Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-04T19:48:23.488Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Resituating Knowledge: Generic Strategies and Case Studies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2022

Abstract

This paper addresses the problem of how scientific knowledge, which is always locally generated, becomes accepted in other sites. The analysis suggests that there are a small number of strategies that enable scientists to resituate knowledge and that these strategies are generic: they are not restricted to specific disciplines or modes of doing science but rather are found in a variety of different forms across the sciences.

Type
Medical and Social Sciences
Copyright
Copyright © The Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

Thanks to the British Academy and Wolfson Foundation for funding my research project “Re-thinking Case Studies across the Social Sciences.” I thank participants at the PSA 2012 Symposium on comparability and cases, Rachel Ankeny, Attilia Ruzzene, Ted Porter, and especially Sharon Crasnow and Stephen Turner, for their comments.

References

Ankeny, Rachel. 2007. “Wormy Logic: Model Organisms as Case-Based Reasoning.” In Science without Laws: Model Systems, Cases, Exemplary Narratives, ed. Creager, Angela, Wise, M. Norton, and Lunbeck, Elizabeth, 4658. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Creager, Angela, Wise, M. Norton, and Lunbeck, Elizabeth 2011. “Using Cases to Establish Novel Diagnoses: Creating Generic Facts by Making Particular Facts Travel Together.” In How Well Do Facts Travel? ed. Howlett, Peter and Morgan, Mary S., 252–72. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Morgan, Mary S. 2014. “The Overlooked Role of Cases in Casual Attribution in Medicine.” Philosophy of Science, in this issue.Google Scholar
Baden-Fuller, Charles. 1989. “Exit from Declining Industries and the Case of Steel Castings.” Economic Journal 99:949–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartwright, Nancy. 2012. “Will This Policy Work for You? Predicting Effectiveness Better: How Philosophy Helps.” Philosophy of Science 79 (Proceedings): 973–89.10.1086/668041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crasnow, Sharon. 2012. “Cases and the Accumulation of Knowledge in International Relations: The Democratic Peace.” Paper for LSE Workshop on Cases and the Accumulation of Knowledge.Google Scholar
Guala, Francesco. 2010. “Extrapolation, Analogy, and Comparative Process Tracing.” Philosophy of Science 77 (Proceedings): 1070–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, Jon. 2013. “Coffee Addiction: Do People Consume Too Much Caffeine?” BBC News Magazine. http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/magazine-22530625.Google Scholar
Leonelli, Sabina. 2009. “On the Locality of Data and Claims about Phenomena.” Philosophy of Science 76 (Proceedings): 737–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonelli, Sabina 2011. “Packaging Small Facts for Re-use: Databases in Model Organism Biology.” In How Well Do Facts Travel? ed. Peter Howlett and Mary S. Morgan. 325–48. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebow, Elliot. 1967. Tally’s Corner: A Study of Negro Streetcorner Men. Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Lynd, Robert S., and Lynd, Helen Merrell. 1929. Middletown: A Study in Contemporary American Culture. New York: Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
McKinney, John C. 1950. “The Role of Constructive Typology in Scientific Sociological Analysis.” Social Forces 28:235–40.10.2307/2572006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, Mary S. 2007. “Reflections on Exemplary Narratives, Cases, and Model Organisms.” In Science without Laws: Model Systems, Cases, Exemplary Narratives, ed. Creager, Angela, Wise, M. Norton, and Lunbeck, Elizabeth, 264–74. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Creager, Angela, Wise, M. Norton, and Lunbeck, Elizabeth 2012. “Case Studies: One Observation or Many? Justification or Discovery?Philosophy of Science 79 (4): 667–77.Google Scholar
Creager, Angela, Wise, M. Norton, and Lunbeck, Elizabeth Forthcoming. “Glass Ceilings and Sticky Floors: Drawing New Ontologies.” In Culture without Culturalism, ed. Evelyn Fox-Keller and Karine Chemla. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Morgan, Mary S., and Knuuttila, Tarja. 2012. “Models and Modelling in Economics.” In Handbook of the Philosophy of Economics, ed. Mäki, Uskali, 4987. Oxford: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novak, Leszek. 1980. The Structure of Idealization. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Ruzzene, Attilia. 2012. “Making Knowledge Usable by Means of Case Studies.” PSA Symposium Paper.Google Scholar
Steel, Daniel. 2008. Across the Boundaries: Extrapolation in Biology and Social Sciences. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sutherland, Edwin Hardin. 1949. White Collar Crime. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar