Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T07:04:29.643Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Object and Event Concepts: A Cognitive Mechanism of Incommensurability

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2022

Abstract

In this paper I examine a cognitive mechanism of incommensurability. Using the frame model of concept representation to capture structural relations within concepts, I reveal an ontological difference between object and event concepts: the former are spatial but the latter temporal. Experiments from cognitive sciences further demonstrate that the mind treats object and event concepts differently. Thus, incommensurability can occur in conceptual change across different ontological categories. I use a historical case to illustrate how the ontological difference between an object and an event concept actually caused incommensurability in the context of nineteenth-century optics. The cognitive and historical analyses indicate that incommensurability can be a local phenomenon and does not necessarily imply incomparability.

Type
Psychology, Cognitive, and Neuroscience
Copyright
Copyright © The Philosophy of Science Association

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barsalou, Lawrence (1992), “Frames, Concepts, and Conceptual Fields”, in Lehrer, A. and Kittay, E. (eds.), Frames, Fields and Contrasts: New Essays in Semantical and Lexical Organization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 2174.Google Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence, and Sewell, Daniel (1985), “Contrasting the Representation of Scripts and Categories”, Contrasting the Representation of Scripts and Categories 24:646665.Google Scholar
Bower, Gordon, Black, John, and Turner, Terrence (1979), “Scripts in Memory for Text”, Scripts in Memory for Text 11:177220.Google Scholar
Chen, Xiang (1997), “Thomas Kuhn's Latest Notion of Incommensurability”, Thomas Kuhn's Latest Notion of Incommensurability 28:257273.Google Scholar
Chen, Xiang, and Barker, Peter (2000), “Continuity Through Revolutions: A Frame-Based Account of Conceptual Change During Scientific Revolutions”, Continuity Through Revolutions: A Frame-Based Account of Conceptual Change During Scientific Revolutions 67(Proceedings): S208S223.Google Scholar
Chi, Michelene (1992), “Conceptual Change within and across Ontological Categories: Examples from Learning and Discovery in Science”, in Giere, R. (ed.), Cognitive Models of Science. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, 129186.Google Scholar
Good, Gregory (1982), J. F. W. Herschel's Optical Researches: A Study in Method. Ph. D. Dissertation. University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Herschel, John (1820), “On the Action of Crystallized Bodies on Homogeneous Light, and on the Causes of the Deviation from Newton's Scale in the Tints Which Many of Them Develop on Exposure to a Polarized Ray”, On the Action of Crystallized Bodies on Homogeneous Light, and on the Causes of the Deviation from Newton's Scale in the Tints Which Many of Them Develop on Exposure to a Polarized Ray 110:4599.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, Philip (1980), “Mental Models in Cognitive Science”, Mental Models in Cognitive Science 4:71115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keil, Frank (1979), Semantic and Conceptual Development: An Ontological Perspective. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, Thomas (1962), The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, Thomas (1993), “Afterwords”, in Horwich, P. (ed.), World Changes. Cambridge: M. I. T. Press, 311341.Google Scholar
Lakoff, George (1987), Women, Fire, and Dangerous Things: What Categories Reveal about the Mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, Eleanor, Mervis, Carolyn, Gray, Wayne, Johnson, David, and Boyes-Braem, Penny (1976), “Basic Objects in Natural Categories”, Basic Objects in Natural Categories 8:382439.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, David (1980), “Schemata: The Building Blocks of Cognition”, in Bruce, B. and Brewer, W. (eds.), Theoretical Issues in Reading Comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 3358.Google Scholar
Schank, Roger, and Abelson, Robert (1977), Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding: An Inquiry into Human Knowledge Structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sommers, Fred (1971), “Structural Ontology”, Structural Ontology 1:2142.Google Scholar
Tversky, Barbara, and Hemenway, Kathleen (1984), “Objects, Parts, and Categories”, Objects, Parts, and Categories 113:169193.Google ScholarPubMed