Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:34:46.847Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Affective partisan polarization and moral dilemmas during the COVID-19 pandemic

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 June 2022

Lukas F. Stoetzer*
Affiliation:
Humboldt University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany
Simon Munzert
Affiliation:
Hertie School, Berlin, Germany
Will Lowe
Affiliation:
Hertie School, Berlin, Germany
Başak Çalı
Affiliation:
Hertie School, Berlin, Germany
Anita R. Gohdes
Affiliation:
Hertie School, Berlin, Germany
Marc Helbling
Affiliation:
University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
Rahsaan Maxwell
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, USA
Richard Traunmüller
Affiliation:
University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Recent scholarship on affective polarization documents partisan animosity in people's everyday lives. But does partisan dislike go so far as to deny fundamental rights? We study this question through a moral dilemma that gained notoriety during the COVID-19 pandemic: triage decisions on the allocation of intensive medical care. Using a conjoint experiment in five countries we analyze the influence of patients’ partisanship next to commonly discussed factors determining access to intensive medical care. We find that while participants’ choices are consistent with a utilitarian heuristic, revealed partisanship influences decisions across most countries. Supporters of left or right political camps are more likely to withhold support from partisan opponents. Our findings offer comparative evidence on affective polarization in non-political contexts.

Type
Research Note
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is included and the original work is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the European Political Science Association

Affective partisan polarization is on the rise. Scholars first documented affective partisan polarization in the US (Iyengar et al., Reference Iyengar, Sood and Lelkes2012; Doherty et al., Reference Doherty, Kiley and Asheer2019), but similar trends exist in countries across the world (Westwood et al., Reference Westwood, Iyengar, Walgrave, Leonisio, Miller and Strijbis2018; Boxell et al., Reference Boxell, Gentzkow and Shapiro2020; Helbling and Jungkunz, Reference Helbling and Jungkunz2020; Reiljan, Reference Reiljan2020; Wagner, Reference Wagner2021). These trends are worrisome in light of recent scholarship indicating that affective partisanship has divisive implications for many political and nonpolitical situations. The core idea is that partisan group identities trigger in-group and out-group thinking (Tajfel et al., Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979) that can extend beyond politics. For example, in-group/out-group divides are influential in dating behavior (Iyengar et al., Reference Iyengar, Sood and Lelkes2012; Huber and Malhotra, Reference Huber and Malhotra2017), economic activities (Gift and Gift, Reference Gift and Gift2015; Michelitch, Reference Michelitch2015; McConnell et al., Reference McConnell, Margalit, Malhotra and Levendusky2018), as well as personal and professional health decisions (Hersh and Goldenberg, Reference Hersh and Goldenberg2016; Lerman et al., Reference Lerman, Sadin and Trachtman2017).

The broad ramifications of affective partisanship raise questions about how far its effects may extend. Taken to the extreme, affective partisanship degrades and denies fundamental rights to the out-party, which can potentially have lethal consequences (Kalmoe and Mason, Reference Kalmoe and Mason2019). However, measuring such extreme dynamics is difficult, because there are not many situations in which people's chance of surviving is directly and publicly debated.

In this research note, we measure affective partisanship in an extreme scenario, by studying a fundamental rights dilemma that gained notoriety during the COVID-19 pandemic: the triage for access to intensive medical care. We conducted conjoint experiments in five countries (Brazil, Germany, Italy, Poland, and the United States) in August 2020. We presented respondents with the attributes of two hypothetical patients and asked which patient they would prioritize for intensive care.Footnote 1

We find that the partisanship of the patient plays a measurable role in respondents’ triage decisions. While respondents mainly rely on characteristics that are in the guidelines of medical associations, out-party animus also influences their priorities. Partisans on both the left and right are likely to withhold medical support from patients on the other political side. Moreover, right-party patients are more heavily penalized. We also find some evidence of in-group favoritism. Right respondents prioritize right partisan over independent patients.

