Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T16:02:57.985Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Opinion: Which animals have personality?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 February 2024

Ralph Adolphs*
Affiliation:
Division of Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, USA Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, USA
Yue Xu*
Affiliation:
Division of Humanities and Social Sciences, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, USA Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA, USA
*
Corresponding authors: Ralph Adolphs; Email: [email protected] and Yue Xu; Email: [email protected]
Corresponding authors: Ralph Adolphs; Email: [email protected] and Yue Xu; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Human personality generally refers to coherent individuating patterns in affect, behavior, and cognition. We can only observe and measure behavior, from which we then infer personality and other psychological processes (affect, cognition, etc.). We emphasize that the study of personality always explains or summarizes patterns not only in behavior but also in these other psychological processes inferred from behavior. We thus argue that personality should be attributed only to nonhuman animals with behaviors from which we can infer a sufficiently rich set of psychological processes. The mere inference of a biological trait that explains behavioral variability, on our view, is not sufficient to count as a personality construct and should be given a different term. Methodologically, inferring personality in nonhuman animals entails challenges in characterizing ecologically valid behaviors, doing so across rich and varied environments, and collecting enough data. We suggest that studies should gradually accumulate such corpora of data on a species through well-curated shared databases. A mixture of approaches should include both top-down fit with extant human personality theories (such as the Big Five) as well as bottom-up discovery of species-specific personality dimensions. Adopting the above framework will help us to build a comparative psychology and will provide the most informative models also for understanding human personality, its evolution, and its disorders.

Type
Review Paper
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. What is animal personality?

Personality is a psychological construct commonly used to explain relatively temporally stable individual differences in how a person thinks, feels, and behaves (Roberts & Mroczek, Reference Roberts and Mroczek2008; Coon & Mitterer, Reference Coon and Mitterer2012; Baumert et al., Reference Baumert, Schmitt, Perugini, Johnson, Blum, Borkenau and Wrzus2017). Nonhuman animals also exhibit stable individual differences in behavior, but it remains a challenge how to infer their thoughts and feelings. These conceptual differences are reflected in differences in method: for people, we can assess personality with self-report questionnaires (which may of course turn out to be inaccurate or invalid), as well as from behavioral observation; for nonhuman animals, we have only the behavioral observation. Like all psychological processes, we think of personality as a latent variable that cannot be observed directly, but that must ultimately be inferred from behavior. It is important to stress that both lay and scientific conceptions of human personality generally encompass individual differences in a range of psychological processes: “personality is an abstraction used to explain consistency and coherency in an individual’s pattern of affects, cognitions, desires and behaviors” (Revelle, Reference Revelle, Robins, Fraley and Krueger2007, p. 37).

By contrast, definitions of personality as applied to animals generally restrict themselves to patterns of behavior: personality refers to “between-individual differences in behavior that persist through time” (Carter et al., Reference Carter, Feeney, Marshall, Cowlishaw and Heinsohn2013, p. 467), or “labile behavioral traits that tend to differ consistently between individuals of the same species” (Maskrey et al., Reference Maskrey, Sneddon, Arnold, Wolfenden and Thomson2021, p. 13). We argue here that personality needs to be inferred when we want to explain behavioral patterns so complex that we need to infer psychological terms: these behavioral patterns cannot be explained efficiently by biology alone. For example, when we use “extraversion” in personality to explain or predict a person’s behavior, it will be much more efficient than describing the biological/neurological mechanisms of the behavior (of course, these biological/neurological mechanisms can also be explained by chemistry and physics, which would be the least efficient). To be sure, we would see all psychological processes ultimately as biological (and, for that matter, chemical and physical), but these disciplines have different terms, different explanatory aims, and offer different efficiency in their explanation. According to our view, we do not need to attribute personality to a sea anemone, disagreeing with Briffa and Greenaway (Reference Briffa and Greenaway2011), Hensley et al. (Reference Hensley, Cook, Lang, Petelle and Blumstein2012), and Maskrey et al. (Reference Maskrey, Sneddon, Arnold, Wolfenden and Thomson2021). Of course, there are phenotypic traits that show stable individual differences, such as eye color, fur color, or the general appearance of an animal or person. Similarly, there are behavioral traits that show stable individual differences, such as stable differences only in running speed, reaction time, a limp on one side, or a tremor. These examples of traits have biological and neurological explanations, respectively. They do not need psychological explanations. Personality is, in our view, first and foremost, a psychological term and it is always concerned not only with behavior but also with other psychological processes; different terminology should be applied to explanations that are purely biological or neurological.

Indeed, personality should not even be applied to cases where there are individual differences in only one or a narrow set of psychological processes; it needs to be more comprehensive than that. For instance, individual differences in memory or attention alone are just that — like individual differences in running speed, we do not need to attribute personality: we can just say they are individual differences in memory, attention, or running speed and leave it at that. Occam’s razor should be applied here: explanation with a personality trait should be reserved for those cases that would be incomplete otherwise.

