Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T06:58:21.509Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Science and Core Knowledge

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2022

Susan Carey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, New York University
Elizabeth Spelke
Affiliation:
Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology

Abstract

While endorsing Gopnik's proposal that studies of the emergence and modification of scientific theories and studies of cognitive development in children are mutually illuminating, we offer a different picture of the beginning points of cognitive development from Gopnik's picture of “theories all the way down.” Human infants are endowed with several distinct core systems of knowledge which are theory-like in some, but not all, important ways. The existence of these core systems of knowledge has implications for the joint research program between philosophers and psychologists that Gopnik advocates and we endorse. A few lessons already gained from this program of research are sketched.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Philosophy of Science Association 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Send reprint requests to the authors, S. Carey, Department of Psychology, New York University, 6 Washington Place, New York, NY 10003; E. Spelke, Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, 77 Massachusetts Ave., Cambridge, MA 02139.

References

Aguiar, A. and Baillargeon, R. (1996), “2.5-Month-Old's Reasoning about Occlusion Events”, Infant Behavior and Development 19: 293 (abstract).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Antinucci, F. (1989), Cognitive Structure and Development in Nonhuman Primates. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1993), “The Object Concept Revisited: New Directions in the Investigation of Infants’ Physical Knowledge”, in Granrud, C. E. (ed.), Visual Perception and Cognition in Infancy. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 265315.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1994), “A Model of Physical Reasoning in Infancy”, in Rovee-Collier, C. and Lipsitt, L. (ed.), Advances in Infancy Research, Volume 9. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 114139.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. A. and Moses, L.J. (1994), “Early Understanding of Referential Intent and Attentional Focus: Evidence from Language and Emotion”, in Lewis, C. and Mitchell, P. (ed.), Children's Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 133156.Google Scholar
Ball, W. A. (1973, April), “The Perception of Causality in the Infant”, paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Philadelphia, PA.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1993), Autism: The Facts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boysen, S. T. (1993), “Counting in Chimpanzee: Nonhuman Principles and Emergent Properties of Number”, in S. T. Boysen and E. J. Capaldi, The Development of Numerical Competence: Animal and Human Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 3959.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1985), Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1986), “Cognitive Science and Science Education”, American Psychologist 41: 11231130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (1988), “Conceptual Differences Between Children and Adults”, Mind and Language 3: 167181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (1991), “Knowledge Acquisition: Enrichment or Conceptual Change?”, in S. Carey and R. Gelman (1991), pp. 257291.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1995), “On the Origins of Causal Understanding”, in Sperber, D., Premack, D., and Premack, A.J. (eds.), Causal Cognition. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 268303.Google Scholar
Carey, S. and Gelman, R. (1991), The Epigenesis of Mind: Essays on Biology and Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carey, S. and Spelke, E. S. (1994), “Domain-Specific Knowledge and Conceptual Change”, in L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (ed.), Mapping The Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 169200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clement, J. (1982), “Students’ Preconceptions in Introductory Mechanics”, American Journal Of Physics 50: 6671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clements, W. A. and Perner, J. (1994), “Implicit Understanding of Belief”, Cognitive Development 9: 377395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crider, C (1981), Children's Conceptions of the Body Interior”, in Bibace, R. and Walsh, M. (ed.) Children's Conceptions of Health, Illness and Bodily Functions. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, pp. 000–000Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (1988), “Differences Between Adult and Infant Cognition: Is the Crucial Variable Presence or Absence of Language?”, in Weiskrantz, L. (ed.), Thought Without Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 337370.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (1990), “The Development and Neural Bases of Higher Cognitive Functions”, Annals Of The New York Academy Of Sciences 608: 267317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (1991), “Neuropsychological Insights into the Meaning of Object Concept Development”, in S. Carey and R. Gelman (1991), pp. 67110.Google Scholar
Duhem, P. (1949), The Aim and Structure of Physical Theory. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Feyerabend, P. (1962), “Explanation, Reduction, Empiricism”, in Feigl, H. and Maxwell, G. (ed.), Minnesota Studies In The Philosophy Of Science, Vol. 3. Minneapolis: University Of Minnesota Press, pp. 41–87.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1992), “A Theory of the Child's Theory of Mind. Cognition 44: 283296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallistel, C.R. (1990), The Organization of Learning. New York: Crowell.Google Scholar
Gelman, R. (1991), Epigenetic Foundations of Knowledge Structures: Initial and Transcendent Constructions”, in S. Carey and R. Gelman (1991), pp. 293322.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., Nadasky, Z., Csibra, G., and Biro, S (1995), “Taking the Intentional Stance at 12 Months of Age”, Cognition 56: 165193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1987), “Circuitry of Primate Prefrontal Cortex and Regulation of Behavior by Representational Memory. Handbook Of Physiology 5: 373417.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. and Meltzoff, A. N. (in press), Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hacking, I. (1993), “Working In a New World: The Taxonomic Solution”, in Horwich, P. and Thomson, J. (ed.), World Changes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 275310.Google Scholar
Harris, P. (1974) The A/Not B Error When Object Permanence Is Not at Issue. Journal Of Experimental Child Psychology 18: 535542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatano, G. and Inagaki, K. (1994), “Young Children's Naive Theory of Biology”, Cognition 50: 171188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, M. D. and Carey, S. (in press), “Building a Cognitive Creature from a Set of Primitives: Evolutionary and Developmental Insights”, in Allen, C. and Cummins, D. (ed.), The Evolution of Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hermer, L. and Spelke, E. S. (in press), “Modularity And Development: The Case Of Spatial Reorientation”, Cognition.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, L. A. and Gelman, S. A. (1994), Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. and Carey, S. (1996), “Distinguishing Knowledge Enrichment from Conceptual Change; Evidence from Williams Syndrome”, submitted ms.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., and Baron-Cohen, S. (1995), “Is There a Social Module? Language, Face Processing, and Theory of Mind in Individuals with Williams Syndrome”, Journal Of Cognitive Neuroscience 7: 196208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keil, F. C. (1989), Concepts, Kinds and Cognitive Development. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Keil, F. C. (1992), “The Origins of an Autonomous Biology”, in M. R. Gunnar and M. Maratsos (ed.), Modularity and Constraints in Language and Cognition. The Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology 25: 103256.Google Scholar
Keil, F. C. (1994), “The Birth and Nurturance of Concepts by Domains”, in L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (1994), pp. 234254.Google Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1988), “The Child as Parent of the Scientist”, Mind And Language 3: 217228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koocher, G. P. (1974), “Talking with Children about Death”, American Journal Of Ortho-Psychiatria 44: 404410.Google ScholarPubMed
Kotovsky, L. and Baillargeon, R. (1994), “Calibration-Based Reasoning about Collision Events in 11-Month-Old Infants”, Cognition 51: 107129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, T. S. (1962), The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University Of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1977), “A Function for Thought Experiments”, in T. S. Kuhn, The Essential Tension. Chicago: University Of Chicago Press, pp. 240265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1982), “Commensurability, Comparability, Communicability”, PSA 1982, 2. East Lansing, MI: Philosophy Of Science Association, pp. 669–688.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1988), “The Necessity of Illusion: Perception and Thought in Infancy”, in Weiskrantz, L. (ed.), Thought Without Language. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 185210.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. (1994), “Tomm, Toby, and Agency: Core Architecture and Domain Specificity”, in L. A. Hirschfeld and S. A. Gelman (1994), pp. 119148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M., Bauer, P. L., and Mcdonough, L. (1991), “Separating the Sheep from the Goats: Differentiating Global Categories”, Cognitive Psychology 23: 263298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsuzawa, T. (1985), “Use Of Numbers by a Chimpanzee. Nature 315: 5759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCloskey, M. (1983), “Naive Theories Of Motion”, in Gentner, D. and Stevens, A. L. (ed.) Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 299324.Google Scholar
Michotte, A. (1963), The Perception Of Causality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Milner, A. D and Goodale, M. A. (1995), The Visual Brain In Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nagy, M. H. (1953), “The Child's Theories Concerning Death”, Journal Of Genetic Psychology 83: 199216.Google Scholar
Nersessian, N. J. (1992), “How Do Scientists Think? Capturing the Dynamics of Conceptual Change in Science”, in Giere, R. N. (ed.), Cognitive Models Of Science. Minnesota Studies In The Philosophy Of Science, vol. 15. Minneapolis, MI: University Of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Oakes, L. M. (1994), “The Development of Infants’ Use of Continuity Cues in Their Perception of Causality”, Developmental Psychology 30: 869879.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Reilly, R. C. and Johnson, M. H. (1994), “Object Recognition and Sensitive Periods: A Computational Analysis of Visual Imprinting”, Neural Computation 6: 357389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M. (1987), “Evidence For Conceptual Quantitative Abilities in the African Grey Parrot: Labeling of Cardinal Sets. Ethology 75: 3761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954), The Construction of Reality in the Child. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1941), Le Development des Quantites Chez L'enfant. Neufchatel: Delchaux et Niestle.Google Scholar
Proffitt, D. R. and Gilden, D. L. (1989), “Understanding Natural Dynamics”, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 15: 384393.Google ScholarPubMed
Regolin, L. and Vallortigara, G. (1995), “Perception of Partly Occluded Objects by Young Chicks”, Perception And Psychophysics 57: 971976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Regolin, L., Vallortigara, G., and Zanforlin, M. (1995), “Detour Behavior in the Domestic Chick: Searching for a Disappearing Prey or a Disappearing Social Partner”, Animal Behavior 50: 203211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochat, P. and Hespos, S. J. (1996), “Tracking and Anticipation of Invisible Spatial Transformations by 4- To 8-Month-Old Infants”, Cognitive Development 11: 318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C., Carey, S., and Wiser, M. (1985), “On Differentiation: A Case Study of the Development of Concepts of Size, Weight, and Density”, Cognition 21: 177237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C., Snir, Y., Grosslight, L., and Unger, C. (1988), “Using Conceptual Models to Facilitate Conceptual Change: Weight and Density”, Educational Technology Center Technical Report, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Smith, L. B. and Thelen, E. (1995), “Tests of a Dynamic Systems Theory: The Object Concept”, symposium presented at the Meeting of The Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Kestenbaum, R., Simons, D., and Wein, D. (1995), “Spatio-Temporal Continuity, Smoothness of Motion, and Object Identity in Infancy”, British Journal of Developmental Psychology 13: 113142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A., and Woodward, A. L. (1995), “Infants’ Knowledge of Object Motion and Human Action”, in Sperber, D., Premack, D., and Premack, A. (ed.), Causal Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 4478.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S. and Tsivkin, S. (in press), “Initial Knowledge and Conceptual Change: Space and Number”, in Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. (ed.), Language Acquisition And Conceptual Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., and Walle, G. A. Van De (1993), “Perceiving and Reasoning about Objects: Insights from Infants”, in Eilan, N., Brewer, W., and Mccarthy, R. (ed.), Spatial Representation. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, pp. 132161.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Vishton, P., and Hofsten, C. Von (1994), “Object Perception, Object-Directed Action, and Physical Knowledge in Infancy”, in Gazzaniga, M. S. (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1994), “The Modularity of Thought and the Epidemiology of Representations”, in L. Hirschfeld and S. Gelman (1994), pp. 3967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suppe, F. (1977), The Structure Of Scientific Theories. Urbana, IL: University Of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1994), “Theory of Mind in Young Children with Williams Syndrome”, paper presented at the Biannual Meeting of the Williams Syndrome Association, San Diego.Google Scholar
Trick, L. and Pylyshyn, Z. (1994), “Why Are Small and Large Numbers Enumerated Differently? A Limited-Capacity Preattentive Stage in Vision”, Psychological Review 101: 80102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uller, C., Carey, S., Huntley-Fenner, G., and Klatt, L. (1996), “What Representations Might Underlie Infant Numerical Knowledge?”, submitted ms.Google Scholar
Vosniadu, S. and Brewer, W. F., (1992), “Mental Models of the Earth: A Study of Conceptual Change in Childhood”, Cognitive Psychology 24: 535585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. (1979), “Perception of Contingency as a Determinant of Social Responsiveness”, in Tohman, E. (ed.), The Origins Of Social Responsiveness. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 136153.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., and Bartsch, K. (1987), “Infant Search and Object Permanence: A Metaanalysis of the A-Not-B Error”, Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 51.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. and Gelman, S. A. (1992), “Cognitive Development: Foundational Theories of Core Domains”, Annual Review Of Psychology 43: 337375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilcox, T., Rosser, R., and Nadel, L. (1994), “Representation of Object Location in 6.5-Month-Old Infants”, Cognitive Development 9: 193210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiser, M. (1988a), “Can Models Foster Conceptual Change? The Case of Heat and Temperature”, Educational Technology Center Technical Report, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Wiser, M. (1988b), The Differentiation of Heat and Temperature: History of Science and Novice-Expert Shift”, in Strauss, S. (ed.), Ontogeny, Philogeny, and Historical Development. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 2848.Google Scholar
Wiser, M. and Carey, S. (1983), “When Heat and Temperature Were One”, in Gentner, D. and Stevens, A. (ed.), Mental Models. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 267297.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (1996), “Infants Selectively Encode the Goal Object of an Actor's Reach”, submitted ms.Google Scholar
Wynn, K. (1990), “Children's Understanding of Counting”, Cognition 36: 155193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wynn, K. (1992a), “Addition and Subtraction in Infants”, Nature 358: 749750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, K. (1992b), “Children's Acquisition of the Number Words and the Counting System”, Cognitive Psychology 24: 220251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, F. and Carey, S. (1996), “Infants’ Metaphysics: The Case of Numerical Identity”, Cognitive Psychology 30: 111153.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed