Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T18:16:50.152Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of nutrition on cognition and brain health in ageing: a targeted approach

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 December 2015

Jim M. Monti*
Affiliation:
Abbott Nutrition, Columbus, OH, USA
Christopher J. Moulton
Affiliation:
Abbott Nutrition, Columbus, OH, USA Center for Nutrition, Learning, and Memory, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA
Neal J. Cohen
Affiliation:
Center for Nutrition, Learning, and Memory, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA Neuroscience Program, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA Beckman Institute, Urbana, IL, USA Interdisciplinary Health Sciences Initiative, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Urbana, IL, USA
*
*Corresponding author: Jim M. Monti, fax +1 614 727 5270, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Animal experiments and cross-sectional or prospective longitudinal research in human subjects suggest a role for nutrition in cognitive ageing. However, data from randomised controlled trials (RCT) that seek causal evidence for the impact of nutrients on cognitive ageing in humans often produce null results. Given that RCT test hypotheses in a rigorous fashion, one conclusion could be that the positive effects of nutrition on the aged brain observed in other study designs are spurious. On the other hand, it may be that the design of many clinical trials conducted thus far has been less than optimal. In the present review, we offer a blueprint for a more targeted approach to the design of RCT in nutrition, cognition and brain health in ageing that focuses on three key areas. First, the role of nutrition is more suited for the maintenance of health rather than the treatment of disease. Second, given that cognitive functions and brain regions vary in their susceptibility to ageing, those that especially deteriorate in senescence should be focal points in evaluating the efficacy of an intervention. Third, the outcome measures that assess change due to nutrition, especially in the cognitive domain, should not necessarily be the same neuropsychological tests used to assess gross brain damage or major pathological conditions. By addressing these three areas, we expect that clinical trials of nutrition, cognition and brain health in ageing will align more closely with other research in this field, and aid in revealing the true nature of nutrition’s impact on the aged brain.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2015 

Introduction

A plethora of data across species indicates that nutrition and dietary patterns modulate brain health during ageing. For example, recent reviews and meta-analyses indicate that adherence to the Mediterranean diet, consisting of plant-based foods, olive oil and seafood, reduces the risk of developing Alzheimer’s disease (AD)(Reference Lourida, Soni and Thompson-Coon1Reference Van de Rest, Berendsen and Haveman-Nies3). Similar neuroprotective effects against cognitive decline are apparent in studies focusing on dietary intake of vitamin E, vitamin B12 and folate, and fish oils rich in n-3 fatty acids(Reference Morris4, Reference Gomez-Pinilla5). Conversely, high intake of n-6 fatty acids coupled with low intake of n-3 fatty acids and/or high saturated fat consumption may be associated with pathological ageing(Reference Gomez-Pinilla5, Reference Morris and Tangney6).

Research in animal models of ageing and AD has probed the mechanisms that may underlie these effects in humans, and results indicate that a seemingly diverse group of nutrients or foods have beneficial effects on the ageing brain in rodent models. For instance, vitamin C has been demonstrated to improve memory in rodents, while the anti-inflammatory compound curcumin, found in the spice turmeric, has been shown to increase neurogenesis (the birth of new neurons) in the rodent hippocampus(Reference Kennard and Harrison7, Reference Kim, Son and Park8). Further, a common method in ageing research is to induce or observe naturally the development of pathological formations characteristic of AD, and then investigate how various interventions may mitigate the accumulation of this pathology. The two principal pathologies studied are extra-cellular amyloid-β plaques and intracellular neurofibrillary tangles (i.e. ‘Alzheimer-like pathology’). Interestingly, a variety of compounds including curcumin, DHA, blueberries and other polyphenols have been documented to ameliorate Alzheimer-like pathology in rodent models(Reference Frautschy, Hu and Kim9Reference Wang, Ho and Zhao12). Additionally, canines fed a diverse diet rich in vitamins C and E, n-3 fatty acids, l-carnitine, and polyphenols display similar protection from amyloid-β pathology and improved cognition in old age(Reference Fahnestock, Marchese and Head13Reference Pop, Head and Hill15). One possible mechanism accounting for these effects centres on the reduction of oxidative stress via these nutrients and foods, which in turn inhibits Alzheimer-like pathology.

Despite these promising results, randomised controlled trials (RCT) in older humans searching for causal links between nutrients and cognitive function often produce null results(Reference Aisen, Schneider and Sano16Reference Kang, Cook and Manson25). Large RCT provide the strongest evidence for determining if a particular nutrient has a direct effect on human cognition or brain health, since random assignment to subject groups minimises any confounding effects that may be present in prospective longitudinal or epidemiological studies. Therefore, given these conflicting results across experimental methodologies, it is conceivable that the effects observed in prospective longitudinal or epidemiological studies on nutrition and cognitive ageing, no matter how well-controlled for extraneous effects, reflect confounding factors other than the nutritional variables of interest. An alternative possibility is that previous RCT in this field may have been suboptimally designed to detect the effects of nutrition on the ageing brain. We favour this latter view, and believe there is an opportunity to leverage the results from the important foundational work in this area to inform future studies. The inability to detect positive results in the complex interaction of nutrition and the ageing brain may be due to innumerable factors. For instance, baseline nutritional status may need to be accounted for more thoroughly, as it may be the case that supplementing a nutrient when the majority of the population has optimal status of that nutrient may not be efficacious. Additionally it may be the case that the interaction of certain nutrients, especially from food intake rather than by supplementation may confer the benefits(Reference Morris4). In this review, however, we specifically suggest that the field re-examine its design and implementation of RCT with learnings from the cognitive neuroscience of ageing in order to more fully realise the type of causal evidence sought from RCT on nutrition in cognitive ageing. In order to deliver more successful nutritional RCT in this area, we suggest that careful attention be paid to: (a) the selection of a population of older adults most likely to benefit from a nutritional intervention, with an emphasis that nutrition plays a key role in the maintenance of health rather than treatment of pathological disease; (b) the domains of cognitive neuroscience investigated, with priority given to areas differentially affected by senescence; and (c) the tools chosen to assess these domains, which need not be the same assessment tools used to evaluate diseased populations.

Population selection

There exists a vast amount of inter-individual variability in cognition among older adults, as a typical sampling of 65-year-old individuals would reveal. For instance, within cognitively healthy individuals, some individuals may still be working productively in their fields and only experiencing infrequent so-called ‘senior moments’, while others may be retired and could be more forgetful but would be deemed to have normal cognition for his/her age. Yet, there are others who deal with more significant cognitive difficulties. Some individuals may have declines in cognition greater than expected for their age, but have not progressed to dementia, a condition identified as mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Finally, others may already be in the throes of progressive dementia such as AD. This diverse range of cognitive function in older adults presents a quandary in choosing the population(s) in which cognitive and brain health are most likely to benefit from a nutritional intervention. Populations selected for clinical trials on nutrition and cognitive ageing typically include participants from one to three samples: AD patients, MCI patients, or healthy older adults (>60 years old) with no objective cognitive impairment for their age. Here, we wish to emphasise previous contributions(Reference Benton26, Reference De Jager and Kovatcheva27) highlighting that optimal nutrition can maintain and augment cognition in healthy older adults, with an end-goal of reducing the likelihood of developing MCI or AD and maximising cognition throughout adult life, rather than modifying cognitive diseases of ageing via nutrition.

Alzheimer’s disease

A substantial proportion of large-scale, double-blind RCT have focused on slowing cognitive decline in those afflicted with AD or another dementia, with the results being largely disappointing (for a review, see Otaegui-Arrazola et al. (Reference Otaegui-Arrazola, Amiano and Elbusto28)). For instance, trials investigating the efficacy of B vitamins or n-3 fatty acids in mild-to-moderate AD patients found no beneficial effect of these compounds on slowing disease progression, despite ample evidence from non-RCT studies suggesting a role for these nutrients in cognition late in life(Reference Aisen, Schneider and Sano16, Reference Kwok, Lee and Law19, Reference Quinn, Raman and Thomas22, Reference Sun, Lu and Chien23, Reference Freund-Levi, Eriksdotter-Jönhagen and Cederholm29). One possible reason for the preponderance of null results in clinical trials with AD patients is that it may be too late to intervene nutritionally when AD becomes clinically apparent. It is widely appreciated that before even the earliest symptoms of AD becoming manifest, pathological changes are occurring in the brain which give rise to ‘preclinical AD’ and ‘MCI’. Thus, the disease process of AD certainly begins years, if not decades, before the emergence of the clinical syndrome of AD(Reference Sperling, Aisen and Beckett30). During the preclinical AD and MCI phases, pathology in the form of amyloid-β and hyperphosphorylated tau proteins accumulates in the brain, followed by synaptic dysfunction and neuronal injury and loss(Reference Sperling, Aisen and Beckett30, Reference Jack, Knopman and Jagust31). These processes must occur in sufficient magnitude for the development of any clinically relevant cognitive abnormalities. By the time notable cognitive change that is indicative of the mildest stage of AD begins to occur, considerable deleterious changes in the brain have taken place. In two of the studies above, the mean Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score of the participants was approximately 21 out of 30, such that the average participant in these trials was just outside of the moderate stage of AD; by the time the disease has reached this stage, substantial brain damage has occurred. (The MMSE is a global cognitive screening tool for dementia on a scale of 0–30. Generally, scores of 21–25 are indicative of mild AD, 11–20 moderate AD, and 0–10 severe AD(Reference Perneczky, Wagenpfeil and Komossa32). However, individuals with scores above 25 can still be diagnosed with AD, especially in the earliest stages of the disorder.)

Given the years- to decades-long head start of AD, implementing a nutritional intervention at the mild-to-moderate stages of AD in order to affect cognition seems unlikely to succeed. Furthermore, when considering the notion that pharmaceutical approaches such as acetylcholinesterase inhibitors offer only palliative effects(Reference Aisen, Cummings and Schneider33), it is perhaps unsurprising that single-nutrient interventions tend to fail in this population. Therefore, in order to offer a role for nutrition to modify cognition in older adults, researchers must focus on areas of the ageing spectrum where brain function is less impaired. Supporting this notion, Freund-Levi et al. (Reference Freund-Levi, Eriksdotter-Jönhagen and Cederholm29) evaluated a range of AD patients with MMSE scores from 15 to 30. Across the full spectrum of patients, they found no effect of n-3 fatty acids on the rate of cognitive decline over 12 months. However, when selectively looking at the most mildly affected patients (MMSE≥28), n-3 fatty acid supplementation attenuated cognitive decline in this small sample(Reference Freund-Levi, Eriksdotter-Jönhagen and Cederholm29). The fact that Freund-Levi et al. (Reference Freund-Levi, Eriksdotter-Jönhagen and Cederholm29) found a positive effect of n-3 fatty acids in patients with MMSE scores between 28 and 30, which more likely reflects a cognitive state akin to MCI rather than AD, underscores the notion that initiating a nutritional intervention before the onset of dementia is more likely to influence cognition in late life. Nonetheless, two other studies that supplemented n-3 fatty acids to participants in the same MMSE range did not see beneficial cognitive effects(Reference Dangour, Allen and Elbourne17, Reference Van de Rest, Geleijnse and Kok24), furthering the complexity of this area.

Mild cognitive impairment

Individuals with a diagnosis of MCI display heterogeneous cognitive impairments that are exacerbated for their age. However, these decrements in cognition have not progressed to the level observed in AD or other dementias. Importantly though, the clinical development of MCI symptoms is preceded by a protracted period of brain damage(Reference Sperling, Aisen and Beckett30) occurring in the medial temporal lobe (including the hippocampus) as well as portions of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and parietal lobe. Therefore, these underlying pathological changes that have developed may make successful nutritional intervention at this stage difficult.

There have been some studies indicative of a cognitive benefit via nutritional supplementation in MCI populations. For instance, cognitive impairment was attenuated in an MCI population with elevated homocysteine levels when receiving B vitamin supplementation (vitamins B6, B12, folic acid) relative to the control group(Reference De Jager, Oulhaj and Jacoby34). The focus on homocysteine reduction directly addresses previous findings linking high levels of this compound to dementia and white matter damage in the elderly(Reference Dufouil, Alpérovitch and Ducros35, Reference Seshadri, Beiser and Selhub36). Further, a few smaller-scale studies have observed an improvement in cognition when providing n-3 fatty acid supplementation to those with MCI(Reference Chiu, Su and Cheng37Reference Lee, Shahar and Chin39), and it will be important for larger trials to replicate these beneficial effects.

Despite these successes, not all vitamin trials in MCI patients report positive results, as van Uffelen et al. (Reference Van Uffelen, Chinapaw and van Mechelen40) did not find an improvement following vitamin B supplementation. One possibility explaining these opposing findings concerns dosage, as vitamin B12 and folic acid/folate in too high of a dose may actually worsen cognitive outcomes(Reference Eussen, de Groot and Joosten41Reference Morris, Evans and Schneider43). Similarly administration (in supplement form) of the antioxidant vitamin E in a double-blind RCT in MCI patients did not reduce the rate at which these participants converted to AD(Reference Petersen, Thomas and Grundman21). These results seem at odds with prospective cohort investigations indicating a protective role of dietary vitamin E intake and cognitive ageing(Reference Engelhart, Geerlings and Ruitenberg44, Reference Morris, Evans and Bienias45).

One explanation for these mixed results may be revealed by an investigation of the changes occurring in the brain that give rise to MCI. Similar to AD but on a lesser scale, in order for the clinical symptoms of MCI to become apparent (at least when due to an AD-like aetiology), substantial brain pathology, along with synapse and neuron dysfunction or loss, must accumulate in a ‘preclinical phase’, which typically occurs on a timescale of years(Reference Sperling, Aisen and Beckett30). Thus, any nutritional intervention initiated when MCI is clinically apparent faces a disease process with a years-long head start, thereby decreasing the likelihood of success. It is clear from some of the above studies that this head start is not insurmountable (perhaps unlike AD), but including these patients in RCT investigating nutrition and cognitive ageing may mask the beneficial effects of certain nutrients (for example, vitamin E) on brain health, as the nutrient cannot fully or successfully act on the brain in this diminished state. Moreover, while any intervention that can prevent patients from converting to dementia from MCI is highly valuable, when considering quality-of-life issues and overall public health, it would be even more valuable to find approaches that reduce the risk of a healthy older adult from developing MCI. Though not as impaired as an AD patient, the patient with MCI may lose his/her ability to work or drive, and he/she also faces an increased risk of developing AD. Therefore, identifying roles for nutrition in cognitive ageing that can lower the risk of developing MCI or AD, rather than attempting to use nutrition to treat these disorders outright, is of paramount importance. By focusing efforts on healthy older adults (and even in middle-aged populations), it may be possible to further achieve this goal.

Healthy older adults

Healthy older adults without a diagnosis of AD or MCI do not have an objective cognitive impairment for their age, as measured by current neuropsychological tests. Importantly, this does not indicate that they are operating at the same cognitive level as someone 40 years their junior. Ageing research demonstrates that healthy older adults exhibit decline in several areas of cognition relative to younger adults, including episodic and working memory, as well as processing speed. Interestingly, some cognitive aspects, such as semantic knowledge, stay the same or even improve with age(Reference Park, Lautenschlager and Hedden46). Furthermore, advancing age is a major risk factor for the development of MCI or AD, so a healthy older adult at age 70 years may still develop MCI or AD by the age of 75 years. Therefore, despite normal cognition for his/her age, any given healthy older adult still has: (a) room for improvement in certain cognitive domains; and (b) an increasing risk of objective cognitive impairment stemming from MCI, AD or another dementia as he/she increases in age. By intervening to address these two related points, nutrition has great potential to maintain or improve healthy cognitive ageing; however, the research to date paints a murky picture as to whether nutrition has a role in cognition and brain health for healthy older adults.

The tension between prospective longitudinal and epidemiological studies on the one hand, and RCT on the other, is perhaps greatest when considering the available data from healthy older adults. Though not without controversy, numerous prospective longitudinal and epidemiological studies indicate that dietary intake of n-3 fatty acids, various vitamins, flavonoids and/or adherence to a Mediterranean-type diet reduces AD risk and/or improves cognition (for a review, see Otaegui-Arrazola et al. (Reference Otaegui-Arrazola, Amiano and Elbusto28)). Despite this, the preponderance of data from nutritional RCT in healthy older adults tilts towards these nutrients not having a modulatory effect in cognitive ageing.

With regard to RCT indicating positive effects, an enhancement for memory was observed in healthy older adult males undergoing long-term β-carotene supplementation(Reference Grodstein, Kang and Glynn47). Another report assessed the effect of 24 weeks of DHA administration in older adults who were free of dementia or MCI, but had subjective complaints about their memory. In this population, there was an improvement in memory for the DHA group compared with those receiving the placebo(Reference Yurko-Mauro, McCarthy and Rom48). Also, a study investigating 3-year folic acid supplementation in participants aged 50–70 years with elevated (>13 µmol/l) homocysteine levels found that the folic acid group had better scores on memory and information processing speed tasks relative to the placebo group(Reference Durga, van Boxtel and Schouten49). This study had some unique characteristics that may shed light on the proper population and length of time necessary for a nutrition intervention. The study by Durga et al. (Reference Durga, van Boxtel and Schouten49) included younger participants than many studies on ageing and nutrition, as in most studies the youngest included age is 65 years, with the mean age being much higher. The inclusion of a sample of individuals in their fifties may provide a population in which the negative brain changes accruing from ageing are in a more benign state and thus more responsive to nutritional intervention, thereby allowing for a greater preservation of cognitive function that is in turn more readily detectable on cognitive tests. The supplementation period was also 3 years, which is also longer than most studies. Yet the larger examination of the literature paints a more complex picture. A recent meta-analysis of homocysteine-lowering B vitamin supplementation and cognition in the elderly did not find support for the role of B vitamins on cognition, even when looking at younger subpopulations and including studies of long duration (up to 8 years(Reference Clarke, Bennett and Parish50)). It should be noted that this meta-analysis did include studies investigating cerebrovascular patients, and the primary variable in many trials was not cognition but rather lowering of homocysteine. Nonetheless, these null results are seemingly reinforced by the lack of an effect found by van der Zwaluw et al. (Reference van der Zwaluw, Dhonukshe-Rutten and Van Wijngaarden51), who studied the effect of B vitamin treatment and cognition in older adults with elevated homocysteine. As described below, one explanation for these inconsistent findings centres on the neurocognitive domains studied and the tools used to assess these domains.

A final note that warrants discussion in the study of nutrition and cognition in healthy adults concerns the length of time necessary for intervention studies. Ideally, studies should be long enough to capture natural deterioration in cognitive abilities over time and/or of sufficient duration for nutrition to have a biological effect on the brain to improve cognitive performance. When studying healthy adults, especially if the study population includes individuals closer to middle-aged, multi-year longitudinal studies would be the ideal norm. This must be balanced with the pragmatic realities that work against multi-year studies such as cost, dropout rate, etc. Below we advocate for the use of assessment tools more appropriate for a healthy population in including more challenging cognitive tests that may potentially reveal subtle cognitive change over time. Therefore, this may shorten the length of interventions, though studies that seek to understand the role of nutrition on the trajectory of cognitive decline in healthy adults still need to be of sufficient length for that decline to occur.

Conclusion

There are certainly roles for nutrition with respect to quality of life for all older adults, including those in the end stages of AD. However, nutrition will have the greatest likelihood of influencing cognition when it operates on a brain not yet affected by disease states such as MCI, AD or another type of dementia. In the cases of these diseases, the extant brain changes will often dwarf any modifying role from a newly established nutritional intervention. Rather, the most promising role of nutrition in cognitive ageing is maintenance of healthy brain function and reducing the risk of these diseases from occurring; consequently, the population in which we should seek to establish efficacy of nutritional intervention in cognitive ageing should be healthy adults.

Specific cognitive domains and brain regions

As noted above, many large-scale RCT in healthy older adults have not fared better than those in AD or MCI. RCT studying the effects of B vitamins, vitamin E or DHA in healthy older adults have produced null results with respect to improvements in cognition(Reference Dangour, Allen and Elbourne17, Reference Ford, Flicker and Alfonso18, Reference McMahon, Green and Skeaff20, Reference Van de Rest, Geleijnse and Kok24, Reference Kang, Cook and Manson25, Reference Andreeva, Kesse-Guyot and Barberger-Gateau52). There is a multitude of reasons why these and other studies have not found any modulation of cognition due to nutritional intervention, such as nutritional factors relating to absorption, or a lack of consideration for the heterogeneous status of participants’ baseline nutrient levels. We will focus on an alternative (but not mutually exclusive) explanation of these null results that concerns the cognitive domains investigated and the possible inadequacy of the tools used to measure them.

A large proportion of the published nutritional interventions in ageing has tested broad cognitive domains rather than taking a targeted approach that focuses on the areas of cognition most affected by ageing; further, insensitive tools have often been used as a means of measuring these broad domains. Particularly problematic are cases where blunt tools that assess global cognition, and are intended for dementia screening (for example, MMSE), are used as outcome measures in healthy older adults (for a review, see Macready et al. (Reference Macready, Butler and Kennedy53)). Healthy older adults are not likely to have any meaningful variance on the MMSE because it is a screening tool intended to detect dementia. Even though the MMSE is out of 30 points, a score indicative of mild dementia is 25 or below, with scores of 26–29 indicating questionable dementia(Reference Perneczky, Wagenpfeil and Komossa32); therefore, the range of scores for healthy older adults on the MMSE is restricted, and many will be near ceiling, making the detection of cognitive improvements from nutrition extremely difficult. In addition to the insensitivity issues that occur with global measures, a global cognition approach is misaligned with what is known both about the brain regions and cognitive domains that decline most in ageing, which are probably the brain areas to benefit from nutritional approaches.

Just as all older adults do not decline cognitively at equal rates, brain regions and associated cognitive functions also vary with their susceptibility to ageing (for a review, see Jagust(Reference Jagust54)). For instance, primary sensory areas such as the visual cortex show little volume reduction with advanced age, and they are even relatively spared from the pathology of severe AD(Reference Raz, Ghisletta and Rodrigue55, Reference Yankner, Lu and Loerch56). Similarly, even though most individuals associate ageing with cognitive decline, certain areas of cognition, such as semantic memory or vocabulary knowledge, remain stable or increase across the lifespan(Reference Park, Lautenschlager and Hedden46, Reference Nyberg, Maitland and Rönnlund57). Given that RCT in cognitive ageing use improvement or maintenance of a brain region or cognitive construct as a primary outcome measure, the regions/constructs most susceptible to ageing should be selected for investigation; regions/constructs less affected by the ageing process are not as likely to show change over time as a result of interventions, and should therefore be de-emphasised in RCT of cognitive ageing and nutrition.

Relational memory and the hippocampus

One brain structure particularly sensitive to ageing is the hippocampus, a structure located near the middle of the brain, within the medial temporal lobes, that primarily supports aspects of memory function (see below). MRI studies measuring the volume of the hippocampus indicate that it begins to shrink at an accelerated rate during the sixth decade of life(Reference Fjell, Westlye and Grydeland58); this volumetric decline probably reflects, at least in part, loss and alternation of synapses and a reduction in proliferation of neurons, but little overt neuronal death in individuals free of neurodegenerative disorders(Reference Morrison and Baxter59). Additionally, nearly all adults over the age of 65 years, even those without a diagnosis of MCI or AD, will have some degree of AD-like pathology in the form of neurofibrillary tangles within the medial temporal lobe, and these tangles are negatively associated with memory performance(Reference Bennett, Wilson and Boyle60). When occurring in adults without MCI or AD, this pathology is almost always limited to the transentorhinal and entorhinal cortices, which are located adjacent to, and are densely connected with, the hippocampus; this pathology can also be found in portions of the hippocampus proper in healthy older adults(Reference Braak, Thal and Ghebremedhin61).

While the hippocampus contributes to a variety of cognitive abilities, its main function may be to serve as the critical hub for a network underlying relational (associative) memory formation and retrieval. Through its cellular architecture that displays disproportionate and rapid plasticity, and anatomical location, which serves as a convergence zone for cortical afferents, the hippocampus is capable of binding together various pieces of information into lasting representations (for reviews, see Eichenbaum & Cohen(Reference Eichenbaum and Cohen62), Cohen & Eichenbaum(Reference Cohen and Eichenbaum63) and Konkel & Cohen(Reference Konkel and Cohen64)). This system allows one to later remember what the person who introduced himself as ‘Roger Atwood’ looks like, and that you met him when staying at a hotel the first time you visited Santa Fe. Only the hippocampal memory system can learn such novel relations with just one exposure, making it vital to forming new memories of experience throughout a lifetime. Consequently, tests that maximally engage this function, using novel stimuli and novel relations rather than familiar items, are likely to best tap into hippocampal processing.

Given that the hippocampus deteriorates with age, it is unsurprising that relational memory function declines disproportionately as well(Reference Naveh-Benjamin65). Relational memory, because it depends upon a network of brain structures interacting with the hippocampus, may be particularly susceptible to ageing and hence particularly useful in the study of nutrition and cognitive ageing. Further, the hippocampus is aided by areas of the PFC and parietal lobe during encoding and retrieval, which serve to modulate functions such as goal-directed behaviour and attention (for reviews, see Cabeza et al. (Reference Cabeza, Ciaramelli and Olson66), Simons & Spiers(Reference Simons and Spiers67) and Wagner et al. (Reference Wagner, Shannon and Kahn68)), and that these structures, too, are sensitive to age-related effects (see below).

Critically, the literature provides reason to believe that these deleterious effects of hippocampal ageing may be partially ameliorated by nutrition. For instance, animal models indicate that intake of flavonoids increases hippocampal neurogenesis, and it is associated with better spatial memory in rodents(Reference Green, Martinez-Coria and Khashwji10, Reference Casadesus, Shukitt-Hale and Stellwagen69, Reference Van Praag, Lucero and Yeo70). Moreover, DHA administration in rodents also has been linked to higher levels of neurogenesis and enhanced hippocampal memory(Reference He, Qu and Cui71, Reference Petursdottir, Farr and Morley72). In addition to these nutrition-induced changes in gross brain structure that allow for better spatial memory, both DHA and various flavonoids reduce the amount of tau protein deposition in the brain(Reference Frautschy, Hu and Kim9, Reference Wang, Santa-Maria and Ho73). Pertinent to hippocampal health in ageing, the neurofibrillary tangles that form in the medial temporal lobe with ageing are composed of hyperphosphorylated tau protein. Though amyloid-β has a more widespread distribution in the brain, its accumulation is also known to impair the type of memory processes that rely on the hippocampus. Importantly, evidence exists for potential modulation of amyloid-β by diet. In murine models of ageing, DHA and curcumin supplementation reduce the amount of amyloid-β burden, with curcumin also preventing amyloid-associated memory impairment(Reference Kim, Son and Park8, Reference Green, Martinez-Coria and Khashwji10, Reference Lim, Calon and Morihara74). In a canine model of ageing, pleiotropic diets composed of multiple antioxidants can ameliorate amyloid deposition and improve retention of information(Reference Milgram, Zicker and Head14, Reference Pop, Head and Hill15). Finally in humans with MCI, more hippocampal tissue was spared over a 2-year period in a group receiving vitamins B6, B12 and folic acid supplementation.

Working memory, executive function and the frontoparietal network

In addition to relational memory, several other cognitive constructs may prove amenable to nutritional intervention due to their reliance on brain networks that are disproportionately affected by ageing processes. One good candidate is working memory, which involves the ability to temporarily maintain information in one’s head, especially in the face of interference, and/or to manipulate the temporarily maintained information to achieve a goal. An example of working memory entails recalling the spatial layout and furniture placement of your living room, and then mentally rearranging the furniture to produce a new design. Working memory processes are thought to rely heavily on frontal and parietal regions of the brain(Reference Gazzaley and Nobre75, Reference Kane and Engle76), and older adults frequently experience reductions in working memory capabilities(Reference Park, Lautenschlager and Hedden46). A closely related cognitive construct to working memory is executive function, which is even sometimes thought of as a parent category to working memory. Executive function processes entail abilities such as planning, inhibition and fluid reasoning; these abilities largely rely on a fronto-parietal network(Reference Barbey, Colom and Solomon77), and it is common to observe age-related deficits in executive function(Reference Buckner78).

Biological mechanisms accounting for these deficits in working memory and executive function centre on age-related changes that occur with the PFC and parietal lobe. Normal ageing is associated with a reduction in synapses in the PFC(Reference Morrison and Baxter59), and it is additionally known that both the PFC and posterior parietal lobe undergo volume loss with ageing(Reference Raz, Ghisletta and Rodrigue55). Similar to the hippocampus, intake of B vitamins has been associated with more grey matter volume(Reference Erickson, Suever and Prakash79), suggesting the possibility of B vitamins to spare cortical grey matter volume, which may in turn have an effect on working memory. Additionally, amyloid-β accumulates in a substantial proportion of older adults presumed to be free of MCI or AD, with particular concentration in the PFC and in areas of the medial parietal lobe, such as the precuneus(Reference Rodrigue, Kennedy and Devous80, Reference Sperling, Laviolette and O’Keefe81). The level of amyloid-β in these areas is inversely related to multiple cognitive abilities, including various forms of memory, attention and processing speed(Reference Kennedy, Rodrigue and Devous82, Reference Rentz, Amariglio and Becker83). One intriguing study that manipulated dietary macronutrient proportions over a 4-week period indicated that a diet low in saturated fat and a low glycaemic index led to reduced levels of amyloid-β 42 in the cerebrospinal fluid (a biomarker for amyloid-β in the brain) of healthy older adults(Reference Bayer-Carter, Green and Montine84). Future studies are needed to further discern how macronutrient profiles may mediate amyloid-β clearance in humans.

Processing speed and white matter

A major area of decline in ageing is speed of processing, which refers to how quickly one can process information of multiple modalities(Reference Park, Lautenschlager and Hedden46, Reference Salthouse85). In fact, some have argued that speed of processing may underlie many of the cognitive deficits observed in ageing(Reference Salthouse86, Reference Salthouse87). In the brain, processing speed ability is largely dependent on whole-brain white matter integrity(Reference Borghesani, Madhyastha and Aylward88, Reference Wen, Zhu and He89). Normal ageing is also associated with deterioration of the white matter. These changes in the white matter are more pronounced in the PFC(Reference Wen and Sachdev90). The aetiology of this pathology in the white matter among healthy older adults is most often due to subtle cerebrovascular-related changes, such as arterial stiffening or microscopic bleeding within the brain(Reference Jagust54). Moreover, white matter damage is mitigated by n-3 fatty acid intake and adherence to a Mediterranean-type diet(Reference Bowman, Silbert and Howieson91, Reference Scarmeas, Luchsinger and Stern92), potentially due to the salutary cardiovascular effects of this type of diet and the relationship between white matter integrity and cerebrovascular integrity.

Conclusion

To summarise, the cognitive neuroscience of ageing indicates that the hippocampus, fronto-parietal network and white matter disproportionately decline in ageing, and evidence from both animal and human studies suggests a positive role for nutrition on these brain regions, either through the preservation of tissue or reduction of Alzheimer-like pathology. Therefore, the aforementioned cognitive domains may be good targets for nutritional RCT in cognitive ageing, and tests that provide good indices of these networks’ structure and function should be emphasised. However, despite the strong reasons for being optimistic, there remain many RCT that have tested for nutritional effects on memory or processing speed with null results. In the remainder of this review, we discuss the need to go beyond identification of appropriate cognitive domains and to consider the design and construction of sufficiently sensitive tools for appropriately assessing those domains in the study of nutritional modulation of cognitive ageing, and discuss how trials can further be informed by sensitive brain imaging techniques.

Assessment tools

Cognitive measurements

Through the excellent work of those in the field of neuropsychology, there exists an array of behavioural assessment tools available to measure aspects of cognitive function. Most often, these tests are deployed when evaluating clinical populations, such as those who have sustained a traumatic brain injury or those suspected of having AD. As an example, when measuring hippocampal-dependent memory, the most commonly used tasks involve asking a participant to memorise a list of common words, and then assessing his/her retention for the material after a delay of approximately 20–30 min(Reference Lezak, Howieson and Loring93). In the case of a patient with damage relatively circumscribed to the hippocampus, this type of test (for example, California Verbal Learning Test) works well in detecting the gross memory impairment that follows this injury. Similarly, the current hallmark indication of early clinical AD is a primary deficit on this type of task with relative preservation in other cognitive domains(Reference Weintraub, Wicklund and Salmon94).

Though these tests have worked well in the characterisation of large impairments due to dementia or brain injury, they may not fare as well in detecting the variance in hippocampal-dependent memory performance among those within the so-called ‘normal range’ of cognitive ability. The idea that the classic neuropsychological tests may be insensitive to subtle changes or early pathology is not new, as demonstrated by the classification of individuals with subjective memory impairment. These individuals do not have an objective memory impairment as measured by a delayed-recall test, but they subjectively report, and are often concerned about, their perceived failing memory ability. Interestingly, those who are concerned about the memory lapses they are experiencing are more likely to develop AD(Reference Jessen, Wiese and Bachmann95). Thus, it is conceivable that these individuals may have preclinical AD, and that their memory deficit is too subtle to be detected by standard measures; nonetheless, the deficit is real and it could indicate the earliest stages of AD.

The issue of subtle, yet meaningful, effects bears directly on the study of nutrition, brain health, and cognitive ageing. It is unlikely that nutritional modulation of brain function will be on the same magnitude as the effects of brain damage; therefore, just as more sensitive tests may be needed to detect the earliest stages of AD, there may also be the need to develop sensitive tests to demonstrate the effects of nutrition on the ageing brain. Indeed, it may be the case that some studies have failed to detect a nutritional enhancement in memory, not because nutrition does not benefit memory, but rather that the tool used could not detect the effects of nutrition. To date, the vast majority of RCT assessing cognitive function following nutritional intervention have used standardised neuropsychological tests that may be ill-suited for detecting subtle cognitive change. Below we provide examples of types of tasks often used in cognitive neuroscience research that may be more successful in illustrating an effect of nutrition on cognition in an aged population.

Relational memory assessment: behavioural tasks

Though the types of tasks described above that measure hippocampal-dependent memory are not without merit, in the assessment of hippocampal function, researchers in the field of cognitive neuroscience often use tasks where participants must remember the relations of novel stimuli (for example, unfamiliar faces, scenes, or computer-generated images). Such tasks that have proven to be sensitive to hippocampal structure and function include asking the participant to remember novel face–scene pairs(Reference Hannula, Tranel and Cohen96, Reference Hannula, Ryan and Tranel97), or the relations among novel, computer-generated images(Reference Konkel, Warren and Duff98). During the ‘study phase’ in the ‘Face–Scene Task’, participants are often shown a background novel scene (such as a generic house or beach) on a computer screen for several seconds, followed by a novel face superimposed on the scene for an additional period of time (for example, 2–10 s; study time can be manipulated to vary performance). After exposure to a series of face–scene pairings, the participant then completes the ‘test phase’, where he or she is asked to indicate if the displayed face was previously shown with the scene, or to choose which of several faces was paired with the background scene. There are many variations of ‘foil’ or ‘lure’ trials, which occur when a participant is shown a new face–scene pair they did not see at study; one example of this are ‘re-pair’ trials, where a participant is shown a previously viewed face with a previously viewed scene, but the specific pair did not occur during the study trials (for more detail, see Hannula et al. (Reference Hannula, Tranel and Cohen96, Reference Hannula, Ryan and Tranel97), Konkel et al. (Reference Konkel, Warren and Duff98) and Monti et al. (Reference Monti, Cooke and Watson99, Reference Monti, Hillman and Cohen100)). In forcing participants to remember pairings composed of novel materials, the researcher is taking advantage of the singular ability of the hippocampus to bind the new material together into a new relational representation. Further evidence for a hippocampal proclivity for novelty comes from functional MRI (fMRI) studies, which find that the hippocampus is more active to novel stimuli or novel configurations of stimuli, suggesting it plays a role in processing novelty (for a review, see Kumaran & Maguire(Reference Kumaran and Maguire101)). Since the effect of nutrition on the hippocampus is likely to be subtle (relative to the effects of brain damage), we suggest that tests assessing relational memory through the use of novel stimuli will maximally engage the hippocampus and raise the likelihood of detecting a change in hippocampal function as a result of nutritional intervention.

An additional consideration in identifying the proper tools for the assessment of nutritional enhancement of relational memory concerns the ability to analyse various conditions and error types within a task. Recently two research groups, independently of each other, have reported a specific error type on a spatial memory task that serves to reveal the nature of hippocampal function. Watson et al. (Reference Watson, Voss and Warren102) and Pertzov et al. (Reference Pertzov, Miller and Gorgoraptis103) had patients with hippocampal damage perform tests where they had to study and remember the spatial locations of several objects over short time delays (for example, 4 s). As an example of this type of test, known as the Spatial Reconstruction Task, participants study the location of several objects (for example, three to five items) on a background such as a blank computer screen; the objective is to remember the location of each object. After the study phase, there is a brief delay where the screen appears blank, followed by the start of the test phase. During the test portion, the objects may appear aligned in a row at the top of the screen, and the participant is asked to use a computer mouse to click-and-drag the objects to where they were presented during the study phase. By not restricting the responses of the participants (i.e. they are free to place the objects anywhere), the data can be analysed in multiple ways. In the studies referenced above, when the experimenters simply assessed how far off in the distance the participant’s placed object was to its original location, the effects of hippocampal damage were mild. However, when researchers made their evaluation at the level of relations between object–location bindings, the hippocampal patients performed much worse. Specifically, those with hippocampal lesions made more ‘swap’ errors in that the locations they chose to place objects were correct, but they chose the wrong objects for those specific locations(Reference Watson, Voss and Warren102, Reference Pertzov, Miller and Gorgoraptis103). Findings from one condition in Watson et al. (Reference Watson, Voss and Warren102) were that patients with hippocampal damage were forty times more likely to make a relational swap error than were the comparison group. A recent study used a version of the task in Watson et al. (Reference Watson, Voss and Warren102) with novel, computer-generated images as stimuli, and found that the swap error rate in healthy middle-aged and older adults had a numerically higher correlation with hippocampal volume than traditional neuropsychological tests of hippocampal memory, such as delayed recall on word lists(Reference Monti, Cooke and Watson99). For more information on these types of tasks, see Monti et al. (Reference Monti, Cooke and Watson99), Watson et al. (Reference Watson, Voss and Warren102) and Pertzov et al. (Reference Pertzov, Miller and Gorgoraptis103).

The use of open-ended tasks such as those in the spatial reconstruction experiments described above more closely reflects the complex nature of memory. In many memory tests, the outcome measure is all-or-nothing; either the word is recalled or it is not. In actuality, memory is a far more graded phenomenon in that individuals may remember parts of an event and forget other details. Therefore, tasks that only employ binary dependent measures (for example, correct v. incorrect) may miss important variance in memory performance. Neuropsychological tasks that incorporate continuous measures of memory, such as response times, along with the binary measures are more likely to capture this variance, as faster responses may be indicative of stronger memory representations (though one must be careful to take baseline response time into account and speed–accuracy trade-offs). Further, the use of confidence judgments (for example, asking individuals how confidently they feel they answered a trial correctly) can also be informative in parsing binary data and capturing individual difference variation on memory paradigms.

Relational memory assessment: eye tracking

Nearly all memory tasks, including the examples above, ask participants to overtly respond to reveal what information they remember. This may occur in the form of a free recall test where the participant recites as many words as he or she can remember, or in recognition memory tasks where individuals press buttons indicating if the presented stimulus is old or new. However, overtly asking participants about their memory need not be the only way to measure this cognitive ability. An alternative approach is to measure memory via other means, and a useful tool for this has emerged in eye tracking. Briefly, the technique involves a camera recording the position of the eye, which can be used to infer precisely where the participant is looking on a computer display with millisecond temporal resolution. As an example, the Face–Scene Task mentioned above has been developed for use in conjunction with eye tracking. In one version of the task, participants study single face–scene pairs, and are then shown three faces in spatially distinct locations superimposed on a background scene. When one of the faces was previously paired with the displayed scene, participants with intact hippocampi disproportionately direct their eye gaze to that face(Reference Hannula, Ryan and Tranel97). Memory has been shown to influence viewing patterns in multiple ways, and the eye-tracking technique provides a plethora of potential analyses due to the richness of eye-tracking data (for a review, see Hannula et al. (Reference Hannula, Althoff and Warren104)). As an example of this measure’s sensitivity to subtle change, eye tracking has proven to be able to detect differences in relational memory following an exercise intervention, even though overt behavioural measures failed to detect any change(Reference Monti, Hillman and Cohen100). Since it is conceivable that the effects of nutrition on relational memory and the brain are to be a similar order of magnitude as those of exercise (i.e. a smaller effect size than gross brain damage), the use of eye tracking in nutritional interventions may provide a sensitive measure to detect post-intervention change.

Working memory assessment: behavioural tasks

A sharpening of the evaluation of working memory in the context of nutrition, brain health and cognitive ageing may also prove fruitful. The metric of working memory tasks is generally the amount of information one can maintain and manipulate at a given time. One class of tests often used to measure working memory entails the simple presentation of a series of numbers or spatial locations, followed by asking participants to repeat the stimuli in the order they were presented; an example would be the Digit Span Forward test from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV(Reference Wechsler105). However, given that there is no demand on manipulating or protecting the presented stimuli from interference, these tasks more accurately reflect the simple temporary storage of information, rather than the construct of working memory. Thus, they do not place as large a demand on the fronto-parietal network and are likely to be insensitive to any modulation of brain function via nutrition. Instead, tests that present strings of numbers, letters or other stimuli and ask participants to manipulate them first, and then repeat them (thereby requiring maintenance and manipulation) place greater demand on working memory.

An example of this type of task would entail reading an individual an interleaved list of numbers and letters, and then asking him/her to repeat the letters in alphabetical order, followed by the numbers in numeric order (i.e. Letter–Number Sequencing(Reference Wechsler105)). For instance, a participant may be read the sequence: ‘L-4-F-2-H-9’ whereby the correct response from the participant would be ‘F-H-L; 2-4-9’. Thus, the participant must not only retain the information provided, but mentally rearrange it in order to provide the correct response. These measures are often used in clinical settings, but another set of tests exists that is used more often in experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. These tests are collectively known as complex-span tasks, and are similar to ‘dual-task’ paradigms in that a participant must remember information while performing a secondary task, thereby introducing an element of interference to working memory maintenance. For example, in a version of the reading span paradigm (originally reported by Daneman & Carpenter(Reference Daneman and Carpenter106)), participants read a sentence and are then asked to determine if the sentence makes sense (some sentences do not make semantic sense, such as ‘It was raining so I reached for my vacuum’). After making this judgment, the participant is presented with a word (for example, ‘tree’ or ‘phone’) and they are to remember this word. This pattern continues for two to ten sentences. At the end of the sequence, the participant is presented with a list of words, some of which were presented during the sequence and some which were not presented. The participant must identify the words they were asked to remember, and can also be asked to put them in the order in which they appeared. Thus, in this task the participant must maintain the single words presented from the interference of reading other words in the sentences and making judgments on them. Other variants of complex-span tasks include performing mathematical operations (operation-span) or spatial information (symmetry-span; for more details on the complex-span tasks, see Conway et al. (Reference Conway, Kane and Bunting107) and Harrison et al. (Reference Harrison, Shipstead and Hicks108)).

The complex-span tasks and other working memory tests emphasising manipulation plus maintenance are more likely to detect effects of nutrition than simple maintenance paradigms. This is in large part due to the added load placed on the working memory system when one must either remember information in the face of interference, and/or manipulate that information. By increasing the stress on the fronto-parietal network to perform working memory computations, individual differences, such as those due to nutritional status or intervention, are more likely to emerge. Furthermore, many of the complex-span tasks are now computer-based, allowing for the collection of response times along with accuracy, providing additional data with which one can assess the effects of nutrition. Finally, the complex-span tasks also typically have more trials than the Digit Span or Letter–Number sequencing tasks; this allows for a more accurate estimation of one’s working memory capacity, since the additional trials reduce noise in the data.

Cognitive assessment tools: summary

There are a few common themes embedded in the discussion of memory tasks potentially more sensitive to nutritional effects on cognition, and these themes can be applied to other realms of cognition such as executive function and processing speed. One major idea involves the use of more difficult tasks. Given that the effects of nutrition are likely to be more evident in healthy older adults, the cognitive assessments employed are not meant to be searching for large ‘holes’ in the brain, but rather, for the subtle variability that characterises cognitive function across even healthy older adults. A second factor involves the collection of more and richer data in cognitive assessment. For instance, one may wish to collect many trials on a task and use average performance to ensure a more representative metric of the cognitive construct that one is measuring, rather than only using the highest number of words recalled on a trial. An even more illuminating approach would be to collect multiple types of data, some of which are continuous in nature (for example, accuracy, response time and eye movements) to more fully characterise an individual’s cognitive ability. These approaches are more likely to make evident individual differences in cognitive performance, and may be more sensitive to the effects of nutritional intervention.

As a final note, the psychometric property of reliability warrants mention. Reliability refers to how likely an individual is to receive a similar score on a test if they take it two consecutive times, assuming no major changes in the individual between test administrations. If a test is unreliable, there could be differences in performance between time point zero and one that are not due to differences within the individual. Many of the tests described above are more experimental in nature, and estimates of reliability for many of these tasks do not exist (though this is not true for all). This is in contrast to most clinical neuropsychology tests, where the reliability of a test is often known and generally high. For the purposes of study design and interpreting results, it would be prudent for researchers to follow the lead of neuropsychologists and ascertain reliability estimates for cognitive tests used in nutritional RCT. For further information regarding these issues specifically to testing for effects of foods and nutrients, we refer the reader to de Jager et al. (Reference de Jager, Dye and de Bruin109).

MRI

The preceding discussion has emphasised the importance of using sensitive cognitive tasks in order to effectively test nutritional enhancement of cognition in the ageing brain; however, a complimentary tool for assessing how nutrition may alter the health of the ageing brain involves using neuroimaging to gather structural and physiological (for example, functional connectivity, blood flow, metabolites) data on the brain. Even in nutritional interventions where there is a null effect on cognitive task data, a finding indicating the sparing of hippocampal tissue as a result of the intervention would surely be regarded as positive in the context of the ageing brain, and this would probably have downstream behavioural effects, even if they cannot be detected by current neuropsychological tests. Below we highlight successful examples of the deployment of MRI to nutrition and cognitive ageing research, and discuss potentially fruitful MRI techniques not yet applied sufficiently in this field. Though this review only covers MRI, we note that there are other neuroimaging techniques such as electroencephalography or near-IR spectroscopy that may be more suitable for certain questions, particularly due to the high cost of MRI. For a full review on neuroimaging techniques and their application to nutrition, we refer the reader to a recent review by Sizonenko et al. (Reference Sizonenko, Babiloni and Sijben110).

Structural imaging: grey matter volume

As noted in the previous section, synaptic loss and modification as well as lower proliferation of new neurons (in the hippocampus) contribute to age-related declines in brain volume and cortical thickness, with areas such as the hippocampus and PFC more affected than other brain regions. Reliable estimates of the volume of cortical and subcortical structures (for example, the hippocampus) as well as the thickness of the cortex can be obtained from standard high-resolution structural MRI. Cross-sectional studies have indicated positive relationships of total grey matter volume in ageing with antioxidant vitamin (B, C, D and E) intake or adherence to a Mediterranean diet(Reference Erickson, Suever and Prakash79, Reference Bowman, Silbert and Howieson91, Reference Scarmeas, Luchsinger and Stern92). Further, results from a recent longitudinal study indicate that plasma levels of DHA and vitamin D are associated with reduced cortical thinning in prefrontal and parietal regions in ageing(Reference Walhovd, Storsve and Westlye111). Perhaps the best evidence for the modulation of brain atrophy via nutrition comes from intervention studies in MCI patients with elevated homocysteine levels and who consumed supplemental B vitamins (vitamins B6, B12 and folate). In one report, Smith et al. (Reference Smith, Smith and de Jager112) noted that after 2 years of supplementation, the overall brain volume of the B vitamin group (relative to the placebo group) was higher, indicating reduced atrophy as a result of B vitamin supplementation. In 2013, this group published similar results, using a more targeted approach to show that these effects in those with elevated homocysteine were greatest in the hippocampus and medial temporal lobe cortex, as well as other posterior brain regions(Reference Douaud, Refsum and de Jager113). This latter report is indicative of the strategy favoured here, where regions particularly susceptible to ageing processes are scrutinised. Finally, and furthering the complexity of this area, a recent report by the same group suggests that the reduction in brain atrophy is partially dependent on plasma n-3 fatty acid levels, with those having higher levels experiencing less atrophy(Reference Jernerén, Elshorbagy and Oulhaj114). It is worth mentioning that the results of these B vitamin interventions occurred in MCI patients who are likely to be experiencing a neurodegenerative process; given that neuron loss is not thought to occur in healthy ageing(Reference Peters, Morrison and Rosene115, Reference Rapp and Gallagher116) the mechanisms through which B vitamins work in the MCI brain may or may not be applicable to the healthy aged brain. Also, structural imaging techniques are best suited for longer interventions (for example, at least 6 months), given that a relatively large amount of change must occur within the brain for detectable differences on standard resolution (for example, 1 mm isotropic) structural scans. Thus, studies using this technique in shorter interventions may fail to detect any nutritive effect on brain atrophy simply because enough time has not passed.

Structural imaging: white matter integrity

In addition to the reduction in size of certain brain regions that are largely made up of grey matter, ageing is also associated with damage to the white matter. One consistent alteration in white matter structure with ageing occurs in the form of ‘white matter hyperintensities’(Reference Gunning-Dixon, Brickman and Cheng117), which are areas in the white matter of high intensity or brightness in magnetic resonance images; these are most apparent on T2-weighted fluid attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) images. White matter hyperintensities are heterogeneous in aetiology, but many are due to ischaemic-related changes in the brain; further, they are most pronounced in the frontal white matter, and have been linked to poorer cognition(Reference Gunning-Dixon and Raz118Reference Schmidt, Schmidt and Haybaeck120). Given the relationship between vasculature health and white matter lesions, it stands to reason that nutrition may have a significant modifying effect on white matter hyperintensities, particularly when considering the potential salutary effect of the Mediterranean diet on cardiovascular health(Reference Estruch, Ros and Salas-Salvadó121Reference Gardener, Scarmeas and Gu123). Consistent with this notion, consumption of fish, as well as serum levels of n-3 fatty acids, has been found to be inversely related to white matter hyperintensities(Reference Bowman, Silbert and Howieson91, Reference Virtanen, Siscovick and Longstreth124, Reference Virtanen, Siscovick and Lemaitre125). Further research focusing on the relationship between nutrition, vascular health and white matter lesions in ageing may prove fruitful in elucidating the modifying effects of nutrition and brain ageing.

Structural imaging: magnetic resonance spectroscopy

An underutilised MR technique in the study of nutrition and brain health is magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), which is a non-invasive technique that allows for the quantification of metabolites in the brain, such as choline, creatine and phosphocreatine, and N-acetyl-aspartate. Previous work has indicated that administration of cytidine diphosphate-choline (Citicoline) to older adults increases phosphodiesters in the brain, potentially indicative of increased phospholipid synthesis and turnover; this increase was also correlated with better memory performance(Reference Babb, Wald and Cohen126). Given the unique data afforded by MRS, coupled with reductions in certain metabolite concentrations, such as N-acetyl-aspartate, in normal ageing(Reference Minati, Grisoli and Bruzzone127), the inclusion of this technique in imaging protocols may provide important data pertaining to brain metabolite changes in older adults following a nutritional intervention.

Functional imaging: arterial spin labelling

A direct measure of neurovascular health can now be obtained in a non-invasive manner through the use of an MRI technique known as arterial spin labelling (ASL(Reference Detre, Leigh and Williams128)). ASL measures the perfusion of blood through tissue, and can yield a quantifiable and objective measure of the rate of blood flow through the brain. The ASL technique represents a significant advance over other MRI methodologies for measuring blood flow, as it does not involve the injection of radioactive isotopes. ASL sequences now come standard on many 3T MRI machines, and have been featured in widespread clinical and research use (for a review on ASL technology and application, see Detre et al. (Reference Detre, Rao and Wang129)). Though typically used to measure resting cerebral blood flow, ASL can also be implemented in conjunction with cognitive tasks, providing an estimate of brain function as well. Furthermore, ASL can be used to not only obtain whole-brain measures of perfusion, but also region-specific rates of blood flow. The application of regional analyses could allow researchers to investigate specific hypotheses regarding a nutritional intervention’s effect on a certain domain of cognition and its putative neural substrate (for example, an intervention may improve relational memory and be correlated with blood flow increases local to the hippocampus; see Heo et al. (Reference Heo, Prakash and Voss130) for such an approach). To the extent that a given nutritional intervention will ameliorate or prevent age-related conditions that reduce cerebral blood flow, such as atherosclerosis, the use of ASL represents a method of untapped potential in investigating nutrition, ageing, and brain health relationships. This technique may be particularly applicable in shorter interventions (less than 6 months) as research from the literature on exercise, ageing and brain health suggests that changes in cerebral blood flow may occur on shorter timescales than changes in gross anatomical structure(Reference Chapman, Aslan and Spence131).

Functional imaging: blood oxygen level dependent signal

A more widespread technique implemented in neuroimaging that relies partially on cerebral blood flow is the use of the blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal in fMRI. Briefly, when a neural event occurs in the brain, there is a shift in the ratio of deoxygenated blood and oxygenated blood, which have different magnetic properties, and this causes a signal increase in the image. Though affected by cerebral blood flow, the BOLD signal is also determined by cerebral blood volume and cerebral oxygen consumption, making it an indirect measure of neural activity(Reference Buxton132). Since these changes in blood oxygenation are quite local within the brain, task-related fMRI, where participants perform a cognitive task during fMRI data collection, has been an excellent tool for mapping brain structure to function. Furthermore, it is possible to compare group differences in task-related activity across time, which may be a fruitful technique for nutrition-related interventions of ageing.

One technique that uses the BOLD signal, but does not depend on task-evoked activity to do so, consists of measuring the brain’s ‘functional connectivity’ while at rest. A common method for assessing functional connectivity is to administer a resting-state scan, which simply consists of using a similar MRI sequence for task-based fMRI, but no stimuli are presented and the subject is instructed to lie still. Due to natural fluctuations in the BOLD response, one can conduct an analysis to ascertain which brain areas show synchrony in their fluctuations, and are therefore said to be functionally connected. A remarkably consistent network of brain regions, termed the ‘default-mode network’, emerges under these conditions (for reviews, see Raichle & Snyder(Reference Raichle and Snyder133) and Buckner et al. (Reference Buckner, Andrews-Hanna and Schacter134)). This brain network includes the medial parietal cortex and PFC, lateral parietal lobe, and the medial temporal lobe. In ageing, the intrinsic functional connections weaken, leading to a decrease in network coherence, with negative implications for cognition(Reference Andrews-Hanna, Snyder and Vincent135). Other networks, such as the fronto-parietal or frontal-executive can also be interrogated with this technique, and there is now research indicating these networks’ malleability in response to exercise intervention(Reference Voss, Prakash and Erickson136). Thus, an important outcome measure for studies of nutrition and ageing could be functional connectivity, specifically the restoration of functionally connected networks to a more youthful state; to our knowledge this area remains unexplored in the study of nutrition, brain health and cognitive ageing.

To summarise, a major reason for the failure of RCT to influence cognition via nutrition in older adults may be that the primary tools selected for measuring cognitive performance were not of the appropriate difficulty to detect subtle change. Rather, we expect that valid and reliable tests that maximally engage the neural networks most susceptible to ageing will illuminate the impact of nutrition on cognition. Further, wherever possible, targeted neuroimaging techniques aimed at assessing the structure and physiological function of these networks should be employed to garner an appreciation for the biological effects that nutrition may have on the ageing brain.

Conclusion

Null results from multiple RCT investigating the cognitive effects of nutritional interventions on ageing may lead to the conclusion that diet does not influence one’s cognitive ageing trajectory; however, to draw such an inference would be premature. Rather, a reassessment of strategy and methodology in this field may provide more optimism for detecting nutritional interactions with cognition, particularly in the context of ageing and brain health late in life. We suggest placing a larger emphasis on studying healthy older adults, potentially even those in middle-age, to understand the role of nutrition in the maintenance of brain health and the prevention of disease. Critically, the effects of nutrition and cognition in ageing are more likely to manifest if researchers study cognitive domains and brain regions particularly sensitive to the effects of ageing, and assess these areas with more challenging behavioural tests and cutting-edge imaging techniques. With these advances in experimental methodology, we expect that the differential findings between cross-sectional and RCT studies in human subjects will be somewhat reconciled, and a more truthful picture of nutrition’s effects on cognitive ageing and brain health will be established.

Acknowledgements

The present review was supported by the Center for Nutrition, Learning, and Memory at the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.

J. M. M., C. J. M. and N. J. C. conceived the idea for the paper. J. M. M. wrote the paper; J. M. M., C. J. M. and N. J. C. edited and revised the paper.

J. M. M. and C. J. M. are employees of Abbott Nutrition.

References

1.Lourida, I, Soni, M, Thompson-Coon, J, et al. (2013) Mediterranean diet, cognitive function, and dementia: a systematic review. Epidemiology 24, 479489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2.Singh, B, Parsaik, AK, Mielke, MM, et al. (2014) Association of Mediterranean diet with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis 39, 271282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3.Van de Rest, O, Berendsen, AA, Haveman-Nies, A, et al. (2015) Dietary patterns, cognitive decline, and dementia: a systematic review. Adv Nutr 6, 154168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4.Morris, MC (2012) Nutritional determinants of cognitive aging and dementia. Proc Nutr Soc 71, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5.Gomez-Pinilla, F (2008) Brain foods: the effects of nutrients on brain function. Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 568578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6.Morris, MC & Tangney, CC (2014) Dietary fat composition and dementia risk. Neurobiol Aging 35, Suppl. 2, S59S64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7.Kennard, JA & Harrison, FE (2014) Intravenous ascorbate improves spatial memory in middle-aged APP/PSEN1 and wild type mice. Behav Brain Res 264, 3442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8.Kim, SJ, Son, TG, Park, HR, et al. (2008) Curcumin stimulates proliferation of embryonic neural progenitor cells and neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus. J Biol Chem 283, 1449714505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9.Frautschy, SA, Hu, W, Kim, P, et al. (2001) Phenolic anti-inflammatory antioxidant reversal of Aβ-induced cognitive deficits and neuropathology. Neurobiol Aging 22, 9931005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10.Green, KN, Martinez-Coria, H, Khashwji, H, et al. (2007) Dietary docosahexaenoic acid and docosapentaenoic acid ameliorate amyloid-β and tau pathology via a mechanism involving presenilin 1 levels. J Neurosci 27, 43854395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
11.Lim, GP, Chu, T, Yang, F, et al. (2001) The curry spice curcumin reduces oxidative damage and amyloid pathology in an Alzheimer transgenic mouse. J Neurosci 21, 83708377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12.Wang, J, Ho, L, Zhao, W, et al. (2008) Grape-derived polyphenolics prevent Aβ oligomerization and attenuate cognitive deterioration in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci 28, 63886392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
13.Fahnestock, M, Marchese, M, Head, E, et al. (2012) BDNF increases with behavioral enrichment and an antioxidant diet in the aged dog. Neurobiol Aging 33, 546554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
14.Milgram, NW, Zicker, SC, Head, E, et al. (2002) Dietary enrichment counteracts age-associated cognitive dysfunction in canines. Neurobiol Aging 23, 737745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15.Pop, V, Head, E, Hill, M-A, et al. (2010) Synergistic effects of long-term antioxidant diet and behavioral enrichment on β-amyloid load and non-amyloidogenic processing in aged canines. J Neurosci 30, 98319839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16.Aisen, PS, Schneider, LS, Sano, M, et al. (2008) High-dose B vitamin supplementation and cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease: a randomized controlled trial. JAMA 300, 17741783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17.Dangour, AD, Allen, E, Elbourne, D, et al. (2010) Effect of 2-y n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation on cognitive function in older people : a randomized, double-blind, controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 91, 17251732.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18.Ford, AH, Flicker, L, Alfonso, H, et al. (2010) Vitamins B12, B6, and folic acid for cognition in older men. Neurology 75, 15401547.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19.Kwok, T, Lee, J, Law, CB, et al. (2011) A randomized placebo controlled trial of homocysteine lowering to reduce cognitive decline in older demented people. Clin Nutr 30, 297302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20.McMahon, JA, Green, TJ, Skeaff, CM, et al. (2006) A controlled trial of homocysteine lowering and cognitive performance. N Engl J Med 354, 27642772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21.Petersen, RC, Thomas, RG, Grundman, M, et al. (2005) Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med 352, 23792788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22.Quinn, JF, Raman, R, Thomas, RG, et al. (2010) Docosahexaenoic acid supplementation and cognitive decline in Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA 304, 19031911.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23.Sun, Y, Lu, C-J, Chien, K-L, et al. (2007) Efficacy of multivitamin supplementation containing vitamins B6 and B12 and folic acid as adjunctive treatment with a cholinesterase inhibitor in Alzheimer’s disease: a 26-week, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study in Taiwanese patients. Clin Ther 29, 22042214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24.Van de Rest, O, Geleijnse, JM, Kok, FJ, et al. (2008) Effect of fish oil on cognitive performance in older subjects: a randomized, controlled trial. Neurology 71, 430438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25.Kang, JH, Cook, N, Manson, J, et al. (2006) A randomized trial of vitamin E supplementation and cognitive function in women. Arch Intern Med 166, 24622468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26.Benton, D (2010) Neurodevelopment and neurodegeneration: are there critical stages for nutritional intervention? Nutr Rev 68, Suppl. 1, S6S10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27.De Jager, CA & Kovatcheva, A (2010) Summary and discussion: methodologies to assess long-term effects of nutrition on brain function. Nutr Rev 68, Suppl. 1, S53S58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28.Otaegui-Arrazola, A, Amiano, P, Elbusto, A, et al. (2014) Diet, cognition, and Alzheimer’s disease: food for thought. Eur J Nutr 53, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29.Freund-Levi, Y, Eriksdotter-Jönhagen, M, Cederholm, T, et al. (2006) Omega-3 fatty acid treatment in 174 patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer disease: OmegAD Study: a randomized double-blind study. Arch Neurol 63, 14021408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30.Sperling, RA, Aisen, PS, Beckett, L, et al. (2011) Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging–Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement 7, 280292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31.Jack, CR, Knopman, DS, Jagust, WJ, et al. (2010) Hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers of the Alzheimer’s pathological cascade. Lancet Neurol 9, 119128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32.Perneczky, R, Wagenpfeil, S, Komossa, K, et al. (2006) Mapping scores onto stages: Mini-Mental State Examination and clinical dementia rating. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 14, 139144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33.Aisen, PS, Cummings, J & Schneider, LS (2012) Symptomatic and nonamyloid/tau based pharmacologic treatment for Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med 2, a006395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34.De Jager, CA, Oulhaj, A, Jacoby, R, et al. (2012) Cognitive and clinical outcomes of homocysteine-lowering B-vitamin treatment in mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 27, 592600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35.Dufouil, C, Alpérovitch, A, Ducros, V, et al. (2003) Homocysteine, white matter hyperintensities, and cognition in healthy elderly people. Ann Neurol 53, 214221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36.Seshadri, S, Beiser, A, Selhub, J, et al. (2002) Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med 346, 476483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37.Chiu, C-C, Su, K-P, Cheng, T-C, et al. (2008) The effects of omega-3 fatty acids monotherapy in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment: a preliminary randomized double-blind placebo-controlled study. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 32, 15381544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38.Kotani, S, Sakaguchi, E, Warashina, S, et al. (2006) Dietary supplementation of arachidonic and docosahexaenoic acids improves cognitive dysfunction. Neurosci Res 56, 159164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39.Lee, LK, Shahar, S, Chin, A-V, et al. (2013) Docosahexaenoic acid-concentrated fish oil supplementation in subjects with mild cognitive impairment (MCI): a 12-month randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 225, 605612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40.Van Uffelen, JGZ, Chinapaw, MJM, van Mechelen, W, et al. (2008) Walking or vitamin B for cognition in older adults with mild cognitive impairment? A randomised controlled trial. Br J Sports Med 42, 344351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41.Eussen, SJ, de Groot, LC, Joosten, LW, et al. (2006) Effect of oral vitamin B-12 with or without folic acid on cognitive function in older people with mild vitamin B-12 deficiency: a randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 84, 361370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42.Kwok, T, Lee, J, Lam, L, et al. (2008) Vitamin B12 supplementation did not improve cognition but reduced delirium in demented patients with vitamin B12 deficiency. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 46, 273282.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43.Morris, MC, Evans, DA, Schneider, JA, et al. (2006) Dietary folate and vitamins B-12 and B-6 not associated with incident Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 9, 435443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
44.Engelhart, MJ, Geerlings, MI, Ruitenberg, A, et al. (2002) Dietary intake of antioxidants and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA 287, 32233229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45.Morris, MC, Evans, DA, Bienias, JL, et al. (2002) Vitamin E and cognitive decline in older persons. Arch Neurol 59, 11251132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46.Park, DC, Lautenschlager, G, Hedden, T, et al. (2002) Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychol Aging 17, 299320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47.Grodstein, F, Kang, JH, Glynn, RJ, et al. (2007) A randomized trial of β carotene supplementation and cognitive function in men: the Physicians’ Health Study II. 167, 21842190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
48.Yurko-Mauro, K, McCarthy, D, Rom, D, et al. (2010) Beneficial effects of docosahexaenoic acid on cognition in age-related cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement 6, 456464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49.Durga, J, van Boxtel, MPJ, Schouten, EG, et al. (2007) Effect of 3-year folic acid supplementation on cognitive function in older adults in the FACIT trial: a randomised, double blind, controlled trial. Lancet 369, 208216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
50.Clarke, R, Bennett, D, Parish, S, et al. (2014) Effects of homocysteine lowering with B vitamins on cognitive aging: meta-analysis of 11 trials with cognitive data on 22,000. Am J Clin Nutr 100, 657666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51.van der Zwaluw, NL, Dhonukshe-Rutten, RA, Van Wijngaarden, JP, et al. (2014) Results of 2-year vitamin B treatment on cognitive performance: secondary data from an RCT. Neurology 83, 21582166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52.Andreeva, VA, Kesse-Guyot, E, Barberger-Gateau, P, et al. (2011) Cognitive function after supplementation with B vitamins and long-chain omega-3 fatty acids: ancillary findings from the SU.FOL.OM3 randomized trial. Am J Clin Nutr 94, 278286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53.Macready, AL, Butler, LT, Kennedy, OB, et al. (2010) Cognitive tests used in chronic adult human randomised controlled trial micronutrient and phytochemical intervention studies. Nutr Res Rev 23, 200229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54.Jagust, W (2013) Vulnerable neural systems and the borderland of brain aging and neurodegeneration. Neuron 77, 219234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55.Raz, N, Ghisletta, P, Rodrigue, KM, et al. (2010) Trajectories of brain aging in middle-aged and older adults: regional and individual differences. NeuroImage 51, 501511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56.Yankner, BA, Lu, T & Loerch, P (2008) The aging brain. Annu Rev Pathol 3, 4166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57.Nyberg, L, Maitland, SB, Rönnlund, M, et al. (2003) Selective adult age differences in an age-invariant multifactor model of declarative memory. Psychol Aging 18, 149160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
58.Fjell, AM, Westlye, LT, Grydeland, H, et al. (2013) Critical ages in the life course of the adult brain: nonlinear subcortical aging. Neurobiol Aging 34, 22392247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59.Morrison, JH & Baxter, MG (2012) The ageing cortical synapse: hallmarks and implications for cognitive decline. Nat Rev Neurosci 13, 240250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60.Bennett, DA, Wilson, RS, Boyle, PA, et al. (2012) Relation of neuropathology to cognition in persons without cognitive impairment. Ann Neurol 72, 599609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
61.Braak, H, Thal, DR, Ghebremedhin, E, et al. (2011) Stages of the pathologic process in Alzheimer disease: age categories from 1 to 100 years. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 70, 960969.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62.Eichenbaum, H & Cohen, NJ (2001) From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection: Multiple Memory Systems in the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
63.Cohen, N & Eichenbaum, HJ (1993) Memory, Amnesia and the Hippocampal System. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
64.Konkel, A & Cohen, NJ (2009) Relational memory and the hippocampus: representations and methods. Front Neurosci 3, 166174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65.Naveh-Benjamin, M (2000) Adult age differences in memory performance: tests of an associative deficit hypothesis. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 26, 11701187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
66.Cabeza, R, Ciaramelli, E, Olson, IR, et al. (2008) The parietal cortex and episodic memory: an attentional account. Nat Rev Neurosci 9, 613625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
67.Simons, JS & Spiers, HJ (2003) Prefrontal and medial temporal lobe interactions in long-term memory. Nat Rev Neurosci 4, 637648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
68.Wagner, AD, Shannon, BJ, Kahn, I, et al. (2005) Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn Sci 9, 445453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69.Casadesus, G, Shukitt-Hale, B, Stellwagen, HM, et al. (2004) Modulation of hippocampal plasticity and cognitive behavior by short-term blueberry supplementation in aged rats. Nutr Neurosci 7, 309316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70.Van Praag, H, Lucero, MJ, Yeo, GW, et al. (2007) Plant-derived flavanol (–)epicatechin enhances angiogenesis and retention of spatial memory in mice. J Neurosci 27, 58695878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71.He, C, Qu, X, Cui, L, et al. (2009) Improved spatial learning performance of fat-1 mice is associated with enhanced neurogenesis and neuritogenesis by docosahexaenoic acid. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106, 1137011375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
72.Petursdottir, AL, Farr, SA, Morley, JE, et al. (2008) Effect of dietary n-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids on brain lipid fatty acid composition, learning ability, and memory of senescence-accelerated mouse. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 63, 11531160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
73.Wang, J, Santa-Maria, I, Ho, L, et al. (2010) Grape derived polyphenols attenuate tau neuropathology in a mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis 22, 653661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
74.Lim, GP, Calon, F, Morihara, T, et al. (2005) A diet enriched with the omega-3 fatty acid docosahexaenoic acid reduces amyloid burden in an aged Alzheimer mouse model. J Neurosci 25, 30323040.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
75.Gazzaley, A & Nobre, AC (2012) Top-down modulation: bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends Cogn Sci 16, 129135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
76.Kane, MJ & Engle, RW (2002) The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: an individual-differences perspective. Psychon Bull Rev 9, 637671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
77.Barbey, AK, Colom, R, Solomon, J, et al. (2012) An integrative architecture for general intelligence and executive function revealed by lesion mapping. Brain 135, 11541164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
78.Buckner, R (2004) Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 44, 195208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
79.Erickson, K, Suever, B, Prakash, R, et al. (2008) Greater intake of vitamins B6 and B12 spares gray matter in healthy elderly: a voxel-based morphometry study. Brain Res 1553, 2026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
80.Rodrigue, KM, Kennedy, KM, Devous, MD, et al. (2012) β-Amyloid burden in healthy aging: regional distribution and cognitive consequences. Neurology 78, 387395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
81.Sperling, RA, Laviolette, PS, O’Keefe, K, et al. (2009) Amyloid deposition is associated with impaired default network function in older persons without dementia. Neuron 63, 178188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
82.Kennedy, KM, Rodrigue, KM, Devous, MD, et al. (2012) Effects of β-amyloid accumulation on neural function during encoding across the adult lifespan. NeuroImage 62, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
83.Rentz, DM, Amariglio, RE, Becker, JA, et al. (2011) Face–name associative memory performance is related to amyloid burden in normal elderly. Neuropsychologia 49, 27762783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
84.Bayer-Carter, JL, Green, PS, Montine, TJ, et al. (2011) Diet intervention and cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 68, 743752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
85.Salthouse, TA (2009) When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging 30, 507514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
86.Salthouse, TA (1996) The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev 103, 403428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
87.Salthouse, TA (2010) Selective review of cognitive aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 16, 754760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
88.Borghesani, PR, Madhyastha, TM, Aylward, EH, et al. (2013) The association between higher order abilities, processing speed, and age are variably mediated by white matter integrity during typical aging. Neuropsychologia 51, 14351444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
89.Wen, W, Zhu, W, He, Y, et al. (2011) Discrete neuroanatomical networks are associated with specific cognitive abilities in old age. J Neurosci 31, 12041212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
90.Wen, W & Sachdev, P (2004) The topography of white matter hyperintensities on brain MRI in healthy 60- to 64-year-old individuals. NeuroImage 22, 144154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
91.Bowman, GL, Silbert, LC, Howieson, D, et al. (2012) Nutrient biomarker patterns, cognitive function, and MRI measures of brain aging. Neurology 78, 241249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
92.Scarmeas, N, Luchsinger, J, Stern, Y, et al. (2011) Mediterranean diet and magnetic resonance imaging-assessed cerebrovascular disease. Ann Neurol 69, 257268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
93.Lezak, MD, Howieson, DB, Loring, DW, et al. (2004) Neuropsychological Assessment, 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
94.Weintraub, S, Wicklund, AH & Salmon, DP (2012) The neuropsychological profile of Alzheimer disease. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med 2, a006171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
95.Jessen, F, Wiese, B, Bachmann, C, et al. (2010) Prediction of dementia by subjective memory impairment: effects of severity and temporal association with cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry 67, 414422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
96.Hannula, DE, Tranel, D & Cohen, NJ (2006) The long and the short of it: relational memory impairments in amnesia, even at short lags. J Neurosci 26, 83528359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
97.Hannula, DE, Ryan, JD, Tranel, D, et al. (2007) Rapid onset relational memory effects are evident in eye movement behavior, but not in hippocampal amnesia. J Cogn Neurosci 19, 16901705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
98.Konkel, A, Warren, DE, Duff, MC, et al. (2008) Hippocampal amnesia impairs all manner of relational memory. Front Hum Neurosci 2, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
99.Monti, JM, Cooke, GE, Watson, PD, et al. (2015) Relating hippocampus to relational memory processing across domains and delays. J Cogn Neurosci 27, 234245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
100.Monti, JM, Hillman, CH & Cohen, NJ (2012) Aerobic fitness enhances relational memory in preadolescent children: the FITKids randomized control trial. Hippocampus 22, 18761882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
101.Kumaran, D & Maguire, EA (2009) Novelty signals: a window into hippocampal information processing. Trends Cogn Sci 13, 4754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
102.Watson, PD, Voss, JL, Warren, DE, et al. (2013) Spatial reconstruction by patients with hippocampal damage is dominated by relational memory errors. Hippocampus 23, 570580.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
103.Pertzov, Y, Miller, TD, Gorgoraptis, N, et al. (2013) Binding deficits in memory following medial temporal lobe damage in patients with voltage-gated potassium channel complex antibody-associated limbic encephalitis. Brain 136, 24742485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
104.Hannula, DE, Althoff, RR, Warren, DE, et al. (2010) Worth a glance: using eye movements to investigate the cognitive neuroscience of memory. Front Hum Neurosci 4, 166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
105.Wechsler, D (2008) Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th ed. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar
106.Daneman, M & Carpenter, PA (1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. J Verbal Learn Verbal Behav 19, 450466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
107.Conway, AR, Kane, MJ, Bunting, MF, et al. (2005) Working memory span tasks: a methodological review and user’s guide. Psychonom Bull Rev 12, 769786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
108.Harrison, TL, Shipstead, Z, Hicks, KL, et al. (2013) Working memory training may increase working memory capacity but not fluid intelligence. Psychol Sci 24, 24092419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
109.de Jager, CA, Dye, L, de Bruin, EA, et al. (2014) Criteria for validation and selection of cognitive tests for investigating the effects of foods and nutrients. Nutr Rev 72, 162179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
110.Sizonenko, SV, Babiloni, C, Sijben, JW, et al. (2013) Brain imaging and human nutrition: which measures to use in intervention studies? Adv Nutr 4, 554556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
111.Walhovd, KB, Storsve, AB, Westlye, LT, et al. (2014) Blood markers of fatty acids and vitamin D, cardiovascular measures, body mass index, and physical activity relate to longitudinal cortical thinning in normal aging. Neurobiol Aging 35, 10551064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
112.Smith, AD, Smith, SM, de Jager, CA, et al. (2010) Homocysteine-lowering by B vitamins slows the rate of accelerated brain atrophy in mild cognitive impairment: a randomized controlled trial. PLoS ONE 5, e12244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
113.Douaud, G, Refsum, H, de Jager, CA, et al. (2013) Preventing Alzheimer’s disease-related gray matter atrophy by B-vitamin treatment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110, 95239528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
114.Jernerén, F, Elshorbagy, AK, Oulhaj, A, et al. (2015) Brain atrophy in cognitively impaired elderly: the importance of long-chain ω-3 fatty acids and B vitamin status in a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 102, 215221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
115.Peters, A, Morrison, JH, Rosene, DL, et al. (1998) Feature article: are neurons lost from the primate cerebral cortex during normal aging? Cereb Cortex 8, 295300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
116.Rapp, PR & Gallagher, M (1996) Preserved neuron number in the hippocampus of aged rats with spatial learning deficits. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93, 99269930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
117.Gunning-Dixon, FM, Brickman, AM, Cheng, JC, et al. (2009) Aging of cerebral white matter: a review of MRI findings. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 24, 109117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
118.Gunning-Dixon, FM & Raz, N (2000) The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging: a quantitative review. Neuropsychology 14, 224232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
119.Sachdev, P, Wen, W, Chen, X, et al. (2007) Progression of white matter hyperintensities in elderly individuals over 3 years. Neurology 68, 214222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
120.Schmidt, R, Schmidt, H, Haybaeck, J, et al. (2011) Heterogeneity in age-related white matter changes. Acta Neuropathol 122, 171185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
121.Estruch, R, Ros, E, Salas-Salvadó, J, et al. (2013) Primary prevention of cardiovascular disease with a Mediterranean diet. N Engl J Med 368, 12791290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
122.Rees, K, Hartley, L, Flowers, N, et al. (2013) “Mediterranean” dietary pattern for the primary prevention of cardiovascular disease. The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2013, issue 8, CD009825. http://www.mrw.interscience.wiley.com/cochrane/clsysrev/articles/CD009825/frame.htmlCrossRefGoogle Scholar
123.Gardener, H, Scarmeas, N, Gu, Y, et al. (2012) Mediterranean diet and white matter hyperintensity volume in Northern Manhattan Study. Arch Neurol 69, 251256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
124.Virtanen, JK, Siscovick, DS, Longstreth, WT, et al. (2008) Fish consumption and risk of subclinical brain abnormalities on MRI in older adults. Neurology 71, 439446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
125.Virtanen, JK, Siscovick, DS, Lemaitre, RN, et al. (2013) Circulating omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids and subclinical brain abnormalities on MRI in older adults: the Cardiovascular Health Study. J Am Heart Assoc 2, e000305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
126.Babb, SM, Wald, LL, Cohen, BM, et al. (2002) Chronic citicoline increases phosphodiesters in the brains of healthy older subjects: an in vivo phosphorus magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 161, 248254.Google Scholar
127.Minati, L, Grisoli, M & Bruzzone, MG (2007) MR spectroscopy, functional MRI, and diffusion-tensor imaging in the aging brain: a conceptual review. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 20, 321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
128.Detre, JA, Leigh, JS, Williams, DS, et al. (1992) Perfusion imaging. Magn Reson Med 23, 3745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
129.Detre, J, Rao, H & Wang, D (2012) Applications of arterial spin labeled MRI in the brain. J Mag Reson Imaging 35, 10261037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
130.Heo, S, Prakash, RS, Voss, MW, et al. (2010) Resting hippocampal blood flow, spatial memory and aging. Brain Res 1315, 119127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
131.Chapman, SB, Aslan, S, Spence, JS, et al. (2013) Shorter term aerobic exercise improves brain, cognition, and cardiovascular fitness in aging. Front Aging Neurosci 5, 75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
132.Buxton, R (2002) Introduction to Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Principles and Techniques. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
133.Raichle, ME & Snyder, AZ (2007) A default mode of brain function: a brief history of an evolving idea. NeuroImage 37, 10831090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
134.Buckner, RL, Andrews-Hanna, JR & Schacter, DL (2008) The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124, 138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
135.Andrews-Hanna, JR, Snyder, AZ, Vincent, JL, et al. (2007) Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 56, 924935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
136.Voss, MW, Prakash, RS, Erickson, KI, et al. (2010) Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2, 32.Google Scholar