Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T01:57:51.940Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Scientist as Child

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2022

Alison Gopnik*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley

Abstract

This paper argues that there are powerful similarities between cognitive development in children and scientific theory change. These similarities are best explained by postulating an underlying abstract set of rules and representations that underwrite both types of cognitive abilities. In fact, science may be successful largely because it exploits powerful and flexible cognitive devices that were designed by evolution to facilitate learning in young children. Both science and cognitive development involve abstract, coherent systems of entities and rules, theories. In both cases, theories provide predictions, explanations, and interpretations. In both, theories change in characteristic ways in response to counterevidence. These ideas are illustrated by an account of children's developing understanding of the mind.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Philosophy of Science Association 1996

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

The research and ideas reported in this paper were supported by NSF grant DBS9213959. A portion of it was presented at the Society for Philosophy and Psychology, June, 1995. I am grateful to Henry Wellman, Andrew Meltzoff, Clark Glymour, John Campbell, Philip Kitcher, Eric Schwitzgebel, and two reviewers for illuminating discussions and comments.

Send reprint requests to the author, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720.

References

Astington, J. W. and Gopnik, A. (1991), “Developing Understanding Of Desire And Intention”, in Whiten, A. (ed.), Natural Theories Of Mind: Evolution, Development and Simulation of Everyday Mindreading. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, pp. 3950.Google Scholar
Bartsch, K. and Wellman, H. M. (1989), “Young Children's Attribution of Action to Beliefs and Desires”, Child Development 60(4): 946964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartsch, K. and Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children Talk about the Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bennett, K. and Harvey, P. (1985), “Brain Size, Development and Metabolism in Birds and Mammals”, Journal of Zoology 207: 491509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (1985), Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1988), “Conceptual Differences between Children and Adults”, Mind and Language 3(3): 167183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. (1984), Matter and Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Davies, M. and Stone, T. (ed.) (1995). Folk psychology: The Theory of Mind Debate. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C, and Youngblade, L. (1991), “Young Children's Understanding of Other People's Feelings and Beliefs: Individual Differences and Their Antecedents”, Child Development 62: 13521366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Everett, B. A., Croft, K., and Flavell, E. R. (1981), “Young Children's Knowledge about Visual Perception: Further Evidence for the Level 1–Level 2 Distinction”, Developmental Psychology 17: 99103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., Green, F. L., and Moses, L. J. (1990), “Young Children's Understanding of Fact Beliefs Versus Value Beliefs”, Child Development 61(4): 915928.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1986), Development of Knowledge about the Appearance-Reality Distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51, No. 1.10.2307/1165866CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1995), Young Children's Knowledge about Thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelman, S. A. and Wellman, H. M. (1991), “Insides and Essence: Early Understandings of the Non-Obvious”, Cognition: 213244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giere, R. (ed.) (1991), Cognitive Models of Science. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (1986), Epistemology and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. (1984), “Conceptual and Semantic Change in Scientists and Children: Why There Are No Semantic Universals”, Linguistics 20: 163179.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. (1988), “Conceptual and Semantic Development as Theory Change”, Mind and Language 3:197217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. (1993), “How We Know Our Minds: The Illusion of First-Person Knowledge of Intentionality”, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16: 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. (1996), “Theories and Modules: Creation Myths, Developmental Realities and Neurath's Boat”, in Carruthers, P. and Smith, P. (ed.), Theories of Theory of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 169183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. and Astington, J. W. (1988), “Children's Understanding of Representational Change and its Relation to the Understanding of False Belief and the Appearance-Reality Distinction”, Child Development 59: 2637.10.2307/1130386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. and Graf, P. (1988), “Knowing How You Know: Young Children's Ability to Identify and Remember the Sources of Their Beliefs”, Child Development 59: 13661371. “CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1994), “Minds, Bodies and Persons: Young Children's Understanding of the Self and Others as Reflected in Imitation and “Theory of Mind” Research”, in Parker, S. and Mitchell, R. (ed.), Self-Awareness in Animals and Humans. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 157181.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1997), Words, Thoughts, and Theories. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/ M.I.T. Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A. N., and Slaughter, V. (1994), “Changing Your Views: How Understanding Visual Perception Can Lead to a New Theory of the Mind”, in Lewis, C. and Mitchell, P. (ed.) Origins of a Theory of Mind. New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. and Slaughter, V. (1991), “Children's Understanding of Changes in Their Mental States”, Child Development 62: 98110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. and Wellman, H. (1992), “Why the Child's Theory of Mind Really is a Theory”, Mind and Language 7 (1 and 2): 145172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. and Wellman, H. (1994), “The ‘Theory Theory'”, in Hirschfield, L. and Gelman, S. (ed.), Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Culture and Cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 257–293Google Scholar