This research note expands our understanding of the extreme ramifications of partisan polarization. Medical associations have clear guidelines for triage decisions and most agree on “the role of justice and benefit maximization as core principles” (Jöbges et al., Reference Jöbges, Vinay, Luyckx and Biller-Andorno2020, p. 948).Footnote 2 Indeed, recent studies show that public opinion seems to side with the utilitarian core of the guidelines (Kneer and Hannikainen, Reference Kneer and Hannikainen2020). Given the prominence of these utilitarian concerns, medical triage is a least likely case for discrimination along partisan lines. Our findings of partisan discrimination are therefore all the more striking. Previous research finds that partisans are willing to dehumanize members of the out-party (Martherus et al., Reference Martherus, Martinez, Piff and Theodoridis2021) and to rationalize physical harm against opponents (Kalmoe and Mason, Reference Kalmoe and Mason2019), all of which has worrisome implications. Our study adds to this line of research by measuring out-group animus in an extreme non-political setting, and expanding our understanding of partisan affective polarization. Moreover, this is one of the first empirical studies to analyze non-political consequences of affective partisanship in a comparative perspective, suggesting these are broad dynamics that exist across the globe.

1. Affective partisan polarization during the COVID-19 pandemic

The COVID-19 pandemic provides a unique opportunity to study cross-national differences in consequences of affective partisanship. The pandemic is a global problem that appeared on the social and political agenda around the world. In addition, several studies have found clear partisan divides in beliefs about the risks and development of the pandemic (Allcott et al., Reference Allcott, Boxell, Conway, Gentzkow, Thaler and Yang2020; Bhanot and Hopkins, Reference Bhanot and Hopkins2020), which suggests partisan divides might play a role in how people evaluate pandemic-related issues. Yet, partisan responses to the pandemic are not uniform across countries. In the US, the Republican party has largely opposed strict lockdown measures, and studies suggest Republicans have been less likely to comply with social distancing measures (Clinton et al., Reference Clinton, Cohen, Lapinski and Trussler2021; Gadarian et al., Reference Gadarian, Goodman and Pepinsky2021). In contrast, Poland's right-wing PiS has supported strict lockdown measures (see supplementary material (SM) 2.3.2). The COVID-19 pandemic is an opportunity to examine how affective partisanship may have different implications in different national contexts.

The COVID-19 pandemic also raised acute moral dilemmas about who deserves access to life-saving medical care. Providing life-saving support to all patients is a moral imperative, but it can be unattainable because of limited medical resources. Throughout the COVID-19 pandemic, all countries debated how to handle potential medical resource shortages, which made the issue of how to triage both prominent and salient. This allows us to study extreme partisan polarization in a realistic real-world scenario.

Clearly, most people will never need to make medical triage decisions because they are not qualified medical professionals. However, we pose our survey questions as measures of moral judgments. In that sense, the real-world scenario of medical care during COVID-19 is a more realistic and accessible scenario than, for example, the question of whether to divert a trolley from killing people, which is extensively used by psychologists to study moral judgments (Greene et al., Reference Greene, Sommerville, Nystrom, Darley and Cohen2001).

2. A conjoint experiment on triage decisions

Case selection and samples

We conducted a conjoint experiment in five countries (Brazil, Germany, Italy, Poland, and the US) in August 2020. The country selection is motivated by four interrelated factors: infection trajectory, party system, incumbent party, and policy responses toward the pandemic. At the time of our data collection, the five countries had different trajectories and experiences with COVID-19. The numbers of infected and deaths were relatively high in Italy, the US, and Brazil, while Germany and Poland were much less affected. Second, we compare the two-party system of the US to the multi-party systems in the other countries. Third, during our study period the governing party in the US, Brazil, and Poland had a right-wing leadership, whereas Germany and Italy were governed by centrist coalitions. Fourth, we observe the policy stance toward COVID-19, where our sample displays variation with respect to position of right parties (see SM 2.3.2). The survey sample includes 6415 individuals from the five countries, recruited from a commercial access panel using quotas to reflect the age, gender, and education distribution of the adult population.Footnote 3

Conjoint experiment and attribute dimensions

The task is framed by the notion that an increasing number of COVID-19 cases could overwhelm the healthcare system and medical staff would have to decide which patients should receive intensive medical care. It then presents respondents with two patient profiles and asks them which of the two they would prioritize.Footnote 4 The patients’ profiles include age (at four different levels: 21, 42, 61, and 76), gender (female and male), whether the patient has children or not, occupation (unemployed, cook, professor, physician, and nurse), arrival at the hospital (first, second, at the same time), the survival chance of the patient (20, 50, and 80percent), and the partisanship of the patient (left partisan, right partisan, none). Respondents see four patient pairs. The attribute levels are randomized, except for the arrival at the hospital.Footnote 5 The order of the attributes is randomized over respondents but kept constant for the four decisions.

The survival chance of the patient most directly aligns with utilitarian ethical guidelines, according to which patients with higher survival chances should be prioritized.Footnote 6 Other aspects are in some of the guidelines: the age of the respondent (younger patients should be prioritized), the job of the patient (patients with “system-relevant” jobs are prioritized), children, and the arrival at the hospital (“first-come-first-serve” principal). Based on these, we derive hypotheses of how the patient profiles influence respondents’ priorities. The full list of hypotheses is described in the pre-analysis plan (see SM 6).

The attribute that we use to test the implication of the affective partisan polarization theory is the partisanship of the patient. We choose different partisan attributes in the five countries. In the bi-partisan system of the US, we use Republican, Democrat, and none. In other contexts, in addition to a non-partisan level we selected parties that stand on the left and the right of the political spectrum. For Germany, we choose the Alternative for Germany (AfD) and the Alliance90/The Greens. For Poland, the Law and Justice Party (PiS) and the Civic Platform (PO). For Brazil, the Social Liberal Party (PSL, Bolsonaro) and the Workers’ Party (PT, Lula), referring to the presidential candidates. For Italy, the Lega and Italia Viva. The supplementary material provides more details on the selection and information about the ideological and lock-down positions of the parties (SM 2.3).

The effects of the partisan attributes of the patients should be pronounced among respondents who are affectively polarized. In line with previous research (see e.g. Druckman et al., Reference Druckman, Klar, Krupnikov, Levendusky and Ryan2020; Wagner, Reference Wagner2021; Harteveld, Reference Harteveld2021), we measure affective polarization using rating-scores for the different parties. The survey contains a battery of 11-pt. rating scores for the different parties. The difference between the rating scores for the left party and the right party serves as our measure.Footnote 7 We also describe the results when using a partisan identification question instead of the rating score-difference in SM 5.4.

To evaluate the effect of patient attributes on respondents’ priorities, we present the marginal means (MM) and the average marginal component (interaction) effects (AMCIE) of the prioritization for intensive care. We present results country-wise and pooled, combining all respondents (weighting for sample size). We estimate the MM and AMCIE using linear regression models and clustered standard errors.

3. Results

Figure 1 shows the marginal means for the different attributes in the five countries, and the sample size-weighted pooled effects. The results reveal that respondents’ decisions broadly align with the medical guidelines. First, the core criteria of the guidelines—the chance of survival—plays a central role in the decisions. With 54% selection probability a patient with an 80percent survival chance has a 10pp (percentge points) higher chance of being prioritized compared to the 44% selection probability of a patient with 20percent chance of survival. This difference is particularly pronounced in Germany (23pp), Italy (14pp), and Poland (10pp). Second, the age of the patient is crucial. Respondents generally prioritize young patients: they prioritize patients with an age of 76 only 41% of the times, but young patients with an age of 26 56% of times. In Italy, this difference is particularly large with 27pp. Third, respondents prioritize patients with system-relevant jobs but the effects are relatively small.Footnote 8 Nurses and physicians roughly have a 6pp higher chance compared to cooks. Fourth, respondents prioritize patients with children (45% versus 54%) and slightly favor female patients (51%). Finally, arriving second at the hospital decreases the selection chances from 55% to 45%, with particularly strong differences in the US (17pp).

