Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T19:46:16.010Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Job Talk: Candidate Gender and Presentation of Prior Experience in Television Ads in the US

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2024

Eric R. Hansen*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
Connor Mautner
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA
*
Corresponding author: Eric R. Hansen; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Does gender influence how candidates in the United States present their prior political experience to voters? Messaging one’s experience might demonstrate a history of power-seeking behavior, a gender role violation for women under traditional norms. As a result, men should be more likely to make experience-based appeals than women candidates. For evidence, we analyze the contents of 1,030 televised advertisements from 2018 state legislative candidates from the Wesleyan Media Project. We find that ads sponsored by experienced men are significantly more likely to highlight experience than ads sponsored by experienced women. However, we find that women’s and men’s ads are roughly equally likely to discuss work experience, suggesting that men’s greater emphasis on experience is limited to prior officeholding. The results contribute to our understanding of gender dynamics in political campaigns, the information available to voters, and how advertising shapes the criteria voters use to assess candidates.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Women, Gender, and Politics Research Section of the American Political Science Association

Why do some candidates emphasize their prior officeholding experience to voters while others do not? Most candidates make biographical details available to voters on the campaign trail, but many decline to emphasize that experience when presenting themselves to voters. For example, when Pennsylvania State Representative Tina Davis challenged State Senator Tommy Tomlinson for his seat in 2018, one TV ad portrayed her as a “mom on a mission to shake up the Senate,” rather than as a four-term, sitting state legislator with the requisite experience for the position.Footnote 1 Understanding why candidates choose to emphasize experience or not is important to understanding the considerations available to voters as they weigh whom to support.

We investigate the role of gender in structuring candidates’ choices to emphasize experience, grounding our expectations in social role theory and role congruity theory (Eagly and Karau Reference Eagly and Karau2002; Eagly and Wood Reference Eagly, Wood, Kruglanski and Higgins2012; Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2019). By emphasizing experience in political office, women candidates would violate traditional gender norms against women demonstrating power-seeking behavior. As a result, we should expect to see women highlight their experience less than men. For empirical evidence, we turn to TV advertisements for state legislative candidates in 2018 from the Wesleyan Media Project (Fowler et al. Reference Fowler, Franz, Ridout and Baum2020).Footnote 2 We find that, among candidates who had held prior elected office, ads sponsored by men are significantly more likely than ads sponsored by women to emphasize the candidate’s prior political experience. However, ads sponsored by men and women are similarly likely to discuss other types of work experience.

The results complicate how political scientists understand the interplay between gender and experience on the campaign trail. Though gender differences do not appear in presentation of political experience in congressional campaign websites (Bauer Reference Bauer2020; McDonald, Porter, and Treul Reference McDonald, Porter and Treul2020), the present results suggest that candidates’ discussion of experience depends on the medium, with men emphasizing their experience more than women in TV ads. Gender differences in the discussion of experience may undermine women candidates’ qualifications for public office in the minds of voters, contributing in a small way to the underrepresentation of women in office.

Gendered Social Roles and Self-Presentation in Campaigns

Women remain numerically underrepresented in public office at all levels of government (Center for American Women and Politics 2022). Many attribute this underrepresentation to sexism against women candidates from the electorate. According to Hayes and Lawless (Reference Hayes and Lawless2016, 3), 47% of Americans agree that women “face bias from voters,” and 31% agree that women “don’t win as often as men.” However, scholarship since the 1990s has generally concluded that men and women perform equally well once they appear on the ballot (see Lawless Reference Lawless2015; Schwarz and Coppock Reference Schwarz and Coppock2022). Women’s underrepresentation may be better explained by gender differences in the decision to run for office. Women disproportionately decline to run compared to similarly situated men for a variety of reasons, including family care obligations (Bernhard, Shames, and Teele Reference Bernhard, Shames and Teele2021; Fulton et al. Reference Fulton, Maestas, Maisel and Stone2006), absent recruitment efforts from party leaders (Fox and Lawless Reference Fox and Lawless2010; Sanbonmatsu Reference Sanbonmatsu2002), and a relative lack of role models in high-level offices (Campbell and Wolbrecht Reference Campbell and Wolbrecht2006; Ladam, Harden, and Windett Reference Ladam, Harden and Windett2018).

Another potential reason women decline to run — one that we argue also relates to women candidates’ messaging choices — is that holding political office violates traditional gender norms. Social role theory holds that gender stereotypes and traits develop from a division of labor by sex in traditional societies — men provide by laboring in the public sphere, whereas women occupy caretaking roles (Eagly and Wood Reference Eagly, Wood, Kruglanski and Higgins2012). Building on that idea, role congruity theory explains that women will face social sanctions for defying gender norms by engaging in agentic behaviors, such as holding leadership positions or positions of power (Eagly and Karau Reference Eagly and Karau2002; Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2019).Footnote 3

Together, these theories can help to explain several patterns in women’s self-selection into political candidacy. Men are more likely than women to express an interest in running for office (Fox and Lawless Reference Fox and Lawless2005; Schneider et al. Reference Schneider, Holman, Diekman and McAndrew2016). Resulting pressure from gendered expectations may lead potential women candidates to underrate their own qualifications for running (Fox and Lawless Reference Fox and Lawless2004) and make more risk-averse choices about when and where to run (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Mansour, O’Connell and Reeves2019; Fox and Oxley Reference Fox and Oxley2003; Fulton Reference Fulton2012; Ondercin Reference Ondercin2022; Pearson and McGhee Reference Pearson and McGhee2013). When explaining their interest in running for office, women are also more likely to express communal motivations (e.g., working with others, serving the community) than agentic motivations (e.g., gaining experience, changing the system) (Conroy and Green Reference Conroy and Green2020).

Gender role congruity should also inform how women candidates present themselves to voters. We focus on the presentation of experience. By experience, we mean a career that provided some skills or preparation to the candidate before running for office. Candidates discuss their pre-electoral experience to telegraph competence and build voters’ confidence in the candidate’s readiness for office. In our view, “experience” is closely related to a “qualification,” a frequently invoked term in the gender literature (e.g., Bauer Reference Bauer2020), since both point toward a candidate’s fitness to handle the responsibilities of an office.

Oftentimes, experience connotes prior service in elected office. In the political realm, prior officeholding experience signals that the candidate is already prepared for public service and can take on the responsibilities of elected office with lower starting costs. However, experience (and achievement) in a nonpolitical occupation can also build perceptions of competence. For example, in 2016, then-candidate Donald Trump stressed his experience in business as a qualification for the presidency. When respondents were provided information that Trump’s wealth was largely inherited from his father rather than built from his business, they perceived him as less competent in business and lowered their support for his presidency (McDonald, Karol, and Mason Reference McDonald, Karol and Mason2020).

Women candidates face a different set of challenges than men in communicating experience on the campaign trail because experience is a gendered characteristic, particularly in politics. Americans have long associated leadership with masculinity (Bauer Reference Bauer2020). Experience connotes competence, power, ambition, status, and leadership, all terms that take on an agentic and thus masculine tone (Conroy and Green Reference Conroy and Green2020). Given these often-unconscious expectations, women candidates face an uphill battle proving themselves to the public because voters do not automatically assume women are qualified or competent to hold office (Ditonto, Hamilton, and Redlawsk Reference Ditonto, Hamilton and Redlawsk2014; Mo Reference Mo2015).

A reasonable response to low expectations from voters would be for women candidates to explain why they are qualified to hold office based on their experience. However, a claim of strong qualifications from prior officeholding would be a claim that one has engaged in power-seeking behavior in the past, itself a gender norm violation for women (Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2019). Declaring one’s qualifications could invite backlash from voters for norm-violating behavior. As an alternative, women candidates might choose to downplay or omit their experience when campaigning in an effort to avoid anticipated backlash from voters. Although we generally expect discussion of experience to violate gender norms, we acknowledge that women candidates could evoke experience in ways that conform with gender norms, such as discussing how their experience helped them collaborate or achieve communal goals. Discussion of experience might also be aimed to demonstrate a candidate’s commitment to action on a specific issue or to loyalty to a group of voters.

In political science research, the extent to which voters truly penalize women candidates for discussing their experience remains an open question. When campaigning, women candidates face diminished prospects in electoral contexts when voters expect more agentic behavior from their leaders, such as in times of terrorism or when choosing leaders for high levels of office (Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2016; Huddy and Terkildsen Reference Huddy and Terkildsen1993; McDonald and Piatak Reference McDonald and Piatak2022). Messaging experience may produce a countervailing effect that helps women overcome stereotypes of incompetence (Bauer Reference Bauer2017, Reference Bauer2020; Ditonto, Hamilton, and Redlawsk Reference Ditonto, Hamilton and Redlawsk2014; Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2017; Mo Reference Mo2015).Footnote 4 Still, that messaging may come at a cost. When women promote their own experience, evaluations of their competence tend to improve, but evaluations of their social attractiveness or likeability simultaneously decline (Bauer Reference Bauer2017; Rudman Reference Rudman1998).

Regardless of the extent to which voters penalize women for messaging experience, candidates’ perceptions of what voters expect should determine candidates’ messaging choices more than voters’ true expectations. Candidates operate in environments of uncertainty about voters’ beliefs and expectations (Hershey Reference Hershey1974). Politicians’ sometimes-misplaced beliefs and assumptions about voters guide their behavior (Miler Reference Miler2010).

Therefore, even if voters’ gendered expectations do not ultimately affect the outcomes of elections, candidates often choose to message as if they might. Dittmar’s (Reference Dittmar2015) study of how campaign professionals address gender dynamics supports this claim. The campaign consultants and strategists she interviewed expected voters to apply gender stereotypes to candidates, even though they held mixed opinions on whether gender stereotypes ultimately affected election outcomes. Nonetheless, they accounted for gender in their messaging choices to counteract or forestall potential threats from violations of gender stereotypes. In Dittmar’s telling, recognizing and reacting to potential voter stereotypes often involved not a direct challenge to gender stereotypes but an adaptation of a message to provide maximum advantage to women candidates within the confines of a gendered political landscape. She writes, “While my interviews revealed little disruption in which credentials are expected of political candidates, they did demonstrate some potential shifts in the sites from which those credentials can be earned” (2015, 125, emphasis in original). Applied to messaging surrounding experience, we might expect women candidates not to evoke their prior officeholding experience directly but instead to discuss how forms of nontraditional experience qualify them for office. Pennsylvania State Rep. Tina Davis’ self-presentation as a “mom on a mission,” rather than a long-serving state legislator, would fit that mold.

To summarize the argument, candidates have incentives to present their experience to voters to establish their competence and qualifications for an elected office. However, the risk of violating gender role norms presents women candidates with opposing incentives to downplay or avoid discussion of their prior officeholding experience. On the campaign trail, public communications from women candidates should feature less information about their experience than similarly qualified men. We test the following hypothesis:

H1: Ads sponsored by women seeking public office will be less likely to mention the candidate’s prior political experience than ads sponsored by men.

Although it is common for prior officeholders to emphasize their experience in ads, it is also common for candidates to emphasize their experience outside of elected office — for example, as business leaders or professionals. It is unclear whether women candidates would violate gender norms in advertising their work experience. A simplistic read of role congruity theory might yield the expectation that gender role violations would occur with any discussion of women’s agency, regardless of whether those pursuits are in political or professional realm. Yet, Schneider and Bos (Reference Schneider and Bos2019, 184) point out a distinction in agentic goals between dominance-oriented goals and independence or individual mastery goals. With the increased entry of women into the workforce over the last century, including greater representation in high-status professions, men and women alike share an interest in achievement. However, achievement does not necessarily indicate holding power-oriented goals; achievement may be instrumental to attaining communal goals. Women tend to endorse individual mastery goals and pursue careers that allow for achievement instead of dominance (Schneider et al. Reference Schneider, Holman, Diekman and McAndrew2016).

Applied to the discussion of experience on the campaign trail, women candidates might be hesitant to discuss political experience — more closely aligned with holding power-oriented goals — but less hesitant to discuss professional experience — which could indicate either power-oriented goals or achievement-oriented goals. Candidates could also intend for a discussion of their career to reinforce a personal brand, to lead voters to evaluate the candidate favorably based on positive stereotypes they have of the candidate’s occupation, or to appeal to specific constituencies in a district. Relevantly, Bauer (Reference Bauer2020) finds that women candidates, relative to men, are more likely to emphasize professional experience on their campaign websites than their political experience.

We acknowledge too, however, that achievement could be taken as a gender norm violation if discussion of it is perceived as self-promoting or bragging. Yet we might reasonably expect a discussion of political experience to evoke power-seeking goals more than other types of experience given its more direct connection to decision-making authority and political ambition. We ultimately hold mixed expectations about the extent to which women candidates will emphasize their professional experience relative to men, but to allow for comparability of results, we test a hypothesis directionally in line with our expectations around political experience. We test the following hypothesis:

H2: Ads sponsored by women seeking public office will be less likely to mention the candidate’s work experience than ads sponsored by men.

Though we expect women candidates to downplay their experience in campaign ads, our hypotheses are falsifiable. Relevant literature has offered evidence supporting competing expectations. Bauer (Reference Bauer2020) advances and finds support for a strategic emergence hypothesis in which women emphasize their experience no less than (and perhaps even more than) men. There are two reasons for this expectation. First, women candidates may anticipate a gendered landscape and assert their qualifications to compete on an even footing with men and head off voters’ stereotypes about incompetence (see Ditonto, Hamilton, and Redlawsk Reference Ditonto, Hamilton and Redlawsk2014). Second, because of unequal expectations surrounding women’s qualifications, better-qualified women with more agentic motivations would select into running for office and run stronger campaigns than their male counterparts (Conroy and Green Reference Conroy and Green2020; Fulton Reference Fulton2012; Pearson and McGhee Reference Pearson and McGhee2013). As a result, self-selection would produce women candidates willing to emphasize their credentials, and no gender gap in messaging surrounding experience would emerge. Such a null finding would also be consistent with a range of studies finding few or no differences between male and female candidates in the issues and traits they choose to emphasize (Dolan Reference Dolan2005; Evans, Cordova, and Sipole Reference Evans, Cordova and Sipole2014; McDonald, Porter, and Treul Reference McDonald, Porter and Treul2020; Niven and Zilber Reference Niven and Zilber2001; Sapiro et al. Reference Sapiro, Walsh, Strach and Hennings2011).

Data and Methods

We test these hypotheses using evidence from television advertisements. Scholars have recently used TV ads in the study of campaign messaging and gender in part because they offer some advantages to understanding candidate self-portrayals over other media (Bauer and Santia Reference Bauer and Santia2023; English, Branton, and Friesenhahn Reference English, Branton and Friesenhahn2024). Unlike news coverage, messaging in TV ads falls under the control of candidates. Although both TV ads and campaign websites are highly accessible to the public, TV ads are more likely to be viewed by more casual observers of politics and may be more relevant to how an average voter perceives a candidate. Finally, and most importantly for this study, the short format of TV ads forces candidates to choose their priorities in messaging to the electorate. Campaign websites are constrained by space but allow candidates to provide much more information about themselves than TV ads. Therefore, TV ads allow us to observe better what “bottom-line” message a candidate wants to convey to the public. A limitation of using TV ads as data is that their production cost limits our ability to observe messaging choices from poorly funded candidates. Any results may or may not generalize to candidates with limited campaign funds.

To evaluate how candidates of both genders presented their previous experience, we watched and coded 1,030 unique TV ads for state legislative campaigns aired during the 2018 election cycle. We obtained ad data from the Wesleyan Media Project, which partners with Kantar Media/CMAG to capture television ads in each electoral cycle (Fowler et al. Reference Fowler, Franz, Ridout and Baum2020). CMAG’s automated system tracks ads on local channels, national networks, and national cable channels. It monitors and records each unique ad that airs, including its date, time, media market, station, and television show.

We chose the 2018 midterm because it produced a unique candidate pool for these state-level races. As a midterm election occurring under Donald Trump’s presidency, the 2018 election cycle saw a record number of women register to run for public office, allowing for greater variation in candidate gender and, in particular, women candidates’ background experiences. However, given the unique gender dynamics of that election cycle, it is possible that the sample of candidates in our data does not generalize to other election cycles.

Although many studies analyze the contents of congressional ads, we chose state legislative ads because the candidates they represent are usually not well known to the public. Candidates at that level must spend more time introducing themselves to voters than congressional or presidential candidates, including by discussing their biographies. Moreover, state legislative campaign spending has continued to increase over the last decade, creating an opportunity to study more widespread televised advertising among state legislative candidates.Footnote 5 Finally, thousands of Americans, both men and women, run for state legislature every year. Investigating campaign messaging at this level allows us to observe greater variation in messaging choices and to make inferences about lower-level campaigns, in contrast with studies of higher-salience but lower-frequency campaigns for statewide or national office.

To create a dataset for coding, we eliminated duplicates so that each observation is a unique ad.Footnote 6 We also limited our analysis to ads sponsored directly by the candidate’s campaign organization, removing ads sponsored by party organizations or outside groups.Footnote 7 Finally, to observe the relationship between a sponsor’s gender and message more cleanly, we removed ads jointly sponsored by multiple candidates, such as ads sponsored by copartisan candidates running in multimember districts. The resulting 1,030 ads represent all unique ads available from the Wesleyan Media Project that fit these criteria. Ads came from the campaigns of 529 different candidates, with each candidate recording between one and 20 unique ads.

We coded the ads to construct two binary dependent variables. The first, Political Experience, is intended to capture candidates’ prior service in elected office. We coded ads as mentioning political experience if the ad presented information that either states explicitly or logically implies the candidate had served in public office before. Sometimes, this came in the form of an explicit naming of a prior office they held. Other times, this came from candidates’ discussions of their votes or actions as a public official, even if they did not explicitly name the office they held.

The second dependent variable, Work Experience, is intended to capture whether the candidate discusses jobs or careers they held before running for public office. We recorded a “1” if a candidate mentions a job title or an industry in which they worked. Work experience would be counted regardless of how much focus was placed on it in the ad. For example, we counted an ad that centered the work experience in the message, as in the case of a Kentucky welder who used his work experience to frame his desire to work in a bipartisan manner in the state legislature. We also counted an ad in which the candidate primarily discussed issues but flashed a job title (e.g., “small business owner”) briefly on screen.

Two coders watched all 1,030 ads individually in their original video format in an initial round of coding. Comparing results, the coders agreed 87.5% of the time that an ad mentioned work experience (Cohen’s kappa = 0.70, signifying good reliability) and 78.7% of the time that an ad mentioned political experience (Cohen’s kappa = 0.58, signifying fair reliability). The coders then reviewed the ads on which they disagreed and made a joint final coding decision. Section A in the online appendix provides further details about the coding process, including a codebook, examples of coded ads, and results from the initial round of coding.

Overall, candidate mentions of experience were common but not omnipresent. Most ads (54.66%) mentioned either the candidate’s political or work experience. However, it was more common for ads to mention political (37.48%) than work (24.17%) experience. Few ads (only 6.99%) mentioned both political and work experience. Candidates messaged their experience quite consistently across their own ads. The average within-candidate standard deviation for Political Experience is about 0.28, and about 0.29 for Work Experience. For context, one would find a similar standard deviation with a binary variable among twelve observations if eleven observations took a value of “0” and one observation took a value of “1.”

We coded the primary independent variable, Woman Sponsor, based on the gender of the candidate sponsoring the ad. We caution that comparing differences between men and women alone has limitations, as it captures the gender identity of the candidate rather than their gender presentation. Conformity to normative social roles varies widely within genders (Diekman and Eagly Reference Diekman, Eagly, Shah and Gardner2008; Schneider and Bos Reference Schneider and Bos2019). Even within genders, gender presentation may have serious consequences for how voters interpret messages about experience and use those messages to decide their vote choice (see Teele, Kalla, and Rosenbluth Reference Teele, Kalla and Rosenbluth2018). To our knowledge, none of the candidates sponsoring ads in our dataset identified as trans, non-binary, or otherwise outside the traditional gender binary.

In the following section presenting the results, we conduct two tests of each of the two hypotheses. We first conduct simple difference of means tests to establish a baseline, bivariate relationship between candidate gender and presentation of each type of experience. Then we proceed with more rigorous tests of the hypotheses by estimating multilevel logistic regression models with covariates. Logistic regression is an appropriate choice for our binary dependent variables of interest. Because ads are the unit of analysis and one candidate could sponsor several ads, the multilevel approach helps account for the clustered structure of the data and candidate effects. We nest ads within candidates and calculate robust clustered standard errors. We note that this modelling approach cannot identify causal effects of gender on discussion of experience. Readers should treat the analysis as descriptive in nature.

Confounding factors like the varying length of the ads, the partisanship of the candidates, and the districts in which they choose to run might also help to explain differences in candidate messaging. To account for potential confounding variables, we add controls for candidate characteristics (incumbency status, prior officeholding experience, party affiliation, race/ethnicity, and campaign contributions received), ad characteristics (ad length, tone, and estimated cost), and the type of election the ad was prepared for (general, primary, or special/runoff election).Footnote 8 Table B1 in the online appendix displays descriptive statistics for all variables in the models.

We evaluate each hypothesis using different subsets of the dataset. To evaluate the first hypothesis, we must reduce the sample only to candidates with experience in elected office who could be considered “at risk” of mentioning it. No comprehensive source lists the prior elected experience of state legislative candidates. Through an online search of candidates’ webpages, state legislators’ official websites, local media reports, and election information sites (e.g., Ballotpedia), a research assistant recorded the actual officeholding background of each of the ad sponsors in our dataset. Examining the messaging only in the 614 ads sponsored by candidates who had held elected office before running for state legislature, 384 (62.54%) alluded to the sponsor’s political experience.Footnote 9 We assume all candidates have held some prior work experience and so use the full sample of ads to evaluate the second hypothesis.

Political Experience

We begin by evaluating the first hypothesis that men are more likely to mention prior political experience in ads than women. We take a first step by conducting a bivariate analysis of the frequency of experience mentions by candidate gender. Figure 1 displays the results. Among ads from prior officeholders, a statistically significant gender difference appears — 66.81% of ads from politically experienced men mention that experience compared to 48.61% ads from politically experienced women.Footnote 10 The bivariate results lend tentative support to our expectations.

Figure 1. Ads mentioning political experience by candidate gender.

Note: Data from the Wesleyan Media Project and the authors.

We proceed with a more rigorous test of the first hypothesis. We specify a multilevel logistic regression model with covariates as described in the previous section. Supporting evidence would be present if we find a negative statistically significant coefficient for the independent variable of interest, Woman Sponsor. We present the full table of regression results in the first column of Table A2 in the online appendix. In line with the descriptive findings in Figure 1, we continue to find that male-sponsored ads are more likely to discuss political experience than female-sponsored ads among experienced candidates. The difference is statistically significant at the 0.05 level of confidence.

Because it is difficult to interpret the size of a relationship from coefficients in a multilevel logistic regression alone, we illustrate the results in Figure 2 by plotting the predicted probabilities derived from the fully specified model in Table B2 in the appendix following the observed value approach (see Hanmer and Kalkan Reference Hanmer and Kalkan2013). Among men-sponsored ads, the probability of mentioning experience is 0.65, whereas for women-sponsored ads, it is 0.53. The size of the difference is meaningful — it translates to about one more ad discussing experience out of every eight.

Figure 2. Predicted probability of mentioning political experience by candidate gender.

Note: Data from the Wesleyan Media Project and the authors.

Turning to the controls, listed in the first column of Table A2 in the online appendix, we see few meaningful relationships between them and the outcome variable. The results indicate incumbents are significantly more likely to discuss experience than experienced challengers. We also find that promotional ads discuss the sponsor’s political experience more than attack ads. Finally, we see that ads are significantly less likely to mention political experience when run during special or runoff elections than when run during general elections. However, we do not see evidence that a candidate’s party, race, or fundraising level is associated with mentions of experience, nor do we see evidence that the length, cost, or volume of ads is associated with it either. Overall, results of these tests provide empirical support for the first hypothesis.

Work Experience

Next, we turn to evaluating the second hypothesis, that ads sponsored by men will be more likely to mention their work than ads sponsored by women. As before, we begin by observing the raw differences in mentions of work experience between ads sponsored by men and those sponsored by women. Figure 3 displays the findings. We see that women-sponsored ads are slightly more likely than men-sponsored ads to mention the candidate’s prior work experience (26.51% vs. 23.22%, respectively). However, the difference is small — 3.29 percentage points — and not statistically distinct from zero.

Figure 3. Ads mentioning work experience by candidate gender.

Note: Data from the Wesleyan Media Project and the authors.

We proceed with a more formal test of the second hypothesis by again specifying a multilevel logistic regression model with full controls. We add the control variable for Prior Experience to this model, omitted from the prior model because the sample was limited to ads from politically experienced candidates. Evidence supporting the second hypothesis would be present if we find a negative statistically significant coefficient for the independent variable of interest, Woman Sponsor. The second column in Table B2 in the online appendix presents the full regression results. Contrary to expectations, but consistent with the descriptive findings in Figure 3, we find no differences in discussion of work experience between men-sponsored ads and women-sponsored ads.

Figure 4 illustrates the finding. It shows the predicted probability that an ad mentions the candidate’s work experience by the sponsoring candidate’s gender, holding controls at observed values in the data. The predicted probabilities fall very close to the proportions illustrated in Figure 3. Ads sponsored by male candidates have a 0.26 likelihood of mentioning work experience, whereas ads sponsored by female candidates have a 0.23 likelihood. The difference is not statistically significant. Therefore, we fail to find support for the second hypothesis.

Figure 4. Predicted probability of mentioning work experience by candidate gender.

Notes: Data from the Wesleyan Media Project and the authors. The figure shows the predicted probability of a mention of political experience with controls held at observed values.

Among the controls listed in the second column in Table B2 in the online appendix, we find that promotional ads are significantly more likely to mention candidates’ work experience. White candidates are significantly more likely to mention their work experience than nonwhite candidates, perhaps reflecting a difference in messaging strategies from candidates of color. Incumbents and prior officeholders are significantly less likely to mention their work experience, in line with the idea that politically experienced candidates advertise their backgrounds in public office instead of their prior careers.Footnote 11 We find no statistically significant relationship between the dependent variable and any of the other controls.

The results with regard to work experience may differ depending on whether candidates hold prior political experience. The descriptive results above show that most politically experienced candidates choose to mention that experience in TV ads, but most candidates do not mention their work experience. Such results suggest that the choice to mention work experience may be conditional on whether the candidate has prior political experience. Politically experienced candidates choose to evoke that background; inexperienced candidates may opt to evoke work experience as a second-best option.

Table B3 in the online appendix shows the results when a mention of work experience is regressed on the sponsoring candidate’s gender and the full set of controls, but with results broken out among politically experienced and inexperienced candidates. In both cases, ads sponsored by women are estimated to be less likely to mention work experience than ads sponsored by men. However, the coefficient estimates are not significantly different from zero in either case. Therefore, we continue to infer that there are no meaningful differences between ads sponsored by men and women in terms of mentioning work experience among either politically experienced or inexperienced candidates.

Supplementary Analyses

In this section, we summarize a series of supplementary analyses exploring additional possibilities in the data. First, we consider whether Democratic and Republican women candidates messaged differently in their ads. We present expectations for the analysis and the findings in Section C of the online appendix. Overall, we find few differences between the two parties. Women’s ads appear less likely to mention political experience than men’s ads within both parties. The difference is more pronounced among Democrats than among Republicans, but Republican women’s ads are not significantly more likely to mention political experience than Democratic women’s ads. We continue to find no notable partisan or gender differences for ad mentions of work experience.

Second, we consider whether candidates message their experience when their opponents do so as well. Section D in the online appendix provides details and results. Here too, we find little noteworthy. Analyzing only general election ads, we find that ads are no more or less likely to mention a candidate’s political experience when at least one of their opponent’s ads mentions the opponent’s political experience. The same finding applies to mentions of candidate’s work experience.

Finally, we consider whether women with high levels of actual political experience undersell their qualifications relative to men with less actual political experience. It is difficult to measure actual experience on a single scale given the wide variety of candidates’ backgrounds. However, we can compare incumbents to nonincumbent prior officeholders. Although not universally true, incumbents usually have more experience in the offices they currently hold than challengers do. Section E in the online appendix provides the findings. We find that gender differences persist for both groups. Women’s ads discuss the candidate’s political experience less than men’s ads among both incumbents and nonincumbents. However, incumbent women’s ads emphasize experience at a higher rate than nonincumbent men’s ads, suggesting that the actual level of experience is related to a candidate’s likelihood of advertising their experience despite gender differences.

Discussion

Gender appears to be related to the likelihood that candidates advertise their prior political experience in TV ads. Among ads sponsored by prior officeholders, men’s ads were more likely to discuss their sponsors’ political experience than women’s even after controlling for potential confounders like the incumbency status of the candidate and the type of election. Interestingly, neither the party of the sponsoring candidate nor the messaging strategy of the candidate’s opponent appears related to the choice to emphasize political experience. At the same time, we find no evidence that women are more or less likely to talk about their work experience than men in televised campaign ads.

The results add a wrinkle to the field’s understanding of gender and self-presentation of experience. This study contributes to the literature by assessing gender differences in self-presentation of experience in a novel medium (TV ads rather than websites) and at a different level of office (state rather than national) than prior studies. The most direct prior evidence on communicating experience comes from Bauer (Reference Bauer2020), who finds that US Senate candidates of both genders are equally likely to discuss their political experience on campaign websites, but that women are more likely to highlight their professional experience. Likewise, McDonald, Porter, and Treul (Reference McDonald, Porter and Treul2020) find that, among Democrats, experienced women are no more likely than experienced men to highlight their experience on congressional campaign websites. However, websites allow for more expansive communication about a candidate than 30-second ads and often mimic one another in terms of the types of content provided. The results from the current study suggest that, in campaign communications where time constraints force candidates to consider trade-offs in which traits and topics they think most important to convey to a mass audience, women candidates may be more likely to sacrifice discussion of their experience than men.

This study also contributes to our broader understanding of experience beyond that in elected office — how candidates explain their work histories to voters. Although a growing body of research, including the present study, examines messaging to illuminate how voters evaluate women’s qualifications for office, few studies have examined media coverage and campaign messaging of candidates’ occupational backgrounds for its own sake (see Carnes Reference Carnes, Cameron, Canes-Wrone, Gordon and Huber2023; McDermott Reference McDermott1999). Future research in this area would help us understand the messages voters are receiving about who is qualified to run for office based on their work backgrounds.

The analysis holds several limitations. The evidence is purely descriptive; we do not claim causality with any analysis above. We focus solely on the choice to mention prior experience. As a result, the analysis does not explore the context or tone with which that experience is mentioned. We also treat all work and political experience as equal. Future research may distinguish between types of work experience (e.g., “pipeline professions” like law vs. other types of work) or types of political experience (e.g., school board vs. mayor).

We caution that these TV ads come from a rather small number of state legislative candidates. The cost of state legislative campaigns varies dramatically nationwide. The traits advertised on TV in this sample may not be representative of the traits state legislative candidates nationwide would advertise with sufficient funding. Candidates with the money to air TV ads might disproportionately hold prior political experience and differ qualitatively from poorly funded candidates. Likewise, it is unclear that the results here would generalize to campaign ads for higher-level offices, where we might expect candidates to have even greater qualifications and campaign resources at their disposal. Although we claim that the medium might help explain differences between our results and prior findings (Bauer Reference Bauer2020; McDonald, Porter, and Treul Reference McDonald, Porter and Treul2020), we cannot rule out that differences in campaigns across levels of government (or some other reason entirely) explain our divergent findings instead.

Though we posit gender norms surrounding agency and power-seeking goals as a theoretical explanation, the precise mechanism remains murky from an analysis of TV ads alone. The results from this specific analysis of 2018 campaign ads could be a short-term reaction to recent political events; the period of observation occurred immediately after Hillary Clinton’s failed presidential campaign, which strongly emphasized her experience. However, critics hounded Clinton with allegations of unseemly ambition and power-seeking behavior. Women candidates in 2018 may have worked to avoid a replay of Clinton’s campaign in a way that women candidates more distant from the 2016 presidential election will not.

Nonetheless, the results suggest that women candidates face a double bind. Women are more likely than men to wait to run until they gain lower-level elected experience before running and are more cautious about running for higher office once elected (Brown et al. Reference Brown, Mansour, O’Connell and Reeves2019; Ondercin Reference Ondercin2022; Pearson and McGhee Reference Pearson and McGhee2013). Yet at the same time, women candidates appear less likely than men to make that political experience a salient part of their campaign on the airwaves, perhaps in expectation of voter backlash. This pattern suggests that women candidates feel constrained in capitalizing politically on their hard-won experience. They may also need to expend additional effort and resources in campaigns to fine-tune messaging that portrays them as competent without running afoul of gendered expectations.

The implications of the findings for electoral outcomes are unclear. On one hand, experimental evidence suggests that a greater emphasis on experience in messaging would help voters to see women candidates as more competent, perhaps increasing their electoral support (Bauer Reference Bauer2020; Holman, Merolla, and Zechmeister Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2017). On the other hand, in polarized times, increasing messaging around experience may do little to change electoral outcomes for women who have already chosen to run.

However, the findings do have important implications for discourse surrounding political candidacy. Elite messaging choices shape the way that ordinary citizens understand politics. When politicians frame messages to appeal to some audiences, it denies other audiences the opportunity to influence how their fellow citizens understand politics (Pottle Reference Pottle2023). In the context of race, for instance, candidate distancing from racial minorities helps maintain inattention to their political needs (Stephens-Dougan Reference Stephens-Dougan2020). If women candidates decline to emphasize their experience, particularly in media like TV ads that reach voters who are less tuned into politics, voters have little encouragement to update preconceived notions about who is fit to serve in office. A gender imbalance in how candidates discuss their experience publicly helps preserve voters’ and political insiders’ perceptions of who makes a viable candidate, which itself has implications for the overall representation of women in office.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S1743923X24000370.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Gabriela Granados and Roisin O’Carroll for research assistance as well as Mirya Holman, participants at the 2022 State Politics & Policy Conference, and the anonymous reviewers for valuable feedback. Any errors remain the responsibility of the authors.

Competing interest

The authors declare none.

Footnotes

1. Although there are many reasons a candidate might decline to mention experience, one possible reason is so that a challenger can take on the mantle of an outsider, a rhetorical device that resonates strongly with voters in contemporary US politics. In this case, highlighting Davis’s tenure in the statehouse would have muddled her claim elsewhere in the same ad that she was “sick of the old Harrisburg politics.”

2. The data were obtained from the Wesleyan Media Project, a collaboration between Wesleyan University, Bowdoin College, and Washington State University, and includes media tracking data from Kantar/Campaign Media Analysis Group in Washington, DC. The Wesleyan Media Project was sponsored in 2018 by a grant from The John S. and James L. Knight Foundation. The opinions expressed in this article are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Wesleyan Media Project, Knight Foundation, or any of its affiliates.

3. Partially complementing these theories, expectancy violations theory holds that individuals evoke extreme reactions when they engage in behaviors that do not fit stereotypes of their groups (Bettencourt et al. Reference Bettencourt, Dill, Greathouse, Charlton and Mulholland1997; Burgoon Reference Burgoon, Berger and Roloff2015; Jussim, Coleman, and Lerch Reference Jussim, Coleman and Lerch1987). Applied to politics, women candidates might incur penalties from voters for participating in political leadership, a stereotypically masculine activity (Cassese and Holman Reference Cassese and Holman2018). However, expectancy violations theory may also lead to a prediction that voters regard women more positively for violating gender norms. Closeness to or warmness toward the target before the norm violation occurs will predict whether the reaction will be positive or negative (Burgoon Reference Burgoon, Berger and Roloff2015).

4. More generally, researchers have arrived to mixed results on whether communicating experience improves candidates’ odds of victories irrespective of gender (Fridkin and Kenney Reference Fridkin and Kenney2011; Hansen and Treul Reference Hansen and Treul2021; Kirkland and Coppock Reference Kirkland and Coppock2018). Even though prior political experience is a strong predictor of a higher vote share, experience advantages more likely stem from these candidates’ advantages in campaigning, such as superior fundraising ability and strategic selection into winnable races (Bonica Reference Bonica2020; Jacobson Reference Jacobson1989; Maestas and Rugeley Reference Maestas and Rugeley2008) than from an explicit voter preference for experience.

5. Montemayor, Stacy, Pete Quist, Karl Evers-Hillstrom, and Douglas Weber. “Joint Report Reveals Record Donations in 2020 State and Federal Races.” National Institution of Money in Politics and Center for Responsive Politics. November 19, 2020. https://www.followthemoney.org/research/institute-reports/joint-report-reveals-record-donations-in-2020-state-and-federal-races.

6. This choice allows us to compare the distinct messages the candidates send to voters through ads but could bias the analysis if candidates run ads with different frequencies. For example, if a candidate sponsors one ad emphasizing their position on abortion and a second ad emphasizing their prior experience, the ads would be equally weighted in our dataset even if the abortion ad appears 10 times on television and the experience appears 200 times. However, we later estimate models weighting for the frequency of ad appearances and reach the same set of conclusions.

7. Though outside groups share a goal of electing the candidate and may even coordinate directly with the campaign, outside groups also have incentives to highlight different facets of the candidate. For example, a pro-life advocacy group would have more incentive to spotlight a candidate’s pro-life positions than her pre-candidacy experience. In contrast, candidates have greater incentive to present themselves as individuals with unique and compelling sets of experiences and traits that merit their election to office.

8. Prior Office is a binary variable with a value of 1, indicating the candidate is an incumbent or held any prior elected office (e.g., mayor). Incumbent is a binary variable with a value of 1, indicating ads sponsored by incumbents and 0 indicating ads sponsored by challengers. Democrat is a binary variable with a value of 1, indicating Democratic affiliation and 0 indicating any other party. White is a binary variable with a value of 1, indicating that the candidate identifies as white and 0 indicating any other race/ethnicity. (Because 87% of the ads in the data were sponsored by white candidates, a small sample size precludes using a richer set of measures of candidate race/ethnicity.) Data come from Shah et al. (Reference Shah, Juenke and Fraga2022). Campaign Contributions are measured in $100,000 and gathered from the Follow the Money (previously the National Institute of Money in State Politics.) The remaining variables come from the Wesleyan Media Project. Ad Length is measured in seconds. Promotional Ad is a binary variable taking a value of 1 if the ad promotes a positive image of the sponsoring candidate and 0 if the ad attacks the sponsor’s opponent or contrasts the two candidates. Estimated Ad Cost is measured in $1,000. Primary and Special/Runoff are binary variables taking a value of 1 if the ad appeared as part of a campaign in each type of election, with general election ads serving as the reference category of 0.

9. Two ads were coded as referring to the sponsor’s political experience when the candidate had never held elected office. These ads were sponsored by a longtime state legislative aide and an appointed city administrator (both men), candidates who could reasonably make claims about their experience in a political arena without having been elected to office.

10. A corollary of our first hypothesis is that men highlight their inexperience more than women. We conducted a second round of coding among the ads that did not mention political experience to identify the ads in which candidates affirmatively said they lacked prior experience in elected office. However, we identified only one ad where this occurred — a Missouri State Senate candidate said, “I’ve never run for office.” We interpret this finding to mean that, although women candidates might expect self-promotion of their qualifications to cause a backlash, candidates of both genders avoid self-effacing statements about their qualifications.

11. An ad was more likely to substitute a mention of its sponsor’s work experience if the candidate lacked political experience. For candidates with no political experience, 149 ads (35.82%) mentioned the sponsor’s work experience, compared with 100 similar ads (16.29%) from politically experienced candidates.

References

Bauer, Nichole M. 2017. “The Effects of Counterstereotypic Gender Strategies on Candidate Evaluations.” Political Psychology 38 (2): 279–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nichole M. 2020. The Qualifications Gap: Why Women Must Be Better than Men to Win Political Office. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, Nichole M., and Santia, Martina. 2023. “Gendered Times: How Gendered Contexts Shape Campaign Messages of Female Candidates.” Journal of Communication 73 (4): 329–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhard, Rachel, Shames, Shauna, and Teele, Dawn Langan. 2021. “To Emerge? Breadwinning, Motherhood, and Women’s Decisions to Run for Office.” American Political Science Review 115 (2): 379–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bettencourt, B. Ann, Dill, Karen E., Greathouse, Scott A., Charlton, Kelly, and Mulholland, Amy. 1997. “Evaluations of Ingroup and Outgroup Members: The Role of Category-Based Expectancy Violation.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 33 (3): 244–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonica, Adam. 2020. “Why Are There So Many Lawyers in Congress?Legislative Studies Quarterly 45 (2): 253–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Ryan, Mansour, Hani, O’Connell, Stephen D., and Reeves, James. 2019. “Gender Differences in Political Career Progression: Evidence from U.S. Elections.” CESifo Working Paper no. 7821. https://www.econstor.eu/bitstream/10419/207212/1/cesifo1_wp7821.pdf.Google Scholar
Burgoon, Judee K. 2015. “Expectancy Violations Theory.” In The International Encyclopedia of Interpersonal Communication, eds. Berger, Charles R. and Roloff, Michael E., 19. Wiley Online. doi:10.1002/9781118540190.wbeic102.Google Scholar
Campbell, David E., and Wolbrecht, Christina. 2006. “See Jane Run: Women Politicians as Role Models for Adolescents.” Journal of Politics 68 (2): 233–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carnes, Nicholas. 2023. “Inequality, or Invisibility and Inaccuracy? How Local Newspapers Cover the Occupational Backgrounds of Members of Congress.” In Accountability Reconsidered: Voters, Interests, and Information in US Policymaking, eds. Cameron, Charles M., Canes-Wrone, Brandice, Gordon, Sanford C., and Huber, Gregory A., 150–72. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cassese, Erin C., and Holman, Mirya R.. 2018. “Party and Gender Stereotypes in Campaign Attacks.” Political Behavior 40 (3): 785807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Center for American Women and Politics. 2022. “Current Numbers.” New Brunswick, NJ: Center for American Women and Politics, Eagleton Institute of Politics, Rutgers University-New Brunswick. April 15, 2022. https://cawp.rutgers.edu/facts/current-numbers.Google Scholar
Conroy, Meredith, and Green, Jon. 2020. “It Takes a Motive: Communal and Agentic Articulated Interest and Candidate Emergence.” Political Research Quarterly 73 (4): 942–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diekman, Amanda B, and Eagly, Alice H. 2008. “Of Men, Women, and Motivation: A Role Congruity Account.” In Handbook of Motivation Science, eds. Shah, James Y. and Gardner, Wendi L., 434–47. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Ditonto, Tessa M., Hamilton, Allison J., and Redlawsk, David P.. 2014. “Gender Stereotypes, Information Search, and Voting Behavior in Political Campaigns.” Political Behavior 36(2): 335–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dittmar, Kelly. 2015. Navigating Gendered Terrain: Stereotypes and Strategy in Political Campaigns. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Dolan, Kathleen. 2005. “Do Women Candidates Play to Gender Stereotypes? Do Men Candidates Play to Women? Candidate Sex and Issues Priorities on Campaign Websites.” Political Research Quarterly 58 (1): 3144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagly, Alice H., and Karau, Steven J.. 2002. “Role Congruity Theory of Prejudice toward Female Leaders.” Psychological Review 109 (3): 573–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eagly, Alice H., and Wood, Wendy. 2012. “Social Role Theory.” In Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology, eds. Paul A. M. Van Lange, Kruglanski, Arie W., and Higgins, E. Tory, 458–76. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
English, Ashley, Branton, Regina, and Friesenhahn, Amy. 2024. “Outside of the Old Boys Club? Gender Differences in Outside Groups’ Advertising Support for U.S. Senate Candidates.” Political Research Quarterly 77 (1): 328–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, Heather K., Cordova, Victoria, and Sipole, Savannah. 2014. “Twitter Style: An Analysis of How House Candidates Used Twitter in Their 2012 Campaigns.” Political Science & Politics 47 (2): 454–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, Erika Franklin, Franz, Michael M., Ridout, Travis N., and Baum, Laura N.. 2020. Political Advertising in 2018. Middletown, CT: The Wesleyan Media Project, Department of Government at Wesleyan University.Google Scholar
Fox, Richard L., and Lawless, Jennifer L.. 2004. “Entering the Arena? Gender and the Decision to Run for Office.” American Journal of Political Science 48 (2): 264–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Richard L., and Lawless, Jennifer L.. 2005. “To Run or Not to Run for Office: Explaining Nascent Political Ambition.” American Journal of Political Science 49 (3): 659–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Richard L., and Lawless, Jennifer L.. 2010. “If Only They’d Ask: Gender, Recruitment, and Political Ambition.” Journal of Politics 72 (2): 310–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, Richard L., and Oxley, Zoe M.. 2003. “Gender Stereotyping in State Executive Elections: Candidate Selection and Success.” Journal of Politics 65 (3): 833–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridkin, Kim L., and Kenney, Patrick J.. 2011. “The Role of Candidate Traits in Campaigns.” Journal of Politics 73 (1): 6173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fulton, Sarah A. 2012. “Running Backwards and in High Heels: The Gendered Quality Gap and Incumbent Electoral Success.” Political Research Quarterly 65 (2): 303–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fulton, Sarah A., Maestas, Cherie D., Maisel, L. Sandy, and Stone, Walter J.. 2006. “The Sense of a Woman: Gender, Ambition, and the Decision to Run for Congress.” Political Research Quarterly 59 (2): 235–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanmer, Michael J., and Kalkan, Kerem Ozan. 2013. “Behind the Curve: Clarifying the Best Approach to Calculating Predicted Probabilities and Marginal Effects from Limited Dependent Variable Models.” American Journal of Political Science 57 (1): 263–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, Eric R., and Treul, Sarah A.. 2021. “Inexperienced or Anti-establishment? Voter Preferences for Outsider Congressional Candidates.” Research & Politics 8 (3). https://doi.org/10.1177/20531680211034958.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, Danny, and Lawless, Jennifer L.. 2016. Women on the Run: Gender, Media, and Political Campaigns in a Polarized Era. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hershey, Marjorie Randon. 1974. The Making of Campaign Strategy. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Holman, Mirya R., Merolla, Jennifer L., and Zechmeister, Elizabeth J.. 2016. “Terrorist Threat, Male Stereotypes, and Candidate Evaluations.” Political Research Quarterly 69 (1): 134–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holman, Mirya R., Merolla, Jennifer L., and Zechmeister, Elizabeth J.. 2017. “Can Experience Overcome Stereotypes in Times of Terror Threat?Research & Politics 4 (1). https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168016688121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddy, Leonie, and Terkildsen, Nayda. 1993. “The Consequences of Gender Stereotypes for Women Candidates at Different Levels and Types of Office.” Political Research Quarterly 46 (3): 503–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, Gary C. 1989. “Strategic Politicians and the Dynamics of U.S. House Elections, 1946-86.” American Political Science Review 83 (3): 773–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jussim, Lee, Coleman, Lerita M, and Lerch, Lauren. 1987. “The Nature of Stereotypes: A Comparison and Integration of Three Theories.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 (3): 536–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirkland, Patricia A., and Coppock, Alexander. 2018. “Candidate Choice Without Party Labels.” Political Behavior 40 (3): 571–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladam, Christina, Harden, Jeffrey J., and Windett, Jason H.. 2018. “Prominent Role Models: High-Profile Female Politicians and the Emergence of Women as Candidates for Public Office.” American Journal of Political Science 62 (2): 369–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawless, Jennifer L. 2015. “Female Candidates and Legislators.” Annual Review of Political Science 18: 349–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maestas, Cherie, and Rugeley, Cynthia. 2008. “Assessing the ‘Experience Bonus’ through Examining Strategic Entry, Candidate Quality, and Campaign Receipts in U.S. House Elections.” American Journal of Political Science 52 (3): 520–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDermott, Monika L. 1999. “Shortcut Voting: Candidate Characteristics and Voter Inference.” PhD diss. University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
McDonald, Jared, Karol, David, and Mason, Lilliana. 2020. “‘An Inherited Money Dude from Queens County’: How Unseen Candidate Characteristics Affect Voter Perceptions.” Political Behavior 42 (3): 915–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, Jared, and Piatak, Jaclyn. 2022. “Penalties for Going against Type: How Sexism Shapes Voters’ Perceptions of Candidate Character.” Behavioral Science & Policy 8 (2): 4756.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, Maura, Porter, Rachel, and Treul, Sarah A.. 2020. “Running as a Woman? Candidate Presentation in the 2018 Midterms.” Political Research Quarterly 73 (4): 967–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miler, Kristina C. 2010. Constituency Representation in Congress: The View from Capitol Hill. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mo, Cecilia Hyunjung. 2015. “The Consequences of Explicit and Implicit Gender Attitudes and Candidate Quality in the Calculations of Voters.” Political Behavior 37 (2): 357–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niven, David, and Zilber, Jeremy. 2001. “Do Women and Men in Congress Cultivate Different Images? Evidence from Congressional Web Sites.” Political Communication 18 (4): 395405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ondercin, Heather L. 2022. “Location, Location, Location: How Electoral Opportunities Shape Women’s Emergence as Candidates.” British Journal of Political Science 52 (4): 1523–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, Kathryn, and McGhee, Eric. 2013. “What It Takes to Win: Questioning ‘Gender Neutral’ Outcomes in U.S. House Elections.” Politics & Gender 9 (4): 439–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pottle, Justin. 2023. “Epistemic Injustice and the Electoral Connection.” American Journal of Political Science. Early View. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajps.12806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, Laurie A. 1998. “Self-Promotion as a Risk Factor for Women: The Costs and Benefits of Counterstereotypical Impression Management.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 (3): 629–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanbonmatsu, Kira. 2002. “Political Parties and the Recruitment of Women to State Legislatures.” Journal of Politics 64 (3): 791809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapiro, Virginia, Walsh, Katherine Cramer, Strach, Patricia, and Hennings, Valerie. 2011. “Gender, Context, and Television Advertising: A Comprehensive Analysis of 2000 and 2002 House Races.” Political Research Quarterly 64 (1): 107–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, Monica C., and Bos, Angela L.. 2019. “The Application of Social Role Theory to the Study of Gender in Politics.” Political Psychology 40 (S1): 173213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, Monica C., Holman, Mirya R., Diekman, Amanda B., and McAndrew, Thomas. 2016. “Power, Conflict, and Community: How Gendered Views of Political Power Influence Women’s Political Ambition.” Political Psychology 37 (4): 515–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, Suzanne, and Coppock, Alexander. 2022. “What Have We Learned about Gender from Candidate Choice Experiments? A Meta-Analysis of Sixty-Seven Factorial Survey Experiments.” Journal of Politics 84 (2): 655–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shah, Paru R., Juenke, Eric Gonzalez, and Fraga, Bernard L.. 2022. “Here Comes Everybody: Using a Data Cooperative to Understand the New Dynamics of Representation.” PS: Political Science & Politics 55 (2): 300302. doi: 10.1017/S1049096521001542.Google Scholar
Stephens-Dougan, LaFleur. 2020. Race to the Bottom: How Racial Appeals Work in American Politics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teele, Dawn Langan, Kalla, Joshua, and Rosenbluth, Frances. 2018. “The Ties That Double Bind: Social Roles and Women’s Underrepresentation in Politics .” American Political Science Review 112 (3): 525–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Ads mentioning political experience by candidate gender.Note: Data from the Wesleyan Media Project and the authors.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Predicted probability of mentioning political experience by candidate gender.Note: Data from the Wesleyan Media Project and the authors.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Ads mentioning work experience by candidate gender.Note: Data from the Wesleyan Media Project and the authors.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Predicted probability of mentioning work experience by candidate gender.Notes: Data from the Wesleyan Media Project and the authors. The figure shows the predicted probability of a mention of political experience with controls held at observed values.

Supplementary material: File

Hansen and Mautner supplementary material

Hansen and Mautner supplementary material
Download Hansen and Mautner supplementary material(File)
File 1.4 MB