Harris, P. (1991), “The Work of the Imagination”, in Whiten, A. (ed.), Natural Theories of Mind: The Evolution, Development, and Simulation of Second-Order Mental Representations. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, pp. 283304.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1988), “The Child is a Theoretician, Not an Inductivist”, Mind and Language 3(3): 183197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. and Inhelder, B. (1974), “If You Want to Get Ahead, Get a Theory”, Cognition 3(3): 195212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keil, F. (1987), “Conceptual Development and Category Structure”, in Neisser, U. (ed.), Concepts and Conceptual Development. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 175201.Google Scholar
Keil, F. C. (1989), Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1993), The Advancement of Science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kornblith, H. (ed.) (1985), Naturalizing Epistemology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lempers, J. D., Flavell, E. R., and Flavell, J. H. (1977), “The Development in Very Young Children of Tacit Knowledge Concerning Visual Perception”, Genetic Psychology Monographs 95: 353.Google ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. M. (1991), “Information Processing and Conceptual Knowledge: The Theory of TOMM”, paper presented at the meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, WA.Google Scholar
Lillard, A., and Flavell, J. (1990), “Young Children's Preference for Mental State Versus Behavioral Descriptions of Human Action”, Child Development 61(5): 731741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masangkay, Z., McCluskey, K., McIntyre, C., Sims-Knight, J., Vaughan, B., and Flavell, J. H. (1974), The Early Development of Inferences About the Visual Percepts of Others”, Child Development 45: 357366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. and Gopnik, A. (1993), “The Role of Imitation in Understanding Persons and Developing a Theory of Mind”, in Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., and Cohen, D. (ed.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives From Autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 335366.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1983), “Newborn Infants Imitate Adult Facial Gestures”, Child Development 54, 702709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, P. and Lacohee, H. (1991), “Children's Early Understanding Of False Belief”, Cognition 39(2): 107109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moses, L. J. (1993), “Young Children's Understanding of Belief Constraints on Intention”, Cognitive Development, pp. 127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moses, L. J., and Flavell, J. H. (1990). “Inferring False Beliefs from Actions and Reactions. Child Development 61(4): 929945.10.2307/1130866CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, G. and Medin, D. (1985), “The Role of Theories in Conceptual Coherence”, Psychological Review 92: 289316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Neill, D. K., Astington, J. W., and Flavell, J. H. (1992), “Young Children's Understanding of the Role that Sensory Experiences Play in Knowledge Acquisition”, Child Development 63(2): 474491.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perner, J. (1991), Understanding the Representational Mind. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Ruffman, T., and Leekam, S. R. (1994), “Theory of Mind is Contagious: You Catch It from Your Sibs”, Child Development 65, 5: 12281238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. O., and Ullian, J. S. (1970), The Web of Belief. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Repacoli, B. and Gopnik, A. (in press), “Early Reasoning about Desires: Evidence from 14-and 18-Month-Olds”, Developmental Psychology.Google Scholar
Slaughter, V. and Gopnik, A. (in press), “Conceptual Coherence in the Child's Theory of Mind”, Child Development.Google Scholar
Stich, S. (1983), From Folk Psychology to Cognitive Science: The Case Against Belief. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Van Fraassen, B. (1980), The Scientific Image. Oxford; Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. (1990), The Child's Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. and Bartsch, K. (1988), “Young Children's Reasoning about Beliefs”, Cognition 30: 239277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M. and Estes, D. (1986), “Early Understanding of Mental Entities: A Reexamination of Childhood Realism”, Child Development 57: 910923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M. and Gelman, S. A. (1992), “Cognitive Development: Foundational Theories of Core Domains”, Annual Review of Psychology 43: 337375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wellman, H. M. and Woolley, J. D. (1990), “From Simple Desires to Ordinary Beliefs: The Early Development of Everyday Psychology”, Cognition 35(3): 245275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983), “Beliefs About Beliefs: Representation and Constraining Function of Wrong Beliefs in Young Children's Understanding of Deception”, Cognition 13: 103108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuill, N. (1984), “Young Children's Coordination of Motive and Outcome in Judgements of Satisfaction and Morality”, British Journal of Developmental Psychology 2: 7381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar