Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T11:58:42.211Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Shining a light on parasite behaviour: daily patterns of Argulus fish lice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 March 2021

Rhi Hunt*
Affiliation:
School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, CF10 3AX, UK
Jo Cable
Affiliation:
School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, CF10 3AX, UK
Amy Ellison
Affiliation:
School of Natural Sciences, Bangor University, Deiniol Road, Bangor, GwyneddLL57 2UW, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Rhi Hunt, E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Parasites display a wide range of behaviours that are frequently overlooked in favour of host responses. Understanding these behaviours can improve parasite control through a more precise application or development of new behaviour-based strategies. In aquaculture fish lice are an ongoing problem, infections reduce fishery production and control options are limited. Fish lice are distinct in their ability to survive and swim off hosts, allowing the transmission to multiple fish hosts across their lifespan. Here we assessed the off-host behaviour of Argulus foliaceus (a freshwater fish louse) and observed a diurnal rhythmical pattern in their behaviour. This pattern was lost when lice were exposed to constant darkness, indicating that the behaviour is not endogenously driven. Males were consistently active in light with reduced activity in darkness. In contrast, females were active during light and dark phases with peak activity at the start of dark periods. A. foliaceus was also strongly attracted to a light stimulus, preferring white- and blue-coloured lights over green- or red-coloured lights. Light is a strong driver of fish louse activity and could be used to trap parasites. Aquaculture light regimes could also be altered to reduce parasite attraction and activity.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Parasites are a fundamental component of ecosystems; practically all known species carry parasites and food webs can be dominated by their presence (Marcogliese and Cone, Reference Marcogliese and Cone1997; Poulin and Morand, Reference Poulin and Morand2000; Lafferty et al., Reference Lafferty, Dobson and Kuris2006; Dobson et al., Reference Dobson, Lafferty, Kuris, Hechinger and Jetz2008). In addition to their ecological importance, parasitic infections play a critical role in the global health of humans and both domesticated and wild species. The conflict between humans and parasites drives the development and use of control strategies to prevent and reduce the health and socio-economic impacts of infection. Understanding behaviour can aid the development and employment of control strategies, but research tends to focus on the host rather than parasite behaviours (Barnard, Reference Barnard1990; Sukhdeo and Chappell, Reference Sukhdeo and Chappell1994; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Campbell and Sukhdeo2002). This is despite the fact that parasites have developed a wide range of complex behaviours to facilitate transmission, infection, reproduction and survival (Rea and Irwin, Reference Rea and Irwin1994; Sukhdeo and Chappell, Reference Sukhdeo and Chappell1994; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Campbell and Sukhdeo2002). Behaviours involved in host finding are of particular interest regarding the development of control strategies to interrupt and prevent infection. Many parasites have adopted ‘active’ host finding behaviours to locate suitable hosts, whereby a parasite responds to environmental and/or host signals (Rea and Irwin, Reference Rea and Irwin1994). Parasites utilize a range of stimuli (such as chemical, thermal, mechanical and visual), often in combination to locate hosts and assess their suitability (Van Leerdam et al., Reference Van Leerdam, Smith and Fuchs1985; Ashton et al., Reference Ashton, Li and Schad1999; Bailey et al., Reference Bailey, Birkett, Ingvarsdóttir, Mordue (Luntz), Mordue, O'Shea, Pickett and Wadhams2006; Mordue (Luntz) and Birkett, Reference Mordue (Luntz) and Birkett2009).

Organisms can temporally synchronize to their environment by detecting and responding to external cues, resulting in biological rhythms of physiology and behaviour (Vitaterna et al., Reference Vitaterna, Takahashi and Turek2001; Bell-Pedersen et al., Reference Bell-Pedersen, Cassone, Earnest, Golden, Hardin, Thomas and Zoran2005). Light–dark cycles are the dominant cue for a majority of organisms, however for parasites both environmental and host cues influence rhythmicity (Bell-Pedersen et al., Reference Bell-Pedersen, Cassone, Earnest, Golden, Hardin, Thomas and Zoran2005; Reece et al., Reference Reece, Prior and Mideo2017). By synchronizing with hosts, parasites can increase their survival. During dispersal and transmission, rhythms allow parasites to maximize infection success by optimizing the presence of infective stages with host availability (Sukhdeo and Chappell, Reference Sukhdeo and Chappell1994; Bogéa et al., Reference Bogéa, Favre, Rotenberg, Silva and Pieri1996). In addition, infection success and parasite survival can be influenced by fluctuations (daily and/or seasonal) in host immune responses (Martinez-Bakker and Helm, Reference Martinez-Bakker and Helm2015; Kiessling et al., Reference Kiessling, Dubeau-Laramée, Ohm, Labrecque, Olivier and Cermakian2017; Carvalho Cabral et al., Reference Carvalho Cabral, Olivier and Cermakian2019). Identification of cues used by parasites and the rhythms they exhibit could help reduce infection and transmission risks; for example, by avoiding/preventing access to locations during peak parasite presence or deploying control measures at such times to maximize capture.

The environmental and/or host cues utilized by parasites can differ between life stage or sexes. While this can reduce the efficacy of broad control applications and induce bias, it can also be used for highly targeted control. Sex-specific control schemes have been employed to successfully reduce parasite populations or their vectors. Females can be targeted to reduce the next generation by directing removing reproducers, while male targeting uses sterilization and release techniques to lower population fecundity (Alphey et al., Reference Alphey, Benedict, Bellini, Clark, Dame, Service and Dobson2010; Epsky et al., Reference Epsky, Hendrichs, Katsoyannos, Vásquez, Ros, Zümreoglu, Pereira, Bakri, Seewooruthun and Heath1999). Discrete sexes can be caught using sex-specific behaviours such as pheromone or food-based attraction (Epsky et al., Reference Epsky, Hendrichs, Katsoyannos, Vásquez, Ros, Zümreoglu, Pereira, Bakri, Seewooruthun and Heath1999). These sex-specific behaviours likely lead to sex-specific rhythms, which could also be exploited to further promote control success. Sexual differences in parasite rhythms are yet to be explored (Sikkel et al., Reference Sikkel, Ziemba, Sears and Wheeler2009).

Fish lice are ectoparasitic crustaceans, which are problematic worldwide in fisheries. Control options are limited, with a reduction in chemical applications due to environmental concerns and rising drug resistance (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Sommerville and Wootten2005a; Costello, Reference Costello2009). Recent developments in the control of marine sea lice capitalize on louse behaviour: lice frequently occupy the top of the water column, consequently, fishes are held at >10 m below the sea surface to reduce infection. For freshwater lice (genus Argulus) however, control options remain insufficient with some farmers turning to illegal options (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Sommerville and Wootten2005b). Thus, there is a need to explore alternative, behaviour-based control methods. Argulus spp. are unusual in that they retain the ability to free swim throughout their life cycle with host-switching frequency, especially among male parasites as they seek female partners (Bandilla et al., Reference Bandilla, Hakalahti-Sirén and Valtonen2008). No studies have tested for the presence of endogenous rhythms in Argulus spp. (or any other aquatic ectoparasitic crustacean), although a diurnal pattern is present in the strength of their positive phototaxis response (Yoshizawa and Nogami, Reference Yoshizawa and Nogami2008). Argulus spp. also react to light/dark changes with differing activity, however, this has not been observed over a circadian period or between sexes (Mikheev et al., Reference Mikheev, Mikheev, Pasternak and Valtonen1999).

Here we examine the host-seeking behaviour of a globally problematic fish ectoparasite over a diurnal period, testing for the presence of endogenous cues. Strength of light attraction and wavelength-specific preferences are also assessed to aid control development.

Materials and methods

Parasite and host maintenance

Argulus spp. used in this study were collected from Risca Canal (Newport, UK; grid reference: ST 24344 90686) on 6 June 2018 and 7 August 2019 by hand netting naturally infected 3-spined stickleback Gasterosteus aculeatus. Parasites were removed from fish in the field by lifting the host fish out of the water using a net for a 10 s period; upon re-submersion into a container of freshwater, the parasite detached and was collected using a wide-bore pipette. Argulus spp. were transported to the laboratory off host in sealed containers of dechlorinated water. Once in the Cardiff aquarium, parasites were morphologically identified as Argulus foliaceus (according to Fryer, Reference Fryer1982) and maintained in male: female pairs on 3-spined sticklebacks collected from Roath Brook, Cardiff (ST 18897 78541; an Argulus spp. naïve population). Fish were infected by placing the parasites into fish-holding tanks (9 L) containing an individual stickleback to allow natural parasite attachment. All fish and parasites were maintained under a 12 h light:12 h dark cycle, with fish fed daily with Tubifex bloodworm. Argulus foliaceus were acclimated to laboratory conditions on their hosts for 1 week prior to experimentation. Parasites were not re-used within or across experiments. For the circadian rhythm experiment, both male and non-gravid female parasites were used. For the light attraction/colour preference experiments only male A. foliaceus were used due to higher availability of male parasites vs non-gravid females (as female parasites continuously produce eggs after mating and egg-baring females exhibit egg-laying behaviour when off host).

Argulus foliaceus were removed from sticklebacks for use in experiments using the same collection method as described above. All A. foliaceus were checked visually for damage before use and measured from the rostral edge of the carapace to the anterior end of the abdominal lobes using a dissecting microscope at 10× magnification with a Lumenera Infinity 1 camera and Infinity Capture software version 6.5.4.

The experimental procedures in this study conform to the accepted principles of animal welfare in experimental science and used the minimum number of animals required to produce statistically reproducible results. All animal work was approved by the Cardiff University's Animal Ethics Committee, followed ARRIVE guidelines and was conducted under Home Office License PPL 303424.

Circadian rhythm of parasite swimming activity off host

To understand how A. foliaceus behaves off host/during transmission over a circadian period, individual adult male and non-gravid female A. foliaceus (males: N = 22, average size = 3.93 mm ± 0.23 s.d. and females: N = 18, average size = 4.43 mm ± 0.44 s.d.) were placed into glass Petri dishes (10 cm diameter) filled with 50 mL dechlorinated water. The water level in the Petri dishes was sufficient to allow full horizontal movement while minimizing vertical motion for behavioural tracking. Additionally, the sides of each dish were covered with white fabric to reduce reflections and prevent visual disturbance (Mikheev et al., Reference Mikheev, Valtonen and Rintamäki-Kinnunen1998). Parasites were then subjected to 12 h light:12 h dark (LD; average, 1000 lux) for 48 h, after which they were removed from the setup, given 1 day of recovery on stickleback hosts (to allow feeding/prevent starvation) before returning to the setup for another 48 h under total darkness (DD). The order of light condition (12:12 LD vs DD) could not be randomized as the total darkness regime would disrupt any entrained circadian rhythm, altering any tests post-exposure. The setup was completely reset between trials and light condition tests. Parasitic behaviour was recorded during the 48 h exposures via 24 h infrared CCTV cameras (Sentient Pro HDA DVR 8 Channel CCTV, Maplin). Every 4 h (zeitgeber time = ZT, ZT0 = 7 am, ZT4 = 11 am, ZT8 = 3 pm, ZT12 = 7 pm, ZT16 = 11 pm, ZT20 = 3 am; lights were on at ZT0 and off at ZT12) the total distance covered by the parasite and subsequent average swimming speed was calculated over a 2 min period using ImageJ version 1.51j8 (Schneider et al., Reference Schneider, Rasband and Eliceiri2012) to prepare video files for analysis and Kinovea version 0.8.27 (Ganni et al., Reference Ganni, Botden, Chmarra, Goossens and Jakimowicz2018) to track parasite movement. The proportion of time spent swimming was obtained from Kinovea by calculating the time spent swimming at >1 mm s−1 (approximately one-fourth body length). Patterns of parasite activity were then assessed for a 24 h period and between 12:12 LD/DD trials to determine activity and entrainment of rhythm.

Argulus light attraction in the presence of fish hosts

The attraction of A. foliaceus to a light source vs a live fish host was assessed using 2 different behavioural assays: fish vs light trials in which adult male A. foliaceus were given the choice of either white light or a stickleback in darkness over a 24 h period (N = 20 parasites, average size = 4.12 mm ± 0.31 s.d.; Fig. 1A), and lit fish vs dark fish trials offering the choice of stickleback with white light or a stickleback in darkness (N = 18 parasites, average size = 4.14 mm ± 0.35 s.d.; Fig. 1B) over a 2 h period. Arenas comprised of a glass tank filled to 10 cm water depth, split into 3 identical sized sections (left, middle and right) using a 1 cm aperture mesh to allow free movement of parasites while restricting fish movement (Fig. 1A and B). Stimuli were placed into the left and right thirds, with 2 A. foliaceus restrained under a glass dish in the middle third for 30 min to allow acclimation. After acclimation, the lice were released and monitored via infrared CCTV cameras. All light stimuli used a waterproof light-emitting diode (LED) white light (average 50 lux at a distance of 7 cm), while all stimuli in darkness contained the same type of LED white light but turned off to ensure each section had the same structure. The positions of the stimuli were swapped in between trials to avoid any potential side bias. For the lit fish vs dark fish trials, all host pairs were size matched.

Fig. 1. Plan view of experimental areas for Argulus foliaceus (A/B) light vs fish host preference and (C) light colour preference trials. In each arena, circles represent light-emitting diode (LED) light sources. (A) gives a choice of white light vs a 3-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) host with a turned-off light, (B) gives a choice of a white light + stickleback vs a turned off light + stickleback. In (A/B) dashed lines represent 1 cm aperture mesh which allows the parasites to swim through while blocking fish movement. In (C) dotted lines indicate the total area of each coloured corner for behavioural recording, R = red light, G = green, W = white and B = blue (coloured light placement was changed/randomized for each trial).

Argulus light colour preference

To investigate whether certain wavelengths of light are more attractive to A. foliaceus, adult males (N = 20, average size = 4.08 mm ± 0.33 s.d.) were placed individually into the centre of a 2.5 L opaque white square arena (14 × 14 cm) filled with 1 L water (5 cm water depth). The arena was split into 4 equal quarters, with 4 waterproof lights (3 × 3 × 2 cm, LED with RGB colour) placed into the arena and positioned to flush inside each corner (Fig. 1C). Lights were randomly assigned to emit either red (635–700 nm), green (520–560 nm), blue (450–490 nm), or white (emits all wavelengths, 450–700 nm) light, with brightness controlled so that each light individually generated an average 50 lux (lux meter positioned 7 cm away from light). There was no visual overlap in the colours emitted from each light, and initial testing found that parasites did not swim erratically or behave in any other abnormal manner in the experimental arena (following previous observations in the lab and by Mikheev et al., Reference Mikheev, Valtonen and Rintamäki-Kinnunen1998). The inclusion of an acclimation period in initial testing also had no impact on parasite behaviour, thus parasites were observed immediately after introduction to the arena. After being introduced to the centre of the arena, parasites were monitored for 2 min with their time at each colour recorded. Location at a colour was classified as the parasite being present anywhere in the quarter containing the light (with more than half of the body of the parasites being present in the quarter for when the parasite crossed between sections). Parasites were observed live, with the observer stationed next to the arena looking down into the tank. Room lights were turned off so the only light source during experimentation came from the lights in the arena – this provided enough light to observe parasite movement while preventing the casting of shadows into the arena from the observer. Individual parasites were tested 3 times consecutively by calculating the average time spent in each light corner. Parasites experienced a rest period of a few seconds between replicates as the arena was reset and the light position randomized for each replicate. Parasites did not linger or remain stationary on boundary lines between quarters during observations.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted using R statistical software (v3.6.2; R Core Team, 2017) with the level of significance in all tests taken as P < 0.05. Models were refined through stepwise deletion of insignificant terms and AIC comparisons, with a visual examination of model plots to check standardized residuals for normal distribution and homogeneity of variance (Crawley, Reference Crawley2007). The following packages were used for analyses: ‘ggplot2’ to visualize data (Wickham, Reference Wickham2009), ‘lme4’ to run general linear mixed models (GLMMs) (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015), ‘emmeans’ for post hoc analyses (Searle et al., Reference Searle, Speed and Milliken1980), ‘RAIN’ (rhythmicity analysis incorporating nonparametric methods) and ‘MetaCycle’ to determine circadian rhythmicity (Thaben and Westermark, Reference Thaben and Westermark2014; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Anafi, Hughes, Kornacker and Hogenesch2016) and ‘circacompare’ to compare rhythms (Parsons et al., Reference Parsons, Parsons, Garner, Oster and Rawashdeh2020). For all rhythm analysis, the time period being examined was set to 24 h.

To detect rhythmicity, RAIN was used due to its capability in detecting and accounting for asymmetrical patterns (Thaben and Westermark, Reference Thaben and Westermark2014) alongside MetaCycle due to its inclusion of multiple methods for rhythm evaluation (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Anafi, Hughes, Kornacker and Hogenesch2016). The test ‘rainresult’ was used to examine patterns across parasite sex and light condition by examining phase and peak shape. The phase of a rhythm refers to the time point at which a peak occurs, with peak shape the time (in this case: hours) between a peak and the next trough. Comparison of rhythms between different conditions was then carried out using circacompare to assess midline estimating statistic of rhythm (MESOR), amplitude and phase across rhythms. MESOR is a mean value adjusted for circadian rhythms, amplitude refers to ‘a measure of half the extent of predictable variation within a cycle’ (Cornelissen, Reference Cornelissen2014; Otsuka et al., Reference Otsuka, Cornelissen and Halberg2016). A GLMM using only the 12:12 LD data was then conducted to compare activity at each ZT time point by examining A. foliaceus activity against ZT time, parasite sex and length with an interaction between ZT time/parasite sex. This GLMM was then repeated using the DD trials only. All GLMMs used parasite ID as a random factor to account for pseudoreplication. To determine A. foliaceus colour preference, a general linear model was used to compare swimming activity (average over 3 trials) against light colour and parasite length. Across all tests and trials, parasite length had no significant impact and is thus not reported further.

Results

Circadian rhythm of parasite swimming activity off host

A strong diurnal pattern in off-host swimming activity was observed for both male and female A. foliaceus when maintained under 12:12 LD conditions (RAIN P ⩽ 0.001 for both males and females, MetaCycle P ⩽ 0.001/0.004 for males/females respectively; Fig. 2); however under total darkness (DD) this diurnal rhythm was lost (RAIN P = 0.529/0.202, MetaCycle P = 0.894/0.999 for males/females, respectively), suggesting this pattern is stimulated by light and not endogenously driven. Under 12:12 LD, male parasites had different phase to females (circacompare P = 0.018, male phase = 5.69 h post-ZT0, female = 8.56 h), but there was no difference in MESOR or amplitude (circacompare P = 0.290/0.716, respectively; Fig. 3).

Fig. 2. Average swimming speed of Argulus foliaceus off host over a 48 h period under 2 different light conditions: alternating light and dark (A and C) and total darkness (B and D). (A) Male A. foliaceus under 12 h light:12 h dark. (B) Male A. foliaceus under total darkness. (C) Female A. foliaceus under 12 h light:12 h dark. (D) Female A. foliaceus under total darkness. White backgrounds indicate periods of light, dark grey backgrounds indicate periods of darkness. Zeitgeber time (ZT)0 = 7 am, ZT12 = 7 pm.

Fig. 3. Circacompare output plot of male and female Argulus foliaceus swimming speed over a 12:12 light: dark 48 h period. Lights turn on/off at 0/12 and 24/36.

Under 12:12 LD, the overall average swimming speed of A. foliaceus did not differ among sexes (0.77 and 0.83 cm s−1 for males and females, respectively; GLMM P = 0.591), however when directly comparing ZT timepoints females had a significantly higher swimming speed at ZT12 (7 pm when the lights turn off; GLMM P = 0.008; Fig. 2). Under DD, females had marginally significant higher overall activity than males (0.86 cm s−1 for females, 0.62 cm s−1 for males; GLMM P = 0.049). When examining the proportion of time spent swimming, no patterns were observed except for females under DD which showed a peak at ZT0/20 and drop at ZT8/12 (females under DD: Rain P = 0.005, MetaCycle P = 0.037, all other treatments: RAIN P ⩾ 0.456, MetaCycle P ⩾ 0.956; Supplementary Fig. 1).

Argulus light attraction in the presence of fish hosts

When assessing preference between a light stimulus or a fish host, the average time taken for lice to first enter the light section was 59 s. After 24 h, 85% of parasites were located at the light stimulus and the remaining 15% had been consumed by the fish host (time to consumption ranged from 11 to 378 s). No fish became infected during these trials.

For trials assessing preference between a fish host with or without a light source turned on, 100% of parasites moved to the section containing a fish host with a light on. After 2 h, 17% of these parasites had been eaten by the fish, 22% infected the fish and 61% remained swimming around this section.

Argulus light colour preference

Argulusfoliaceus significantly preferred white- and blue-coloured lights over green- or red-coloured lights (all comparisons P ⩽ 0.001, except white vs green in which P = 0.025), with a preference for blue light over white close to significance (P = 0.052; Fig. 4).