Figure. 1. Marginal means of prioritizing patient for intensive medical care in the five country and combined samples.

The results further show that when averaging across all five countries, the partisanship of the patient has a small influence on the priorities of the respondents. Across all countries, respondents prioritize a patient with no partisanship with 53%, left partisans with 50%, and right partisans with 46%. The difference varies between the country samples. While there is no difference in the US sample, this pattern is more pronounced in Germany, Italy, and Poland. In Germany, a AfD patient is prioritized only 43% of the times. It has to be pointed out that given the varying context, size, tradition, and supporter group of the right and left parties in our sample, these average differences are difficult to compare across context. However, the results show that compared to the other differences described above, patients’ partisanship is less relevant on average. When asking respondents in an open question format what attributes should matter, only 1–2.5% name partisanship. A majority names survival chances as a important feature (see SM 5.6). Furthermore, an analysis of attribute importance using random forests in the supplementary materials reveals that age, children, and the survival chance seem to matter most (see SM 5.7).

To evaluate the partisan affective polarization in triage decisions, we present the average marginal component interaction effects conditional on the difference in rating scores. Figure 2 shows the effects among respondents with low, middle, and high rating score difference between the respective right and left party.Footnote 9 The panel in the top left reveals out-party animus. Affectively polarized left respondents, who give a low evaluation of the right parties and a high evaluation of the left, downgrade patients with a right partisanship by 16pp compared to patients with no partisanship. The same left respondents do not prioritize patients with a left partisanship. For affectively polarized right respondents we find they are 5pp less likely to prioritize left partisan patients but 5pp more likely to prioritize patients with right partisanship compared to independent patients. Respondents in the middle slightly prioritize patients with no partisan attachments (52%) to both left (49%) and right partisan (48%) patients, resulting in ACMIE of −3pp for the left and −5pp for the right. The pattern could be due to the fact that independents dislike partisans and therefore downgrade both groups (Klar et al., Reference Klar, Krupnikov and Barry Ryan2018).

Figure. 2. Average marginal component effects of triage patient's partisan affiliation conditional on respondent's affective polarization.

The country results mimic these general patterns. In the US, respondents with high ratings of the Democratic party and low ratings of the Republican party downgrade Republican patients with 10pp. We find in-group favoritism among Republicans, with polarized Republican supporters prioritizing Republican patients (7pp) compared to non-patients. There is no clear difference between polarized Republican supporters when comparing Democratic patients to patients with no party affiliation. In other country contexts, out-party animus toward the right partisan patients is even more pronounced. In Poland, Brazil, and Germany, left-polarized respondents demote patients with right partisanship with 15, 17, and 20pp. Right-polarized respondents also downgrade left patients in these countries (with 5pp in Brazil, 9pp in Poland, and 10pp in Germany), with the extend of the out-party animus effect being slightly smaller. In Germany and Poland, right-polarized respondents show signs of in-group favoritism, prioritizing right-partisan patients with 6 and 8pp compared to independent patients. In Italy, polarized respondents do not differentiate as strongly based on Italia Viva or Lega partisanship. In some context, it shows that respondents in the middle downgrade right and left-party patients (Brazil, Germany, Poland).

These patterns reveal evidence of out-party animus among affectively polarized left and right respondents and in-group favoritism among affectively polarized right respondents. Out-party animus may be particularly pronounced among left respondents because they believe right-wing partisans are less compliant with COVID regulations. This would influence the perceived deservingness for prioritized treatment, a potential driver of the decisions in moral dilemmas (van Oorschot, Reference van Oorschot2000; van Oorschot, Reference van Oorschot2006). Left-right differences in out-party animus, hence, might be a compound effect of beliefs about the compliance behavior of the patient and the partisanship itself. We cannot tease apart these two possibilities with our research design. Yet, suggestive evidence from the lock-down positions of the parties suggests it is not merely about right-wing approaches to COVID. The AfD in Germany has actively opposed stronger lock-down measures, but the PiS in Poland supported lock-down measures and called for compliance (see SM 2.3.2). Both right partisan patients’ cues trigger equally strong reactions among left respondents, suggesting that the effect is not only due to the position advocated by the patients’ party, but a general reaction toward the right party partisanship.Footnote 10 Another possibility is that partisan cheerleading effects induce differential levels of partisan responses in surveys (Bullock and Lenz, Reference Bullock and Lenz2019), but we leave this for future research to examine.

4. Discussion

How far-reaching are the consequences of affective partisan polarization in times of the global COVID-19 pandemic? Our results suggest affective partisan polarization has nonpolitical consequences in countries across the globe. When asking which patient should receive potentially life-saving treatment for COVID-19, respondents penalize out-party patients. This reveals that out-party animus matters for difficult moral decisions when healthcare resources are distributed. The pattern is particularly strong in Poland and Germany, showing the behavioral nonpolitical relevance of affective polarization beyond the US context.

The findings address pressing issues during the global pandemic. Can the COVID-19 pandemic provide citizens with a sense of shared fate that bridges political divides in favor of a common identity? Or does the zero-sum nature of scarce health resources further increase political conflicts and inflame tribal partisan behavior? The answer to these questions is a vital ingredient for successfully combating the current global health crisis (Van Bavel et al., Reference Van Bavel, Baicker, Boggio, Capraro, Cichocka, Cikara, Crockett, Crum, Douglas, Druckman, Drury, Dube, Ellemers, Finkel, Fowler, Gelfand, Han, Alexander Haslam, Jetten, Kitayama, Mobbs, Napper, Packer, Pennycook, Peters, Petty, Rand, Reicher, Schnall, Shariff, Skitka, Susan Smith, Sunstein, Tabri, Tucker, van der Linden, van Lange, Weeden, Wohl, Zaki, Zion and Willer2020).

There is no reason to expect that partisanship matters in real-world hospital care. Medical staff typically do not know the partisan affiliation of their patients, and medical association guidelines strongly prohibit such discrimination. We are confident that these morally and emotionally difficult decisions are in the hands of trained medical personnel. Nonetheless, the hypothetical triage decision helps us understand how ordinary people think about people with different party affiliations. Our triage experiment provides a more realistic insight into the effect of affective polarization on moral judgments than other more abstract moral dilemmas like the trolley problem. The triage of patients with COVID-19 symptoms is an existing problem that is widely discussed in public and we show that affective partisan polarization can impair moral judgment in these extreme situations.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/psrm.2022.13. To obtain replication material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/SURAOE.

Acknowledgments

This research was generously funded by a grant from the Hertie School Faculty Activity Fund and supported by the Cluster of Excellence “Contestations of the Liberal Script” (EXC 2055, Project-ID: 390715649), funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) under Germany's Excellence Strategy. We thank participants of the ECPR Joint Sessions 2021 and the WZB colloquium “Sociological Perspectives on the Corona Crisis” for valuable feedback on the paper. We thank Johannes Latmann for excellent research assistance. Finally, we are grateful to Respondi for supporting the data collection.

Footnotes

1 Other recent papers have used a similar approach to analyze the moral dilemmas raised by COVID-19, although without examining the importance of partisanship (Helbling et al., Reference Helbling, Maxwell, Munzert and Traunmüller2021; Jin et al., Reference Jin, Huang, Liang and Zhang2021; Larsen and Schaeffer, Reference Larsen and Schaeffer2021; Reeskens et al., Reference Reeskens, Roosma and Wanders2021).

2 We discuss the guidelines for the countries under study in SM Section 1.3.

3 The sample size varies between countries. In Germany it is twice as large as in the other countries. See SM 3.1 for more detail.

4 Decision-making between two patients approximates decisions with multiple alternatives in medical studies (Redelmeier and Shafir, Reference Redelmeier and Shafir1995).

5 Only the first patients level was randomized, the second patients’ arrival to the hospital logically follows from the first.

6 The survey explicitly notes that the stated survival rates already take into account influences of factors such as age and gender. A manipulation check reveals that not all respondents understood this fact (see SM 3.2).

7 For descriptive statistics please see the SM 4.2

8 For perceived job relevance in the different countries see SM 4.1

9 We define the groups based on fix cut-offs of −5 and +5 across all countries. We discuss results when using quantiles of rating-scores differences instead in SM 5.3 and in SM 5.6 provide the linear interaction effect estimates. We further refer to conditional effects on respondents’ partisanship in SM 5.5. The general conclusion is unchanged.

10 In addition, partisans do not generally admit to riskier behavior and compliance in all countries (see SM 4.3 and SM 4.4), questioning the clear attribution of non-compliant behavior for right partisans in all contexts under study.

References

Allcott, H, Boxell, L, Conway, J, Gentzkow, M, Thaler, M and Yang, D (2020) Polarization and public health: partisan differences in social distancing during the coronavirus pandemic. Journal of Public Economics 191, 104254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhanot, SP and Hopkins, DJ (2020) Partisan polarization and resistance to elite messages: results from survey experiments on social distancing. Journal of Behavioral Public Administration 3, 498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boxell, L, Gentzkow, M and Shapiro, JM (2020) Cross-country trends in affective polarization. Technical Report, National Bureau of Economic Research.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bullock, JG and Lenz, G (2019) Partisan bias in surveys. Annual Review of Political Science 22, 325342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clinton, J, Cohen, J, Lapinski, J and Trussler, M (2021) Partisan pandemic: how partisanship and public health concerns affect individuals’ social mobility during covid-19. Science Advances 7, eabd7204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doherty, C, Kiley, J and Asheer, N (2019) Partisan antipathy: More intense, more personal. Available at: https://www.pewresearch.org/politics/wp-content/uploads/sites/4/2019/10/10-10-19-Parties-report.pdfGoogle Scholar
Druckman, JN, Klar, S, Krupnikov, Y, Levendusky, M and Ryan, JB (2020) How affective polarization shapes americans’ political beliefs: a study of response to the covid-19 pandemic. Journal of Experimental Political Science 8, 223234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gadarian, S, Goodman, SW and Pepinsky, T (2021) Partisanship, health behaviors, and policy attitudes in the early stages of the covid 19 pandemic. PLoS ONE 16, e0249596.Google ScholarPubMed
Gift, K and Gift, T (2015) Does politics influence hiring? Evidence from a randomized experiment. Political Behavior 37, 653675.Google Scholar
Greene, JD, Sommerville, RB, Nystrom, LE, Darley, JM and Cohen, JD (2001) An fmri investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293, 21052108.Google ScholarPubMed
Harteveld, E (2021) Ticking all the boxes? A comparative study of social sorting and affective polarization. Electoral Studies 72, 102337.Google Scholar
Helbling, M and Jungkunz, S (2020) Social divides in the age of globalization. West European Politics 43, 11871210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helbling, M, Maxwell, R, Munzert, S and Traunmüller, R (2021) Does every life count the same? evidence from a triage experiment.Google Scholar
Hersh, ED and Goldenberg, MN (2016) Democratic and republican physicians provide different care on politicized health issues. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 113, 1181111816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huber, GA and Malhotra, N (2017) Political homophily in social relationships: evidence from online dating behavior. The Journal of Politics 79, 269283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iyengar, S, Sood, G and Lelkes, Y (2012) Affect, not ideology: a social identity perspective on polarization. Public Opinion Quarterly 76, 405431.Google Scholar
Jin, L, Huang, Y, Liang, Y and Zhang, Q (2021) Who gets the ventilator? Moral decision making regarding medical resource allocation in a pandemic. Journal of the Association for Consumer Research 6, 159167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jöbges, S, Vinay, R, Luyckx, VA and Biller-Andorno, N (2020) Recommendations on covid-19 triage: international comparison and ethical analysis. Bioethics 34, 948959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalmoe, NP and Mason, L (2019) Lethal mass partisanship: prevalence, correlates, & electoral contingencies. Washington, DC: NCAPSA American Politics Meeting. Available at: https://www.dannyhayes.org/uploads/6/9/8/5/69858539/kalmoe___mason_ncapsa_2019_-_lethal_partisanship_-_final_lmedit.pdf.Google Scholar
Klar, S, Krupnikov, Y and Barry Ryan, J (2018) Affective polarization or partisan disdain? Untangling a dislike for the opposing party from a dislike of partisanship. Public Opinion Quarterly 82, 379390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kneer, M and Hannikainen, IR (2020) Triage dilemmas: a window into (ecologically valid) moral cognition.Google Scholar
Larsen, MH and Schaeffer, M (2021) Healthcare chauvinism during the covid-19 pandemic. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 47, 14551473.Google Scholar
Lerman, AE, Sadin, ML and Trachtman, S (2017) Policy uptake as political behavior: evidence from the affordable care act. American Political Science Review 111, 755770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martherus, JL, Martinez, AG, Piff, PK and Theodoridis, AG (2021) Party animals? Extreme partisan polarization and dehumanization. Political Behavior 43, 517540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConnell, C, Margalit, Y, Malhotra, N and Levendusky, M (2018) The economic consequences of partisanship in a polarized era. American Journal of Political Science 62, 518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michelitch, K (2015) Does electoral competition exacerbate interethnic or interpartisan economic discrimination? Evidence from a field experiment in market price bargaining. The American Political Science Review 109, 43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redelmeier, DA and Shafir, E (1995) Medical decision making in situations that offer multiple alternatives. JAMA 273, 302305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reeskens, T, Roosma, F and Wanders, E (2021) The perceived deservingness of covid-19 healthcare in the netherlands: a conjoint experiment on priority access to intensive care and vaccination. BMC Public Health 21, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reiljan, A (2020) Fear and loathing across party lines (also) in Europe: affective polarisation in european party systems. European Journal of Political Research 59, 376396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H, Turner, JC, Austin, WG and Worchel, S (1979) An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. Organizational Identity: A Reader 56, 65.Google Scholar
Van Bavel, JJ, Baicker, K, Boggio, PS, Capraro, V, Cichocka, A, Cikara, M, Crockett, MJ, Crum, AJ, Douglas, KM, Druckman, JN, Drury, J, Dube, O, Ellemers, N, Finkel, EJ, Fowler, JH, Gelfand, M, Han, S, Alexander Haslam, S, Jetten, J, Kitayama, S, Mobbs, D, Napper, LE, Packer, DJ, Pennycook, G, Peters, E, Petty, RE, Rand, DG, Reicher, SD, Schnall, S, Shariff, A, Skitka, LJ, Susan Smith, S, Sunstein, CR, Tabri, N, Tucker, JA, van der Linden, S, van Lange, P, Weeden, KA, Wohl, MJA, Zaki, J, Zion, SR and Willer, R (2020) Using social and behavioural science to support covid-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour 4, 460471.Google Scholar
van Oorschot, W (2000) Who should get what, and why? On deservingness criteria and the conditionality of solidarity among the public. Policy & Politics 28, 3348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Oorschot, W (2006) Making the difference in social Europe: deservingness perceptions among citizens of European welfare states. Journal of European Social Policy 16, 2342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, M (2021) Affective polarization in multiparty systems. Electoral Studies 69, 102199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westwood, SJ, Iyengar, S, Walgrave, S, Leonisio, R, Miller, L and Strijbis, O (2018) The tie that divides: cross-national evidence of the primacy of partyism. European Journal of Political Research 57, 333354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure. 1. Marginal means of prioritizing patient for intensive medical care in the five country and combined samples.

Figure 1

Figure. 2. Average marginal component effects of triage patient's partisan affiliation conditional on respondent's affective polarization.

Supplementary material: PDF

Stoetzer et al. supplementary material

Stoetzer et al. supplementary material

Download Stoetzer et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 805.2 KB
Supplementary material: Link

Stoetzer et al. Dataset

Link