities: Intersectionality, Linked Fate, and Perceptions of Political Candidates.” Political Research Quarterly 74 (4): 970–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bor, A. and Laustsen, L. 2022. “Candidate Evaluations Across the Aisle: Cross-cultural Evidence that Out-partisans Value Candidate Warmth More Than In-partisans.” European Journal of Political Research 61 (4): 878900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bystrom, Dianne G., Robertson, Terry A., and Banwart, Mary C. 2001. “Framing the Fight: An Analysis of Media Coverage of Female and Male Candidates in Primary Races for Governor and U.S. Senate in 2000.” American Behavioral Scientist 44 (12): 19992013.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cargile, Ivy A. M. 2023. “Stereotyping Latinas: Candidate Gender and Ethnicity on the Political Stage.” Politics, Groups and Identities 11 (2): 207–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlin, Diana B. and Winfrey, Kelly L. 2009. “Hillary Clinton, Sarah Palin, and Sexism in 2008 Campaign Coverage.” Communication Studies 60 (4): 326–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassese, E. 2019. Intersectional Stereotyping. In The Oxford Encyclopedia of Political Decision Making, eds. Redlawsk, David and Oxley, Z., 127. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chang, Ho-Chun H., Brichta, Maximilian, Ahn, Soyun and de Vight, Jackson. 2023. “Will She Win? Gendered Media Coverage of the 2020 Democratic Presidential Primaries.” Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly. Advanced online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/10776990231182154Google Scholar
Cohen, Marty, Karol, David, Noel, Hans, and Zaller, John. 2008. The Party Decides: Presidential Nominations Before and After Reform. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coles, Stewart Matthew and Pasek, Josh. 2020. “Intersectional Invisibility Revisited: How Group Prototypes Lead to the Erasure and Exclusion of Black Women.” Translational Issues in Psychological Science 6 (4): 314–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conroy, Meredith. 2015. Masculinity, Media, and the American Presidency. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conroy, Meredith and Green, Jon 2020. “It Takes a Motive: Communal and Agentic Articulated Interest and Candidate Emergence.” Political Research Quarterly 73 (4): 942–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooley, Erin. Winslow, Hannah, Vojt, Andrew, Shein, Jonathon, and Ho, Jennifer. 2018. “Bias at the Intersection of Identity: Conflicting Social Stereotypes of Gender and Race Augment the Perceived Femininity and Interpersonal Warmth of Smiling Black Women.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 74: 43–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuddy, Amy J. C., Glick, Peter, and Beinger, Anna. 2011. “The Dynamics of Warmth and Competence Judgments and Their Outcomes in Organizations.” Research in Organizational Behavior 31: 7398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuddy, Amy J. C., Fiske, Susan T., and Glick, Peter. 2008. “Warmth and Competence as Universal Dimensions of Social Perception.” Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 40: 61149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuddy, Amy J. C., Fiske, Susan T., and Glick, Peter. 2004. ”When Professionals Become Mothers, Warmth Doesn’t Cut the Ice.” Journal of Social Issues 60 (4): 701–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dittmar, Kelly. 2015. Navigating Gendered Terrain: Stereotypes and Strategy in Political Campaigns. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Donovan, Roxanne A. 2011. “Tough or Tender: (Dis) Similarities in White College Students’ Perceptions of Black and White Women.” Psychology of Women Quarterly 35 (3): 458–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dowe, Pearl K. F. 2020. “Resisting Marginalization: Black Women’s Political Ambition and Agency.” PS: Political Science & Politics 53 (4): 697702.Google Scholar
Duerst-Lahti, Georgia. 2008. “Seeing What Has Always Been: Opening Study of the Presidency.” PS: Political Science & Politics 41 (4): 733–7.Google Scholar