Fig. 4. Light preference of male Argulus foliaceus (n = 20) off the host. Average time spent by free-swimming A. foliaceus in the vicinity of different-coloured lights over a 2-min period. Wavelengths of white light = 450–700 nm, blue light = 450–490 nm, green light = 520–560 nm, red light = 635–700 nm.

Discussion

During dispersal, hosts provide a spatially patchy environment in which parasites need to anticipate host availability (Skelton et al., Reference Skelton, Creed and Brown2015). As such, parasites must develop strategies to increase host–parasite contact and facilitate infection and transmission. In many parasites, this involves host-seeking behaviours and synchronization with their hosts. For fish lice, hosts are located by free-swimming parasites responding to host and environmental cues, with light being their dominant stimulus (Bandilla et al., Reference Bandilla, Hakalahti-Sirén and Valtonen2007). While previous studies have recorded variations in fish lice behaviour over diurnal periods (Yoshizawa and Nogami, Reference Yoshizawa and Nogami2008; Heuch et al., Reference Heuch, Parsons and Boxaspen2011), none have determined if these rhythms are endogenously driven. Here A. foliaceus off-host activity followed a diurnal, not endogenous, circadian pattern as the distinct behavioural rhythm under light/dark conditions was lost under total darkness. There was also a sexual difference in off-host behaviour with male and female rhythms offset by ~4 h. When examining light attraction A. foliaceus consistently displayed a strong attraction to light over combined host cues (in the form of a live host) and preferred shorter wavelengths of light.

Argulus display sexually dimorphic host-switching behaviour with males frequently leaving their hosts to find mates while non-gravid females remain on the host (Bandilla et al., Reference Bandilla, Hakalahti-Sirén and Valtonen2008). This dimorphism continues in off-host behaviour. As shown previously by Mikheev et al. (Reference Mikheev, Mikheev, Pasternak and Valtonen1999), female A. foliaceus had the highest activity when the lights turned off and low activity when lights turned on. Examining activity over a circadian period; however, indicates that this is not sustained for 4+ hours after lights turn off female parasite activity drops, and inversely 4+ hours after lights turn on female activity increases. Males do not follow the same pattern with activity consistently higher during light periods and lower during dark periods. The continued high average speed of females when lights turn off (vs a drop-in activity for males) could be related to their host-switching behaviours: females are not predisposed to spending time off host, and thus may not react as quickly as males to light changes. Alternatively, the lights used in this study (and Mikheev et al., Reference Mikheev, Mikheev, Pasternak and Valtonen1999) were turned on/off immediately and could be simulating a passing shadow (a trigger of fish lice activity, Bohn, Reference Bohn1910; Poulin et al., Reference Poulin, Curtis and Rau1990). Females could react stronger than males to potential host cues (due to a higher tendency for females to remain on the host) resulting in high activity when lights turn off. The distinct and strong diurnal rhythm observed when using average swimming speed measurements was not observed when using measurements that only record time spent active. Average swimming speed is more comprehensive accounting for variation in activity, whereas time spent active (i.e. a simple proportion of time moving or not) cannot discern these nuances and would lead to an assumption of arrhythmic behaviour. This highlights the importance of selecting the correct activity measure when assessing rhythmical patterns in behaviour.

Light is an integral component of aquaculture systems, with differing light wavelengths, intensity and photoperiods used to manipulate fish growth and maturation (Boeuf and Le Bail, Reference Boeuf and Le Bail1999; Oppedal et al., Reference Oppedal, Taranger, Juell and Hansen1999; Villamizar et al., Reference Villamizar, Blanco-Vives, Migaud, Davie, Carboni and Sánchez-Vázquez2011). The subsequent impact of these altered light regimes on both fish behaviour and health is now being considered. Recent studies have also found parasitic infection can alter host circadian gene expression, further complicating the relationship between parasites, hosts and the rhythms they both follow (Ellison et al., Reference Ellison, Uren Webster, Rey, Garcia de Leaniz, Consuegra, Orozco-terWengel and Cable2018, Reference Ellison, Uren Webster, Rodriguez-Barreto, de Leaniz, Consuegra, Orozco-terWengel and Cable2020). Considering the positive phototactic response of fish lice, aquaculture lights could attract lice to cages and facilitate infection (Trippel, Reference Trippel2010, Stewart et al., Reference Stewart, Nomura, Piercey, Dunham and Lelliott2013). In this study male A. foliaceus were more active under light vs dark, suggesting lit cages would not only attract lice but also increase their activity which could lead to higher infection success. Shifting the wavelength of light used in aquaculture systems could potentially allow retention of fish manipulation while limiting the impact on pathogenic organisms. For example, when inhibiting Salmo salar sexual maturation to increase production, green and red light treatments used less energy vs white light treatments (Leclercq et al., Reference Leclercq, Taylor, Sprague and Migaud2011). Additionally, Oncorhynchus mykiss raised under red light showed improved growth compared to fish raised under blue or white light (Karakatsouli et al., Reference Karakatsouli, Papoutsoglou, Panopoulos, Papoutsoglou, Chadio and Kalogiannis2008). Red light was the least attractive light colour to A. foliaceus (and A. japonicus: Yoshizawa and Nogami, Reference Yoshizawa and Nogami2008), therefore cages lit with red light could attract less parasites to those lit with shorter wavelengths. This may only be beneficial in outdoor systems where wild parasites enter containers/cages to infect fish, vs enclosed systems where parasites may be trapped in with the fish.

In addition to altering the light regimes in aquaculture to reduce parasite attraction and infection, light could be used to purposefully attract parasites into traps. Light traps have successfully captured sea lice in both the laboratory and field (where, in comparison, plankton tows captured none) and were suggested as a monitoring tool (Novales Flamarique et al., Reference Novales Flamarique, Gulbransen, Galbraith and Stucchi2009). Unlike sea lice which show differing reaction strength to light across their life stages, Argulus spp. appear to be consistent in their light attraction from hatching to adulthood (Bai, Reference Bai1981; Novales Flamarique et al., Reference Novales Flamarique, Browman, Bélanger and Boxaspen2000, Reference Novales Flamarique, Gulbransen, Galbraith and Stucchi2009; Bandilla et al., Reference Bandilla, Hakalahti-Sirén and Valtonen2007). Additionally, freshwater habitats used for aquaculture are often smaller, enclosed areas (e.g. rearing ponds and raceways, recreational fishing lakes and reservoirs) compared to the ocean, potentially increasing the chance of Argulus spp. to encounter traps. Therefore, light traps could be more effective and feasible management tool for freshwater fisheries and aquaculture. Our findings suggest that over relatively short distances lice are strongly attracted to light, therefore future studies should examine the attraction distance of light coupled with trials in freshwater aquaculture systems to determine the efficacy of light traps in controlling lice infections.

Parasite behaviour can be complex and diverse with host cues, external stimulus and diurnal rhythms all affecting parasite activity. When developing control strategies, understanding behaviour allows a more effective application (i.e. during parasite emergence) and offers the potential for identifying new targets for control. Sexual differences are also critical to consider, as differing behaviour could lead to 1 sex avoiding control application. By understanding and manipulating parasites, the impact of infection on global health and economics can be reduced. Parasite behaviour is therefore an important component of management and should be considered for all problematic infections.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0031182021000445

Data

Data will be made available upon request.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Alex Titimeaua for technical assistance during the collection of the circadian data.

Author contributions

RH, JC and AE conceived and designed the study and co-wrote the article. RH gathered all data and performed statistical analyses.

Financial support

RH was supported by a NERC GW4 + Doctoral Training Partnership studentship from the Natural Environment Research Council [NE/L002434/1] in collaboration with a CASE partner, the Environment Agency. AE was supported by a BBSRC Future Leader Fellowship [BB/R010609/1]. Funders were not involved in study design, data collection, interpretation and analysis, manuscript preparation or decision to publish.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare there are no conflicts of interest.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional guides on the care and use of laboratory animals. All animal work was approved by the Cardiff University's Animal Ethics Committee, followed ARRIVE guidelines and was conducted under Home Office License PPL 303424.

References

Alphey, L, Benedict, M, Bellini, R, Clark, GG, Dame, DA, Service, MW and Dobson, SL (2010) Sterile-insect methods for control of mosquito-borne diseases: an analysis. Vector Borne and Zoonotic Diseases 10, 295311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashton, FT, Li, J and Schad, GA (1999) Chemo- and thermosensory neurons: structure and function in animal parasitic nematodes. Veterinary Parasitology 84, 297316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bai, AS (1981) Photic effects on embryonation and phototactic responses by the larvae of Argulus siamensis. Proceedings: Animal Sciences 90, 513517.Google Scholar
Bailey, RJ, Birkett, MA, Ingvarsdóttir, A, Mordue (Luntz), AJ, Mordue, W, O'Shea, B, Pickett, JA and Wadhams, LJ (2006) The role of semiochemicals in host location and non-host avoidance by salmon louse (Lepeophtheirus salmonis) copepodids. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 63, 448456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandilla, M, Hakalahti-Sirén, T and Valtonen, ET (2007) Experimental evidence for a hierarchy of mate- and host-induced cues in a fish ectoparasite, Argulus coregoni (Crustacea: Branchiura). International Journal for Parasitology 37, 13431349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandilla, M, Hakalahti-Sirén, T and Valtonen, ET (2008) Patterns of host switching in the fish ectoparasite Argulus coregoni. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62, 975982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnard, CF (1990) Parasitism and Host Behaviour. London, UK: CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D, Mächler, M, Bolker, B and Walker, S (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67, 148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell-Pedersen, D, Cassone, VM, Earnest, DJ, Golden, SS, Hardin, PE, Thomas, TL and Zoran, MJ (2005) Circadian rhythms from multiple oscillators: lessons from diverse organisms. Nature Reviews Genetics 6, 544556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boeuf, G and Le Bail, P-Y (1999) Does light have an influence on fish growth? Aquaculture 177, 129152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogéa, T, Favre, TC, Rotenberg, L, Silva, HS and Pieri, OS (1996) Circadian pattern of cercarial emergence in Schistosoma mansoni (Platyhelminthes: Digenea) from isolated Biomphalaria glabrata. Chronobiology International 13, 93101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohn, G (1910) Sur les réactions comparées de deux parasites des poissons vis-à-vis de la lumière. Compte-Rendu de l'Association Française Pour l'Avancement des Sciences 38, 726729.Google Scholar
Carvalho Cabral, P, Olivier, M and Cermakian, N (2019) The complex interplay of parasites, their hosts, and circadian clocks. Frontiers in Cellular and Infection Microbiology 9, 22352988.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelissen, G (2014) Cosinor-based rhythmometry. Theoretical Biology and Medical Modelling 11, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, MJ (2009) The global economic cost of sea lice to the salmonid farming industry. Journal of Fish Diseases 32, 115118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawley, MJ (2007) The R Book. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobson, A, Lafferty, KD, Kuris, AM, Hechinger, RF and Jetz, W (2008) Homage to Linnaeus: how many parasites? How many hosts? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105, 1148211489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellison, AR, Uren Webster, TM, Rey, O, Garcia de Leaniz, C, Consuegra, S, Orozco-terWengel, P and Cable, J (2018) Transcriptomic response to parasite infection in Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) depends on rearing density. BMC Genomics 19, 723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellison, AR, Uren Webster, TM, Rodriguez-Barreto, D, de Leaniz, CG, Consuegra, S, Orozco-terWengel, P and Cable, J (2020) Comparative transcriptomics reveal conserved impacts of rearing density on immune response of two important aquaculture species. Fish & Shellfish Immunology 104, 192201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epsky, ND, Hendrichs, J, Katsoyannos, BI, Vásquez, LA, Ros, JP, Zümreoglu, A, Pereira, R, Bakri, A, Seewooruthun, SI and Heath, RR (1999) Field evaluation of female-targeted trapping systems for Ceratitis capitata (Diptera: Tephritidae) in seven countries. Journal of Economic Entomology 92, 156164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fryer, G (1982) The Parasitic Copepoda and Branchiura of British Freshwater Fishes: A Handbook and Key. Cumbria, UK: Freshwater Biological Association.Google Scholar
Ganni, S, Botden, SMBI, Chmarra, M, Goossens, RHM and Jakimowicz, JJ (2018) A software-based tool for video motion tracking in the surgical skills assessment landscape. Surgical Endoscopy 32, 29942999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heuch, PA, Parsons, A and Boxaspen, K (2011) Diel vertical migration: a possible host-finding mechanism in salmon louse (Lepeophtheirus salmonis) copepodids? Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 52, 681689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karakatsouli, N, Papoutsoglou, SE, Panopoulos, G, Papoutsoglou, ES, Chadio, S and Kalogiannis, D (2008) Effects of light spectrum on growth and stress response of rainbow trout Oncorhynchus mykiss reared under recirculating system conditions. Aquacultural Engineering 38, 3642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kiessling, S, Dubeau-Laramée, G, Ohm, H, Labrecque, N, Olivier, M and Cermakian, N (2017) The circadian clock in immune cells controls the magnitude of Leishmania parasite infection. Scientific Reports 7, 10892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lafferty, KD, Dobson, AP and Kuris, AM (2006) Parasites dominate food web links. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103, 1121111216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leclercq, E, Taylor, J, Sprague, M and Migaud, H (2011) The potential of alternative lighting-systems to suppress pre-harvest sexual maturation of 1+ Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar) post-smolts reared in commercial sea-cages. Aquacultural Engineering 44, 3547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, EE, Campbell, JF and Sukhdeo, MVK (2002) The Behavioural Ecology of Parasites. Oxfordshire, UK: CABI.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcogliese, DJ and Cone, DK (1997) Food webs: a plea for parasites. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 12, 320325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez-Bakker, M and Helm, B (2015) The influence of biological rhythms on host–parasite interactions. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 30, 314326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikheev, VN, Valtonen, ET and Rintamäki-Kinnunen, P (1998). Host searching in Argulus foliaceus L. (Crustacea: Branchiura): the role of vision and selectivity. Parasitology 116(Pt 5), 425430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikheev, VN, Mikheev, AV, Pasternak, AF and Valtonen, ET (1999). Light-mediated host searching strategies in a fish ectoparasite, Argulus foliaceus L. (crustacea: branchiura). Parasitology 120(Pt 4), 409416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mordue (Luntz), AJ and Birkett, MA (2009) A review of host finding behaviour in the parasitic sea louse, Lepeophtheirus salmonis (Caligidae: Copepoda). Journal of Fish Diseases 32, 313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novales Flamarique, I, Browman, HI, Bélanger, M and Boxaspen, K (2000) Ontogenetic changes in visual sensitivity of the parasitic salmon louse Lepeophtheirus salmonis. The Journal of Experimental Biology 203, 16491657.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novales Flamarique, I, Gulbransen, C, Galbraith, M and Stucchi, D (2009) Monitoring and potential control of sea lice using an LED-based light trap. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 66, 13711382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oppedal, F, Taranger, GL, Juell, J-E and Hansen, T (1999) Growth, osmoregulation and sexual maturation of underyearling Atlantic salmon smolt Salmo salar L. exposed to different intensities of continuous light in sea cages. Aquaculture Research 30, 491499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Otsuka, K, Cornelissen, G and Halberg, F (2016) Chronomics and Continuous Ambulatory Blood Pressure Monitoring: Vascular Chronomics: From 7-Day/24-Hour to Lifelong Monitoring. Tokyo, Japan: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, R, Parsons, R, Garner, N, Oster, H and Rawashdeh, O (2020) CircaCompare: a method to estimate and statistically support differences in MESOR, amplitude and phase, between circadian rhythms. Bioinformatics (Oxford, England) 36, 12081212.Google ScholarPubMed
Poulin, R and Morand, S (2000) The diversity of parasites. The Quarterly Review of Biology 75, 277293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poulin, R, Curtis, MA and Rau, ME (1990) Responses of the fish ectoparasite Salmincola edwardsii (Copepoda) to stimulation, and their implication for host-finding. Parasitology 100, 417421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Core Team (2017) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rea, JG and Irwin, SW (1994) The ecology of host-finding behaviour and parasite transmission: past and future perspectives. Parasitology 109, S31S39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reece, SE, Prior, KF and Mideo, N (2017) The life and times of parasites: rhythms in strategies for within-host survival and between-host transmission. Journal of Biological Rhythms 32, 516533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, CA, Rasband, WS and Eliceiri, KW (2012) NIH Image to ImageJ: 25 years of image analysis. Nature Methods 9, 671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Searle, SR, Speed, FM and Milliken, GA (1980) Population marginal means in the linear model: an alternative to least squares means. The American Statistician 34, 216221.Google Scholar
Sikkel, PC, Ziemba, RE, Sears, WT and Wheeler, JC (2009) Diel ontogenetic shift in parasitic activity in a gnathiid isopod on Caribbean coral reefs. Coral Reefs 28, 489495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skelton, J, Creed, RP and Brown, BL (2015) A symbiont's dispersal strategy: condition-dependent dispersal underlies predictable variation in direct transmission among hosts. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 282, 20152081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stewart, HL, Nomura, M, Piercey, GE, Dunham, A and Lelliott, TL (2013) Ecological effects of blue LED lights used in aquaculture. Canadian Technical Report of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 3057, iv, +26p.Google Scholar
Sukhdeo, MVK and Chappell, LH (1994) Parasites and Behaviour. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, NGH, Sommerville, C and Wootten, R (2005 a) A Review of Argulus spp. Occurring in UK freshwaters, Science Report SC990019/SR1. UK: Environment Agency.Google Scholar
Taylor, NGH, Sommerville, C and Wootten, R (2005 b) Argulus spp. Infections in UK Stillwater Trout Fisheries, Science Report SC990019/SR2. UK: Environment Agency.Google Scholar
Thaben, PF and Westermark, PO (2014) Detecting rhythms in time series with RAIN. Journal of Biological Rhythms 29, 391400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trippel, EA (2010) Pathway of effects of artificial light on non-target organisms at aquaculture sites in Canada. DFO Canadian Science Advisory Secretariat Research Document 2010/023, vi,+14 p.Google Scholar
Van Leerdam, MB, Smith, JW and Fuchs, TW (1985) Frass-mediated, host-finding behavior of Cotesia flavipes, a Braconid Parasite of Diatraea saccharalis (Lepidoptera: Pyralidae). Annals of the Entomological Society of America 78, 647650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villamizar, N, Blanco-Vives, B, Migaud, H, Davie, A, Carboni, S and Sánchez-Vázquez, FJ (2011) Effects of light during early larval development of some aquacultured teleosts: a review. Aquaculture 315, 8694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitaterna, MH, Takahashi, JS and Turek, FW (2001) Overview of circadian rhythms. Alcohol Research and Health 25, 8593.Google ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H (2009) ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, USA: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, G, Anafi, RC, Hughes, ME, Kornacker, K and Hogenesch, JB (2016) MetaCycle: an integrated R package to evaluate periodicity in large scale data. Bioinformatics (Oxford, England) 32, 33513353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshizawa, K and Nogami, S (2008) The first report of phototaxis of fish ectoparasite, Argulus japonicus. Research in Veterinary Science 85, 128130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Plan view of experimental areas for Argulus foliaceus (A/B) light vs fish host preference and (C) light colour preference trials. In each arena, circles represent light-emitting diode (LED) light sources. (A) gives a choice of white light vs a 3-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) host with a turned-off light, (B) gives a choice of a white light + stickleback vs a turned off light + stickleback. In (A/B) dashed lines represent 1 cm aperture mesh which allows the parasites to swim through while blocking fish movement. In (C) dotted lines indicate the total area of each coloured corner for behavioural recording, R = red light, G = green, W = white and B = blue (coloured light placement was changed/randomized for each trial).

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Average swimming speed of Argulus foliaceus off host over a 48 h period under 2 different light conditions: alternating light and dark (A and C) and total darkness (B and D). (A) Male A. foliaceus under 12 h light:12 h dark. (B) Male A. foliaceus under total darkness. (C) Female A. foliaceus under 12 h light:12 h dark. (D) Female A. foliaceus under total darkness. White backgrounds indicate periods of light, dark grey backgrounds indicate periods of darkness. Zeitgeber time (ZT)0 = 7 am, ZT12 = 7 pm.

Figure 2

Fig. 3. Circacompare output plot of male and female Argulus foliaceus swimming speed over a 12:12 light: dark 48 h period. Lights turn on/off at 0/12 and 24/36.

Figure 3

Fig. 4. Light preference of male Argulus foliaceus (n = 20) off the host. Average time spent by free-swimming A. foliaceus in the vicinity of different-coloured lights over a 2-min period. Wavelengths of white light = 450–700 nm, blue light = 450–490 nm, green light = 520–560 nm, red light = 635–700 nm.

Supplementary material: Image

Hunt et al. supplementary material

Hunt et al. supplementary material 1

Download Hunt et al. supplementary material(Image)
Image 530.8 KB
Supplementary material: Image

Hunt et al. supplementary material

Hunt et al. supplementary material 2

Download Hunt et al. supplementary material(Image)
Image 246 KB