No doubt the view described above will be persuasive in attributing personality to only some species (like humans, monkeys, dogs, and probably rodents), exclude it from others (like sea anemones), and leave a lot in a gray zone where debate and further studies are needed (like fish or octopuses). Even sea anemones might turn out to have personalities (Briffa & Greenaway, Reference Briffa and Greenaway2011; Hensley et al., Reference Hensley, Cook, Lang, Petelle and Blumstein2012; Maskrey et al., Reference Maskrey, Sneddon, Arnold, Wolfenden and Thomson2021) – but, on our view, we would need a lot more evidence than what is currently offered.

To summarize this section: personality can be inferred from relatively temporally stable patterns of complex behaviors that distinguish individuals from one another. However, we stress that the behavior should be complicated enough that personality is used to explain patterns not only in behavior but also in at least some other psychological processes (like emotion or attention or cognitive control – which, again, we here view as latent variables inferred from behavior). Of course, experiments could be devised to use relatively simple behavioral readouts of psychological processes including personality (e.g., pushing buttons to answer self-report questions). But the key point is that personality should fit into an animal or a person’s psychology that relates personality, affect, and cognition to one another and to behavior – in essence, personality should be an ingredient in a theory about a person’s or animal’s mind that is used to explain behavior more comprehensively. Our view differs from that of some others on animal personality, who have focused only on behavior and not prominently included a need for other psychological processes (Kralj-Fišer & Schuett, Reference Kralj-Fišer and Schuett2014; Kaiser & Müller, Reference Kaiser and Müller2021). How much of a psychology would be required is an open question – but we would urge that there should be several other psychological processes we infer in an animal and that there are relations between these and personality. If it is only behavior that is to be explained, we would suggest using a different term than personality.

2. Criteria for animal personality

What observational criteria could be used to infer personality in nonhuman animal species? As Table 1 already shows, quite a variety of approaches have been used, generally requiring substantial interpretation by human observers. The settings vary from the laboratory to zoos to naturalistic environments and immediately raise two critical questions. First, how should behaviors be classified or coded? Second, how much observational data do we need to do this? These two challenges are crucial to identify ethologically meaningful and generalizable patterns in behaviors whose regularity across time is the basis for inferring personality traits. These challenges have been discussed at length by Uher (Reference Uher2008). Building on this discussion, we would emphasize three critical ingredients: collect as complete a dataset over as extended a time as possible; code the data by expert consensus in light of the best knowledge that we have of the animal’s ethology; and collect the data in rich, naturalistic settings. The last point raises a further issue: context dependency.

Table 1. A nonexhaustive list of research on animal personality after 2001. See Gosling (Reference Gosling2001) for a list of research on animal personality before 2001. See Kralj-Fiser and Schuett (Reference Kralj-Fišer and Schuett2014) for a list of additional invertebrate studies

The issue of context dependency offers some valuable criteria for animal personality. Personality constructs need to refer to individual differences that generalize to some extent across time, that is, they should be temporally stable. On the other hand, we would argue that the behaviors from which we can infer the personality constructs need to be context dependent (context sensitive) to some degree (again, contrary to some views in Kaiser & Müller’s Reference Kaiser and Müller(2021) work that have instead stressed contextual consistency). In humans, the context sensitivity of how personality is expressed in behavior has been a well-known feature ever since Walter Mischel’s seminal work (Mischel, Reference Mischel1996).

The inferred personality constructs from the behaviors must have consistency across contexts, otherwise the criterion of temporal stability would be violated as context changes over time. To operationalize this, the consistency merely needs to apply at the level of the inferred personality constructs from recurring patterns of behaviors – not the precise bodily movements themselves. Indeed, behaviors that are too rigid across very different contexts may be inappropriate indicators of personality as we are defining it – they may instead be like sea anemone traits or Parkinsonian tremor in humans: biological traits that are not psychological. Some flexibility and plasticity seem essential (Dall et al., Reference Dall, Houston and McNamara2004). We would speculate that more open-ended contexts, such as complex social situations, might reveal behavioral differences that can best be explained by personality (Gosling, Reference Gosling2001; Uher, Reference Uher2008). An important methodological point would thus be to study personality with an emphasis on those situations/contexts where personality actually has a measurable role in explaining and predicting behavior.

The behavioral patterns from which we infer personality in animals, as in humans, should exhibit broad, situation-dependent (context sensitive) variability. This context- or situation dependence is precisely the evidence that personality processes (together with other psychological processes) are interposed between the situational stimuli and the behaviors that they elicit. The other psychological processes, such as affect and cognition, together with environment/context, constitute an important aspect of personality according to many views (Revelle, Reference Revelle, Robins, Fraley and Krueger2007; Wilt & Revelle, Reference Wilt and Revelle2015; Arden et al., Reference Arden, Bensky and Adams2016; Baumert et al., Reference Baumert, Schmitt, Perugini, Johnson, Blum, Borkenau and Wrzus2017; Renner et al., Reference Renner, Klee and von Oertzen2020). For example, a person being an extrovert (as referring to a temporally stable personality trait) will behave differently depending on the situation, and on what mood they are in, whether they are tired or attentive, etc. Such a distribution of the varied behaviors could fruitfully be thought of along the lines of Fleeson’s whole trait model (Fleeson & Jayawickreme, Reference Fleeson and Jayawickreme2015): we can think of a personality trait as the parameters describing the shape of the distribution of a measurable state/behavior that is expressed transiently in different contexts. How personality expresses itself in behavior, through layers of cognitive and affective processing, accounts for the width of the distribution. Density distributions that are very narrow across very different contexts would argue against an effect of personality because the behavior is insufficiently sensitive to context. On the other hand, too broad density distributions that show extreme variability over context would make it difficult to discover a stable pattern. Animal and human personality should show distributions intermediate between these extremes: temporally stable personality traits are inferred from behaviors that are nonetheless sensitive to different contexts. Of course, it is not merely the density distribution per se, but consistent patterns, and interpretable changes in patterns over time, that are the hallmark of context-sensitive personality (Revelle & Condon, Reference Revelle and Condon2015; Revelle & Wilt, Reference Revelle, Wilt and Rauthmann2021).

In addition to measuring behavior across a range of different contexts in which personality can manifest, other major practical challenges for the study of animal personality include accessibility, sample size, and ecological validity. In humans, we derive personality traits across a typically large and representative population, but many animal studies will be limited in both sample size and representativeness (cf. Table 1). For instance, two mice might have somewhat different behavioral patterns, but we would need to study many more before concluding that these are indeed behavioral patterns explained by personality, or merely individual differences due primarily to external environmental factors that do not generalize to other mice. In addition, personality in laboratory mice will depend on the strain and differ from wild animals (Broadhurst, Reference Broadhurst1975; Blanchard et al., Reference Blanchard, Hebert, Ferrari, Palanza, Figueira, Blanchard and Parmigiani1998; Crabbe et al., Reference Crabbe, Wahlsten and Dudek1999; Augustsson & Meyerson, Reference Augustsson and Meyerson2004). It will be helpful for the field to have a common set of approaches (perhaps including common tasks and observational methods (Kaiser & Müller, Reference Kaiser and Müller2021) as well as common algorithms for analysis, such as tests of reliability and coherence), together with shared databases. This will help accumulate knowledge across larger and larger numbers of animals, and across species. As with other such approaches, funding initiatives aimed at consortia to make this possible would be important. Box 1 summarizes some of the challenges that distinguish the study of animal personality.

Box 1. Criteria for animal personality

  1. 1. Distinguishing personality traits from biological or neurological traits. We urge that the term “personality” be reserved for traits in organisms with behaviors from which we can infer sufficiently rich set of psychological processes. In principle, “personality” is used to explain individual differences not only in behavior but also in other psychological processes (as is the case in humans). Phenotypic traits like eye color or neurological traits like tremor are not examples of personality traits because they are not psychological.

  2. 2. Accounting for context. Although personalities may themselves be temporally stable, how they express themselves in behavior in different contexts depends on how they act through other psychological processes. A given personality can influence an animal’s emotion, attention, or decision-making. But so can different contexts, generating distributions of states/measurable behaviors from which personality can be inferred (along the lines of Fleeson’s whole trait theory (Fleeson & Jayawickreme, Reference Fleeson and Jayawickreme2015)).

  3. 3. Not all contexts will be equally informative about personality. More open-ended and unpredictable situations may elicit behaviors from which personality can best be inferred – but they should not be so open-ended as to bury reliable behavioral patterns in noise. Social contexts may be a particularly good class to use here.

  4. 4. Building a cumulative science of animal personality. Piecemeal studies make comparisons difficult. We urge that the field adopt standards for measuring behaviors, for analysis, and for validation. Ideally, behavioral measures should span a broad range of contexts over a relatively long time. Analyses should include cross-validation and aim for out-of-sample generalizability. We wish to stress the importance of building towards large-scale and integrated projects that can eventually provide uniform datasets and methods, as has been emphasized in reproducibility efforts across psychology (OPEN SCIENCE COLLABORATION, 2015), ecology (Gould et al. Reference Gould, Fraser, Parker, Nakagawa, Griffith, Vesk, Fidler, Hamilton, Abbey-Lee, Abbott, Aguirre, Alcaraz, Aloni, Altschul, Arekar, Atkins, Atkinson, Baker, Barrett and Zitomer2023), and neuroscience (Niso et al. Reference Niso, Botvinik-Nezer, Appelhoff, De La Vega, Esteban, Etzel, Finc, Ganz, Gau, Halchenko, Herholz, Karakuzu, Keator, Markiewicz, Maumet, Pernet, Pestilli, Queder, Schmitt and Rieger2022).

  5. 5. Weighing theory-driven and data-driven personality models. Biological theories that draw on ecology, evolution, and neuroscience could scaffold personality theories in animals that can be tested further with data-driven approaches.

3. Current personality frameworks

One approach to animal personality applies extant (human) personality theories (Table 2) to animals. There are a number of theories on personality dimensions in humans, from Cattell’s 16 (Cattell, Reference Cattell1945; Heather et al., Reference Heather, Cattell, Mead, Boyle, Matthews and Saklofske2008) to the Big Five (McCrae & Costa, Reference McCrae, Costa, Boyle, Matthews and Saklofske2008) to Eysenck’s three (Eysenck, Reference Eysenck1991), as examples. While there are other criteria that might decide in favor of one of these over another (e.g., biological evidence for the reality of a particular dimension), from a statistical point of view they could be compatible with one another: a full trait space would have as many dimensions as there are items or measures, but any one of a number of dimensionality reductions (using factor analysis or principal component analysis, for instance) could compress this to an arbitrary number of fewer dimensions depending on the desired amount of represented variation. In this view, there is no unique personality space, and translation between different spaces is generally straightforward as long as each of them is based on a sufficiently complete set of measures at the outset (Markon et al., Reference Markon, Krueger and Watson2005; Ludeke et al., Reference Ludeke, Bainbridge, Liu, Zhao, Smillie and Zettler2019; Bainbridge et al., Reference Bainbridge, Ludeke and Smillie2022).

Table 2. Theories of human personality, brief description, and rating of applicability to animal studies (0 = not applicable, 1 = applicable with substantial modification, 2 = applicable with minor modification)

Given the challenges of collecting sufficient observational data from animals, our overarching recommendation for applying extant theories of human personality to behavioral data from animals is to begin small. One might characterize a particular facet/dimension of personality, such as timidness vs. boldness, and then gradually build out from there to a more complete personality once sufficient data are available. In general, we would recommend sticking with one of the most widely accepted theories on personality dimensions, such as the Big-Five, and then to see if perhaps one or more of the dimensions drop out because they have too sparse observations or they seem to be absorbed by another dimension. As can be seen in Table 1, most studies of animal personality offer a relatively small number of dimensions that are often a subset of those in the Big Five, or an amalgamation of some of its factors. Once a study has undertaken such a characterization of a nonhuman species’ personality, however limited, it would then be important to see how well it predicts behaviors to a future situation and how well it generalizes across time and individuals. Using existing human personality theories as the approach could observe animal behavior and score it along the dimensions of the theory as well as design specific situations or tasks intended to assess a particular dimension. Testing across multiple contexts would also be important, as noted above, and emphasizes the need for comprehensive and well-curated data sharing among studies. In general, these approaches have confirmed that at least some dimensions commonly used to describe human personality also apply to nonhuman animals; for example, studies of chimpanzees and bears (Table 1) have used the Big Five (Table 2).

Another approach comes from the study of animal behavior itself, which also offers theories to explain the behavior, but from the perspective of ethology rather than derivation from theories of human personality. Some examples come from careful studies of animal behavior by trained human observers. When studying boldness and exploration, Fox et al. (Reference Fox, Ladage, Roth and Pravosudov2009) discovered that mountain chickadees’ behaviors in their exploration of a novel environment and a novel object are uncorrelated, suggesting two independent personality dimensions and contradicting other studies (Carter et al., Reference Carter, Feeney, Marshall, Cowlishaw and Heinsohn2013). In personality studies of chimpanzees, social dominance and dominance-related activities play a pervasive role, conceptualized as a sixth factor distinct from the Big Five in humans (King & Figueredo, Reference King and Figueredo1997). As can be seen from just these brief examples, the ethological factors typically used to characterize variability in some aspects of behavior tend to differ between species and can differ from those popular in humans.

Extant theories of both human and animal personality as described above all have dimensions that are readily interpretable. But all these approaches raise a nagging worry: they are ultimately very much the design and interpretation of humans. An advantage is that we can understand the dimensions used to characterize animal personality. The worry is that we are anthropomorphizing. This brings us to data-driven approaches that can aim to minimize the risk of anthropomorphizing and that could suggest entirely new personality dimensions we might never have thought of intuitively.

4. Data-driven approaches to animal personality

The behavioral data collected for a data-driven approach should be as complete as possible over a long time in varied environments to ensure that all variation in behavior is sampled (Uher, Reference Uher2008; Uher et al., Reference Uher, Asendorpf and Call2008). By applying a data-driven algorithm on such a large high-dimensional dataset, one can extract factors/dimensions or categories. If based on sufficiently diverse and ecologically valid behaviors, this approach can generate personality dimensions from nonhuman animal data that may be quite distinct from those featured in human personality theories. In principle, this could allow us to obtain species-specific personality dimensions suited to a particular nonhuman species’ behaviors, avoiding anthropomorphism (Uher, Reference Uher2008).

A recent example of such a data-driven approach comes from a study by Forkosh et al. (Reference Forkosh, Karamihalev, Roeh, Alon, Anpilov, Touma and Chen2019) in mice (Figure 1). Mouse behaviors were recorded over multiple days in a rich environment (Figure 1B). From video recordings, 60 behavioral features were collected based on location tracking, including social behaviors (Figure 1C). Linear discriminant analysis (LDA) (Figure 1A) was applied to reduce the 60 behavioral-feature dimensions into 4 stable so-called “identity domains” (ID, analogous to personality dimensions) by maximizing the ratio of between-mice to within-mouse variability. These 4 identity domains (Figure 1C) were shown to be replicable and stable over developmental stages and social contexts using separate validation data, and the 4 IDs were shown to capture transcriptomic variance in the brain and variance in genetically driven behavioral differences.

Figure 1. Data-driven inference of personality in mice. A. Linear discriminant analysis (LDA) was used for dimensionality reduction by maximizing the ratio of between-subject to within-subject variability. B. Schematic of the enriched group-housing environment. C. Running LDA on 60 behavioral-feature dimensions (showing 13 representative dimensions) collected from video recordings resulted in four validated identity domains (ID1 - ID4), corresponding to personality dimensions. The IDs themselves are uncorrelated. The width of blue and red connecting lines indicates the strength of the correlation between the four IDs and the 60 behavioral-feature dimensions. For instance, the first personality factor, ID1, is positively correlated with “Chase” and negatively correlated with “Escape”.

Adapted with permission from “Identity domains capture individual differences from across the behavioral repertoire” by Forkosh et al., 2009. Copyright 2019 by The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature America, Inc.

Some of the tools developed for data-driven personality studies in animals could fruitfully be applied to humans as well. For instance, densely sampled data are now available from social media, such as smartphone usage. While studies have generally simply attempted to map such data onto extant personality theories such as the Big-Five (Stachl et al., Reference Stachl, Au, Schoedel, Gosling, Harari, Buschek and Bühner2020), one could imagine using LDA instead and comparing the results to the four identity domains discovered in the mouse study above.

There may be no clear answer whether to use more “top-down” approaches that start with theorized personality dimensions (as suggested in Section 3 above) or more “bottom-up” approaches that depend on a relatively complete sampling of neural or behavioral data, as suggested in the current section. Uher (Reference Uher2008) has provided a useful taxonomy of the different approaches to animal personality that illustrates the diversity available. Our own preference would be to use a mixture of “top-down” and “bottom-up” approaches, with each being used to inform and revise the other. As we mentioned, a main challenge is the number of samples of observational data available. While semi-automated methods are now making very large datasets from quite diverse situations possible (as in the Forkosh et al., Reference Forkosh, Karamihalev, Roeh, Alon, Anpilov, Touma and Chen2019 study) and thus enabling the discovery of strongly data-driven personality dimensions, ecologically based approaches have also been successful in characterizing animal personality. For instance, studies of fear and anxiety have been inventive in constructing situations in the lab that aim to mimic relevant parameters encountered in the wild (e.g., Blanchard & Blanchard, Reference Blanchard, Blanchard, Blanchard, Griebel and Nutt2008; Kumar et al., Reference Kumar, Bhat and Kumar2013; Mobbs & Kim, Reference Mobbs and Kim2015).

5. Concluding comments

The motivations for studying personality in nonhuman animals go well beyond our curiosity and interest in understanding animal behavior per se. Animals offer powerful models for investigating the biological and genetic basis of personality (Gosling, Reference Gosling2001), for understanding how it may have evolved (Dall et al., Reference Dall, Houston and McNamara2004, Reference Dall, Bell, Bolnick and Ratnieks2012), and for explaining pathology, topics we have not discussed here for reasons of space. However, we want to stress that neuroscience (and biology more broadly) certainly should figure both in the creation of hypotheses and in the interpretation of results. In this respect, we see biologically inspired theories of personality, such as the line of work from Pavlov through Eysenck to Gray (see Corr & Perkins, Reference Corr and Perkins2006, for an overview), as excellent candidates also for animal personality.

Complex behavior in both human and nonhuman animals requires psychology for explanation. We have stressed that animal personality ought to be considered as one component to explain complex behavior. This means that there must be at least some other psychological processes that are also influenced by personality and contribute to explanation of complex behavior, analogous to the “thinking” and “feeling” that figure in theories of human personality. We think that these other psychological processes will include processes such as affect and cognition, but it is important to stress that we simply do not yet have a full theory of animal psychology available. Many more studies will be needed to thoroughly characterize animal behavior in rich environments. Building a mature science of animal personality will go hand in hand with building a mature science of animal psychology.

Footnotes

This is a contribution to the special issue on animal personality.

References

Arden, R., Bensky, M. K., & Adams, M. J. (2016). A review of cognitive abilities in dogs, 1911 through 2016: More individual differences, please! Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 307312. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416667718 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Augustsson, H., & Meyerson, B. J. (2004). Exploration and risk assessment: A comparative study of male house mice (Mus musculus musculus) and two laboratory strains. Physiology & Behavior, 81, 685698. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.03.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bainbridge, T. F., Ludeke, S. G., & Smillie, L. D. (2022). Evaluating the Big Five as an organizing framework for commonly used psychological trait scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122, 749777. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000395 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. American Psychologist, 33, 344358. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.33.4.344 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 164180. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00011.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumert, A., Schmitt, M., Perugini, M., Johnson, W., Blum, G., Borkenau, P., … Wrzus, C. (2017). Integrating personality structure, personality process, and personality development. European Journal of Personality, 31, 503528. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2115 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, D., & Blanchard, R. (2008). Defensive behaviors, fear, and anxiety. In Blanchard, R., Griebel, G. & Nutt, D. (Eds.), Handbook of behavioral neuroscience (1st ed., vol. 17, pp. 6379). Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier B. V.Google Scholar
Blanchard, R. J., Hebert, M. A., Ferrari, P., Palanza, P., Figueira, R., Blanchard, D. C., & Parmigiani, S. (1998). Defensive behaviors in wild and laboratory (Swiss) mice: The mouse defense test battery. Physiology & Behavior, 65, 201209. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00012-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G., & Saklofske, D. H. (2008). The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment: Personality Theories and Models (vol. 1). London, England: SAGE Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
Briffa, M., & Greenaway, J. (2011). High in situ repeatability of behaviour indicates animal personality in the Beadlet Anemone Actinia equina (Cnidaria). PLOS ONE, 6, e21963. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021963 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broadhurst, P. L. (1975). The Maudsley Reactive and Nonreactive strains of rats: A survey. Behavior Genetics, 5, 299319. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01073201 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, C., & Braithwaite, V. A. (2004). Size matters: A test of boldness in eight populations of the poeciliid Brachyraphis episcopi. Animal Behaviour, 68, 13251329. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.04.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carslake, C., Occhiuto, F., Vázquez-Diosdado, J. A., & Kaler, J. (2022). Indication of a personality trait in dairy calves and its link to weight gain through automatically collected feeding behaviours. Scientific Reports, 12, 19425. Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-24076-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, A. J., Feeney, W. E., Marshall, H. H., Cowlishaw, G., & Heinsohn, R. (2013). Animal personality: What are behavioural ecologists measuring? Biological Reviews, 88, 465475. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, R. B. (1945). The description of personality: Principles and findings in a factor analysis. The American Journal of Psychology, 58, 6990. https://doi.org/10.2307/1417576 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cloninger, C. R. (1986). A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatric Developments, 4, 167226.Google ScholarPubMed
Coon, D., & Mitterer, J. O. (2012). Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior with Concept Maps and Reviews (13th ed.). Boston, MA: Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Corr, P. J., & Perkins, A. M. (2006). The role of theory in the psychophysiology of personality: From Ivan Pavlov to Jeffrey Gray. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 62, 367376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.01.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crabbe, J. C., Wahlsten, D., & Dudek, B. C. (1999). Genetics of mouse behavior: Interactions with laboratory environment. Science, 284, 16701672. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.284.5420.1670 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dall, S. R. X., Bell, A. M., Bolnick, D. I., & Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2012). An evolutionary ecology of individual differences. Ecology Letters, 15, 11891198. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2012.01846.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dall, S. R. X., Houston, A. I., & McNamara, J. M. (2004). The behavioural ecology of personality: Consistent individual differences from an adaptive perspective. Ecology Letters, 7, 734739. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00618.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual Review of Psychology, 41, 417440. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, N. J., Wright, J., Kazem, A. J. N., Thomas, D. K., Hickling, R., & Dawnay, N. (2007). Behavioural syndromes differ predictably between 12 populations of three-spined Stickleback. Journal of Animal Ecology, 76, 11281138. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2007.01284.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eysenck, H. J. (1991). Dimensions of personality: 16, 5 or 3?—Criteria for a taxonomic paradigm. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 773790. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(91)90144-Z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feist, G. J., Roberts, T.-A., & Feist, J. (2021). Theories of Personality (10th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Education.Google Scholar
Fleeson, W., & Jayawickreme, E. (2015). Whole trait theory. Journal of Research in Personality, 56, 8292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.10.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forkosh, O., Karamihalev, S., Roeh, S., Alon, U., Anpilov, S., Touma, C., … Chen, A. (2019). Identity domains capture individual differences from across the behavioral repertoire. Nature Neuroscience, 22, 20232028. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0516-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, R. A., Ladage, L. D., Roth, T. C., & Pravosudov, V. V. (2009). Behavioural profile predicts dominance status in mountain chickadees, Poecile gambeli. Animal Behaviour, 77, 14411448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.02.022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frost, A. J., Winrow-Giffen, A., Ashley, P. J., & Sneddon, L. U. (2007). Plasticity in animal personality traits: Does prior experience alter the degree of boldness? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274, 333339. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2006.3751 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geller, L. (1982). The failure of self-actualization theory: A critique of Carl Rogers and Abraham Maslow. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 22, 5673. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022167882222004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosling, S. D. (2001). From mice to men: What can we learn about personality from animal research? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 4586. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.45 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gosling, S. D., Kwan, V. S. Y., & John, O. P. (2003). A dog’s got personality: A cross-species comparative approach to personality judgments in dogs and humans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 11611169. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1161 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, E., Fraser, H. S., Parker, T. H., Nakagawa, S., Griffith, S. C., Vesk, P. A., Fidler, F., Hamilton, D. G., Abbey-Lee, R. N., Abbott, J. K., Aguirre, L. A., Alcaraz, C., Aloni, I., Altschul, D., Arekar, K., Atkins, J. W., Atkinson, J., Baker, C., Barrett, M., … Zitomer, R. A. (2023). Same data, different analysts: Variation in effect sizes due to analytical decisions in ecology and evolutionary biology. EcoEvoRxiv. https://doi.org/10.32942/X2GG62 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heather, E. P., Cattell, R. B., & Mead, A. D. (2008). The Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF). In Boyle, G. J., Matthews, G. & Saklofske, D. H. (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment: Personality Measurement and Testing (vol. 2, pp. 134159). London: SAGE Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
Hensley, N. M., Cook, T. C., Lang, M., Petelle, M. B., & Blumstein, D. T. (2012). Personality and habitat segregation in giant sea anemones (Condylactis gigantea). Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 426–427, 14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2012.05.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogan, R., & Blickle, G. (2018). Socioanalytic theory: Basic concepts, supporting evidence and practical implications. In Zeigler-Hill, V. & Shackelford, T. K. (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of personality and individual differences: The science of personality and individual differences (pp. 110129). London: SAGE Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781526451163.n5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, M. I., & Müller, C. (2021). What is an animal personality? Biology & Philosophy, 36, 1. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-020-09776-w CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, J. E., & Figueredo, A. J. (1997). The five-factor model plus dominance in Chimpanzee personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 257271. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.1997.2179 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, J. E., & Landau, V. I. (2003). Can chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) happiness be estimated by human raters? Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 115. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00527-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, J. E., Weiss, A., & Farmer, K. H. (2005). A Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) analogue of cross-national generalization of personality structure: Zoological parks and an African sanctuary. Journal of Personality, 73, 389410. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00313.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kralj-Fišer, S., & Schuett, W. (2014). Studying personality variation in invertebrates: Why bother? Animal Behaviour, 91, 4152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.02.016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kumar, V., Bhat, Z. A., & Kumar, D. (2013). Animal models of anxiety: A comprehensive review. Journal of Pharmacological and Toxicological Methods, 68, 175183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.vascn.2013.05.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurvers, R. H. J. M., Eijkelenkamp, B., van Oers, K., van Lith, B., van Wieren, S. E., Ydenberg, R. C., & Prins, H. H. T. (2009). Personality differences explain leadership in barnacle geese. Animal Behaviour, 78, 447453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lauber, M. C. Y., Hemsworth, P. H., & Barnett, J. L. (2006). The effects of age and experience on behavioural development in dairy calves. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 99, 4152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.10.009 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López, P., Hawlena, D., Polo, V., Amo, L., & Martin, J. (2005). Sources of individual shy-bold variations in antipredator behaviour of male Iberian rock lizards. Animal Behaviour, 69, 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.010 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ludeke, S. G., Bainbridge, T. F., Liu, J., Zhao, K., Smillie, L. D., & Zettler, I. (2019). Using the Big Five Aspect Scales to translate between the HEXACO and Big Five personality models. Journal of Personality, 87, 10251038. https://doi.org/10.1111/jopy.12453 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., & Watson, D. (2005). Delineating the structure of normal and abnormal personality: An integrative hierarchical approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 139157. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.139 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maskrey, D. K., Sneddon, L. U., Arnold, K. E., Wolfenden, D. C. C., & Thomson, J. S. (2021). Temperature-driven changes in behavioural unpredictability and personality in the beadlet sea anemone, Actinia equina. Animal Behaviour, 181, 1327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2021.08.022 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (2008). Empirical and theoretical status of the Five-Factor Model of personality traits. In Boyle, G., Matthews, G. & Saklofske, D. (Eds.), The SAGE Handbook of personality theory and assessment: Volume 1—Personality theories and models (pp. 273294). London: SAGE Publications Ltd. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781849200462.n13 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, K. A., Garner, J. P., & Mench, J. A. (2005). The test–retest reliability of four behavioural tests of fearfulness for quail: A critical evaluation. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 92, 113127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.10.016 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirkó, E., Dóka, A., & Miklósi, Á. (2013). Association between subjective rating and behaviour coding and the role of experience in making video assessments on the personality of the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 149, 4554. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.10.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mischel, W. (1996). Personality and Assessment (1st ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203763643 Google Scholar
Mobbs, D., & Kim, J. J. (2015). Neuroethological studies of fear, anxiety, and risky decision-making in rodents and humans. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 5, 815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.06.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niso, G., Botvinik-Nezer, R., Appelhoff, S., De La Vega, A., Esteban, O., Etzel, J. A., Finc, K., Ganz, M., Gau, R., Halchenko, Y. O., Herholz, P., Karakuzu, A., Keator, D. B., Markiewicz, C. J., Maumet, C., Pernet, C. R., Pestilli, F., Queder, N., Schmitt, T., … Rieger, J. W. (2022). Open and reproducible neuroimaging: From study inception to publication. NeuroImage, 263, 119623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119623 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
OPEN SCIENCE COLLABORATION. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716 Google Scholar
Pastorino, G. Q., Christodoulides, Y., Curone, G., Pearce-Kelly, P., Faustini, M., Albertini, M., … Mazzola, S. M. (2017). Behavioural profiles of brown and sloth bears in captivity. Animals, 7, 39. Article 5. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani7050039 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pederson, A. K., King, J. E., & Landau, V. I. (2005). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality predicts behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 534549. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2004.07.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renner, K.-H., Klee, S., & von Oertzen, T. (2020). Bringing back the person into behavioural personality science using big data. European Journal of Personality, 34, 670686. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2303 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W. (2007). Experimental approaches to the study of personality. In Robins, R. W., Fraley, R. C. & Krueger, R. F. (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology (pp. 3761). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Revelle, W., & Condon, D. M. (2015). A model for personality at three levels. Journal of Research in Personality, 56, 7081. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.12.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revelle, W., & Wilt, J. (2021). The history of dynamic approaches to personality. In Rauthmann, J. F. (Ed.), The Handbook of Personality Dynamics and Processes (pp. 331). London: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-813995-0.00001-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, B. W., & Mroczek, D. (2008). Personality trait change in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 3135. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80, 128. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0092976 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stachl, C., Au, Q., Schoedel, R., Gosling, S. D., Harari, G. M., Buschek, D., … Bühner, M. (2020). Predicting personality from patterns of behavior collected with smartphones. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 117, 1768017687. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1920484117 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strelau, J. (1996). The regulative theory of temperament: Current status. Personality and Individual Differences, 20, 131142. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(95)00159-X CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strelau, J. (2008). Temperament as a regulator of behavior: After fifty years of research. New York, NY: Eliot Werner Publications.Google Scholar
Uher, J. (2008). Comparative personality research: Methodological approaches. European Journal of Personality, 22, 427455. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.680 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uher, J., Asendorpf, J. B., & Call, J. (2008). Personality in the behaviour of great apes: Temporal stability, cross-situational consistency and coherence in response. Animal Behaviour, 75, 99112. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.04.018 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, R. N., & Cummins, R. A. (1976). The open-field test: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 482504. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.83.3.482 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilt, J., & Revelle, W. (2015). Affect, behavior, cognition, and desire in the Big Five: An analysis of item content and structure. European Journal of Personality, 29, 478497. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. A nonexhaustive list of research on animal personality after 2001. See Gosling (2001) for a list of research on animal personality before 2001. See Kralj-Fiser and Schuett (2014) for a list of additional invertebrate studies

Figure 1

Table 2. Theories of human personality, brief description, and rating of applicability to animal studies (0 = not applicable, 1 = applicable with substantial modification, 2 = applicable with minor modification)

Figure 2

Figure 1. Data-driven inference of personality in mice. A. Linear discriminant analysis (LDA) was used for dimensionality reduction by maximizing the ratio of between-subject to within-subject variability. B. Schematic of the enriched group-housing environment. C. Running LDA on 60 behavioral-feature dimensions (showing 13 representative dimensions) collected from video recordings resulted in four validated identity domains (ID1 - ID4), corresponding to personality dimensions. The IDs themselves are uncorrelated. The width of blue and red connecting lines indicates the strength of the correlation between the four IDs and the 60 behavioral-feature dimensions. For instance, the first personality factor, ID1, is positively correlated with “Chase” and negatively correlated with “Escape”.Adapted with permission from “Identity domains capture individual differences from across the behavioral repertoire” by Forkosh et al., 2009. Copyright 2019 by The Author(s), under exclusive license to Springer Nature America, Inc.