Dupree, Cydney H. 2024. “Words of a Leader: The Importance of Intersectionality for Understanding Women Leaders’ Use of Dominant Language and How Others Receive It.” Administrative Science Quarterly 69 (2): 271323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagly, Alice H., and Karau, Steven J. 2002. “Role Congruity Theory of Prejudice toward Female Leaders.” Psychological Review 109 (3): 573–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Entman, Robert M. and Rojecki, Andrew. 2000. Black Image in the White Mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Falk, Erika. 2010. Women for President: Media Bias in Nine Campaigns. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Farnsworth, Steven J. and Lichter, S. Robert. 2016. News Coverage of U.S. Presidential Campaigns: Reporting on Primaries and General Elections, 1988-2012. In Praeger Handbook of Political Campaigning in the United States, ed. Benoit, W. L. London, UK: Bloomsbury Publishing. 233254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiske, Susan T., Cuddy, Amy J., Glick, Peter, and Xu, Jun. 2002.” A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82 (6): 878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiske, Susan T., Xu, Jun, Cuddy, Amy, and Glick, Peter. 1999. “(Dis) respecting Versus (Dis) liking: Status and Interdependence Predict Ambivalent Stereotypes of Competence and Warmth.” Journal of Social Issues 55 (3): 473–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fulton, Sarah A. 2012. “Running Backwards and in High Heels: The Gendered Quality Gap and Incumbent Electoral Success.” Political Research Quarterly 65 (2): 303314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fulton, Sarah A., & Dhima, Kostanca. 2021. “The Gendered Politics of Congressional Elections.” Political Behavior 43 (4): 1611–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funk, Kendall D, Hinojosa, Magda, and Piscopo, Jennifer M. 2021. Women to the Rescue: The Gendered Effects of Public Discontent on Legislative Nominations in Latin America. Party Politics 27 (3): 465–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galinsky, Adam D., Hall, Erika V., and Cuddy, Amy J. C. 2013. “Gendered Races: Implications for Interracial Marriage, Leadership Selection, and Athletic Participation.” Psychological Science 24 (4): 498506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrits, Bailey, Trimble, Linda, Wagner, Angelia, Raphael, Daisy, and Sampert, Shannon. 2017. “Political Battlefield: Aggressive Metaphors, Gender, and Power in News Coverage of Canadian Party Leadership Contests.” Feminist Media Studies 17 (6): 1088–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gershon, Sarah A. and Lavariega Monforti, Jessica. 2019. “Intersecting Campaigns: Candidate Race, Ethnicity, and Voter Evaluations.” Politics, Groups & Identities 9 (3): 439–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gidengil, Elisabeth, & Everitt, Joanna. 2003. “Talking Tough: Gender and Reported Speech in Campaign News Coverage.” Political Communication 20 (3): 209–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Danny. 2011. “When Gender and Party Collide: Stereotyping in Candidate Trait Attribution.” Politics & Gender 7 (2): 133–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Danny and Lawless, Jennifer L. 2016. Women on the Run: Gender, Media, and Political Campaigns in a Polarized Era. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heldman, Caroline, Carroll, Susan J., & Olson, Stephanie. 2005. “‘She Brought Only a Skirt’: Print Media Coverage of Elizabeth Dole’s Big for the Republican Nomination.” Political Communication 22 (3): 315–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heldman, Caroline, Conroy, Meredith, and Ackerman, Alissa R. 2018. Sex and Gender in the 2016 Presidential Election. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-Clio.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddy, Leonie and Terkildsen, Nayda. 1993. “The Consequences of Gender Stereotypes for Women Candidates at Different Levels and Types of Office.” Political Research Quarterly 46 (3): 503–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jamieson, Kathleen H. (1995). Beyond the Double Bind: Women and Leadership. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahn, Kim F. 1994. “The Distorted Mirror: Press Coverage of Women Candidates for Statewide Office.” Journal of Politics 56 (1): 154–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinder, Donald R., Peters, Mark D., Abelson, Robert P., and Fiske, Susan T.. 1980. “Presidential Prototypes.” Political Behavior 2 (4): 315–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koenig, Anne M. and Eagly, Alice H. 2014. “Evidence for the Social Role Theory of Stereotype Content.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 107 (3): 371–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landis, J. Richard and Koch, Gary G. 1977. ”The Measurement of Observer Agreement for Categorical Data.” Biometrics. 33 (1): 159–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laustsen, Lasse and Bor, Alexander. 2017. “The Relative Weight of Character Traits in Political Candidate Evaluations: Warmth is More Important than Competence, Leadership, and Integrity.” Electoral Studies 49: 97107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, Regina G. 2018. Do the Media Give Women Candidates a Fair Shake? In New Directions in Media and Politics, ed. Ridout, Travis N., New York, NY: Routledge. 199209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, Regina G. and Rose, Melody. 2008. Hillary Clinton’s Race for the White House: Gender Politics and the Media on the Campaign Trail. Colorado Springs, CO: Lynne Reinner Publishers.Google Scholar
Lawrence, Regina G. and Rose, Melody. 2011. “Bringing Out the Hook: Exit Talk in Media Coverage of Hillary Clinton and Past Presidential Campaigns.” Political Research Quarterly 64 (4): 870–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lay, Celeste, Holman, Mirya, Bos, Angela L., Greenlee, Jill S., Oxley, Zoe M., and Buffett, A. 2021. “TIME for Kids to Learn Gender Stereotypes: Analysis of Gender and Political Leadership in a Common Social Studies Resource for Children.” Politics & Gender 17 (1): 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerer, Lisa (2019, January 3). Is “Likability” in Politics Sexist? Yes. It’s Also Outdated. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2019/01/03/us/politics/on-politics-likable-elizabeth-warren.htmlGoogle Scholar
Le Foulon, Carmen and Reyes-Housholder, Catherine. 2021. “Candidate Sex, Corruption and Vote Choice.” Electoral Studies 69, 102270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebenow, Hayley A., Boucher, Kathryn L., and Cassidy, Brittany S. 2022. “Understanding Evaluations of Kamala Harris in 2020: Political Ideology Qualifies Perceived Communality Effects When Communal Cues Are Present.” Psychology of Women Quarterly 46 (4): 399419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, Monica H., Kwan, Virginia S. Y., Cheung, Anna, and Fiske, Susan T. 2005. “Stereotype Content Model Explains Prejudice for an Envied Outgroup: Scale of Anti-Asian American Stereotypes.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31 (1): 3447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, Robert W., Rosette, Ashleigh S., and Washington, Ella F. 2012. “Can an Agentic Black Woman Get Ahead? The Impact of Race and Interpersonal Dominance on Perceptions of Female Leaders.” Psychological Science 23 (4): 354–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucciola, Natalia. 2023. “The Perks of Being Female: Gender Stereotypes and Voters’ Preferences in Brazil. Politics & Gender 19 (2): 507–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAllister, Ian 2016. Candidates and Voting Choice. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics, 118. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.013.73Google Scholar
McGraw, K. M. 2011. Candidate Impressions and Evaluations. Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science, In eds. Druckman, James N., Green, Donald P., Kuklinski, James H., and Lupia, Arthur. 187200. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meeks, Lindsey. 2013. “All the Gender That’s Fit to Print: How the New York Times Covered Hillary Clinton and Sarah Palin in 2008.” Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly 90 (3): 520–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolas, Gandalf, Bai, Xuechunzi, and Fiske, Susan T. 2020. “Comprehensive Stereotype Content Dictionaries Using a Semi-Automated Method.” European Journal of Social Psychology 51: 178–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pao, Christina and Akhil, Rajan. 2023. Same Qualifications, Different Identities: Evaluating Voter Perceptions by Candidate Sexuality, Race, and Gender. In Distinct Identities: Minority Women in U.S. Politics, eds. Brown, Nadia E. and Gershon, Sarah. New York, NY: Routledge. 1531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purdie-Vaughns, Valerie and Eibach, Richard P. 2008. “Intersectional Invisibility: The Distinctive Advantages and Disadvantages of Multiple Subordinate-Group Identities.” Sex Roles 59 (5): 377–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritter, Barbara A. and Yoder, Janice D. 2004. “Gender Differences in Leader Emergence Persist Even for Dominant Women: An Updated Confirmation of Role Congruity Theory.” Psychology of Women Quarterly 28 (3): 187–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohrbach, Tobias, Aaldering, Loes, and Van der Pas, Daphne J. 2023. “Gender Differences and Similarities in News Media Effects on Political Candidate Evaluations: A Meta-Analysis.” Journal of Communication 73 (2): 101–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosette, Ashleigh S. and Tost, Leigh P. 2013. Perceiving Social Inequity: When Subordinate-Group Positioning On One Dimension of Social Hierarchy Enhances Privilege Recognition on Another. Psychological Science 24 (8): 1420–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosette, Ashleigh S., Leonardelli, Geoffrey J., and Phillips, Katherine W. 2008. “The White Standard: Racial Bias in Leadership Categorization.” Journal of Applied Psychology 93 (4): 758–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosette, Ashleigh S., Koval, Christy Z., Ma, Anyi, and Livingston, Robert. 2016. ”Race Matters for Women Leaders: Intersectional Effects on Agentic Deficiencies and Penalties.” The Leadership Quarterly 27 (3): 429–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santhanam, Laura. 2019, July 22. In 2020, Democrats Say They’ll Negotiate on Values as Long As They Beat Trump. PBS.org. https://www.pbs.org/newshour/politics/in-2020-democrats-say-theyll-negotiate-on-values-as-long-as-they-beat-trumpGoogle Scholar
Schneider, Monica and Bos, Monica. 2014. “Measuring Stereotypes of Female Politicians.” Political Psychology 35 (2): 245–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siddiqui, Sabrina. 2019, February 4. “Why Women 2020 Candidates Face ‘Likability’ Question Even As They Make History.” The Guardian. https://www.theguardian.com/us-news/2019/feb/04/why-the-likability-question-pursues-2020-female-candidates-even-as-they-make-historyGoogle Scholar
Skelley, Geoffrey. 2019, November 19. Have the Democratic Debates Moved the Polls at All? FiveThirtyEight.com. https://fivethirtyeight.com/features/have-the-democratic-debates-moved-the-polls-at-all/Google Scholar
Smith, Aidan. 2017. Gender, Heteronormativity, and the American Presidency. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens-Dougan, Lafleur. 2020. Race to the Bottom: How Racial Appeals Work in American Politics. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sy, Thomas, Shore, Lynn M., Strauss, Judy, Shore, Ted H., Tram, Susanna, Whiteley, Paul, and Ikeda-Muromachi, Kristine. 2010. “Leadership Perceptions as a Function of Race–Occupation Fit: The Case of Asian Americans.” Journal of Applied Psychology 95 (5): 902–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thames, Frank C. and Williams, Margaret S. 2013. Contagious Representation: Women’s Political Representation in Democracies Around the World. New York, NY: NYU Press.Google Scholar
Thomas, Melanee, Harell, Allison, Rijkhoff, Sanne A. M., and Gosselin, Tania. 2021. “Gendered News Coverage and Women as Heads of Government.” Political Communication 38 (4): 388406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teele, Dawn Langan, Kalla, Joshua, and Rosenbluth, Frances. 2018. “The Ties That Double Bind: Social Roles and Women’s Underrepresentation in Politics.” American Political Science Review 112 (3): 525541.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolley, Erin, Lawlor, Andrea, and Fortier-Chouinard, Alexandre. 2023. “‘Whiny, Fake, and I Don’t Like Her Hair’: Gendered Assessments of Mayoral Candidates.” Urban Affairs Review 59 (4): 9771012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trimble, Linda. 2017. Ms. Prime Minister: Gender, Media, and Leadership. Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uscinski, Joseph E. and Goren, Lilly J. 2009. “What’s in a Name? Coverage of Senator Hillary Clinton During the 2008 Democratic Primary.” Political Research Quarterly 64 (4): 884–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van der Pas, Daphne and Aaldering, Loes. 2020. ”Gender Differences in Political Media Coverage: A Meta-Analysis.” Journal of Communication 70 (1): 114–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, Angelia, Trimble, Linda, Curtin, Jennifer, Auer, Meagan, and Woodman, V. K. G. 2022. “Representations of Political Leadership Qualities in News Coverage of Australian and Canadian Government Leaders.” Politics & Gender 18 (3): 798829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walzer, Amy S. and Czopp, Alexander M. 2011. “Able but Unintelligent: Including Positively Stereotyped Black Subgroups in the Stereotype Content Model.” Journal of Social Psychology 151 (5): 527–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wojciszke, Bogdan and Abele, Andrea E. 2008. “The Primacy of Communion Over Agency and Its Reversals in Evaluations.” European Journal of Social Psychology 38: 1139–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, Wendy and Eagly, Amy H. 2012. “Biosocial Construction of Sex Differences and Similarities in Behavior.” Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 46: 55123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Warmth and competence traits in media coverage of male and female presidential candidates

Figure 1

Figure 2. Warmth and competence trait coverage by valence and candidate gender

Figure 2

Figure 3. Quantity of warmth and competence coverage by candidate gender and race

Figure 3

Figure 4. Valence of warmth and competence coverage by candidate gender and race

Figure 4

Figure 5. Warmth and competence traits disaggregated by candidate

Figure 5

Figure 6. Distinctive trait coverage for female candidates

Figure 6

Figure 7. Distinctive trait coverage for male candidates

Figure 7

Table A1. Media sources and number of documents

Figure 8

Table A2. Warmth and competence attributes in campaign coverage