Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:22:05.798Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Peripheral and central mechanisms involved in the control of food intake by dietary amino acids and proteins

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 May 2012

Gilles Fromentin*
Affiliation:
INRA, CNRH-IdF, UMR914 Nutrition Physiology and Ingestive Behaviour, F-75005Paris, France AgroParisTech, CNRH-IdF, UMR914 Nutrition Physiology and Ingestive Behaviour, F-75005Paris, France
Nicolas Darcel
Affiliation:
AgroParisTech, CNRH-IdF, UMR914 Nutrition Physiology and Ingestive Behaviour, F-75005Paris, France Chaire ANCA, F-75005Paris, France
Catherine Chaumontet
Affiliation:
INRA, CNRH-IdF, UMR914 Nutrition Physiology and Ingestive Behaviour, F-75005Paris, France AgroParisTech, CNRH-IdF, UMR914 Nutrition Physiology and Ingestive Behaviour, F-75005Paris, France
Agnes Marsset-Baglieri
Affiliation:
INRA, CNRH-IdF, UMR914 Nutrition Physiology and Ingestive Behaviour, F-75005Paris, France AgroParisTech, CNRH-IdF, UMR914 Nutrition Physiology and Ingestive Behaviour, F-75005Paris, France
Nachiket Nadkarni
Affiliation:
Chaire ANCA, F-75005Paris, France
Daniel Tomé
Affiliation:
INRA, CNRH-IdF, UMR914 Nutrition Physiology and Ingestive Behaviour, F-75005Paris, France AgroParisTech, CNRH-IdF, UMR914 Nutrition Physiology and Ingestive Behaviour, F-75005Paris, France
*
*Corresponding author: Dr Gilles Fromentin, fax +33 1 44 08 18 58, email [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The present review summarises current knowledge and recent findings on the modulation of appetite by dietary protein, via both peripheral and central mechanisms. Of the three macronutrients, proteins are recognised as the strongest inhibitor of food intake. The well-recognised poor palatability of proteins is not the principal mechanism explaining the decrease in high-protein (HP) diet intake. Consumption of a HP diet does not induce conditioned food aversion, but rather experience-enhanced satiety. Amino acid consumption is detected by multiple and redundant mechanisms originating from visceral (during digestion) and metabolic (inter-prandial period) sources, recorded both directly and indirectly (mainly vagus-mediated) by the central nervous system (CNS). Peripherally, the satiating effect of dietary proteins appears to be mediated by anorexigenic gut peptides, principally cholecystokinin, glucagon-like peptide-1 and peptide YY. In the CNS, HP diets trigger the activation of noradrenergic and adrenergic neurons in the nucleus of the solitary tract and melanocortin neurons in the arcuate nucleus. Additionally, there is evidence that circulating leucine levels may modulate food intake. Leucine is associated with neural mechanisms involving mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR) and AMP-activated protein kinase (AMPK), energy sensors active in the control of energy intake, at least in the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus. In addition, HP diets inhibit the activation of opioid and GABAergic neurons in the nucleus accumbens, and thus inhibit food intake by reducing the hedonic response to food, presumably because of their low palatability. Future studies should concentrate on studying the adaptation of different neural circuits following the ingestion of protein diets.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Introduction

The macronutrient composition of a diet is well known to influence energy intake, energy metabolism and long-term changes to body weight and body composition. High-protein (HP) diets have been extensively studied for their ability to reduce total energy intake and body weight, and to limit fat deposition(Reference Jean, Rome and Mathe1). Among the three macronutrients, protein has been shown to have the greatest satiating effect(Reference Poppitt, McCormack and Buffenstein2). Indeed, dietary proteins are potent inducers of satiety and inhibitors of food intake in both rats(Reference Bensaid, Tome and Gietzen3) and man(Reference Latner and Schwartz4, Reference Westerterp-Plantenga, Nieuwenhuizen and Tome5).

Appetite or hunger is the internal driving force for search, choice and ingestion of food in order to maintain energy homeostasis and body weight, and is therefore responsible for meal initiation. Appetite is antagonised by both satiation (defined as the physiological process that leads to meal termination) and satiety (defined as the period after a meal before the onset of hunger).

Numerous partially redundant hypotheses have been proposed to explain the effects of dietary protein and amino acids on food intake decrease(Reference Latner and Schwartz4Reference Tome, Schwarz and Darcel6). All proposed theories mostly differ on the putative location at which the reduction in eating signals is initiated. After nutrient ingestion, pre-absorptive signals originating from the intestine are transmitted to the brain's satiety centres, via the action of gut peptides on peripheral nerves or via the bloodstream. Post-absorptive signals, occurring after nutrients and/or gut peptides cross the gut wall and enter circulation, are initiated in the hepatic–portal zone. Finally, signals relative to the status of energy stores (leptin and insulin levels) should also be considered as potent mediators for the effect of protein on ingestive behaviour(Reference Schwartz, Woods and Porte7).

Furthermore, the brain areas and neuronal populations responsible for integrating these sensory signals are not yet fully understood. All these latter signals are integrated in the central nervous system where structures in the reward system, the hypothalamus and brainstem are important for the regulation of energy homeostasis as well as the onset of appetite and satiety(Reference Berthoud8). The negative-feedback control of meal size transmitted by gastrointestinal signals and the bloodstream takes place in the dorsal vagal complex in the brainstem, and in the hypothalamic nuclei. Recently, evidence has been accumulating for a modulation of the reward system with the main consequence being the reduction of food wanting. This component of reward is known to be a major driver for eating behaviour.

The purpose of the present review is to assemble current views and knowledge concerning the neuronal mechanisms associated with peripheral and central signalling processes through which dietary protein and amino acids influence the control of energy intake.

Effect of protein and amino acid intake on overall energy intake, body weight and body composition

Protein snacks (or loads)

In the short term dietary protein appears to be a strong appetite inhibitor and reduces food intake in subsequent meals beyond that which can be accounted for by its energy content alone, both in rats(Reference Bensaid, Tome and Gietzen3, Reference Trigazis, Orttmann and Anderson9) and in man(Reference Porrini, Santangelo and Crovetti10). Moreover in man(Reference Marmonier, Chapelot and Louis-Sylvestre11) and in rats(Reference Burton-Freeman, Gietzen and Schneeman12), consumption of a HP snack delayed the request for the following meal (inter-meal interval).

In rats(Reference Bensaid, Tome and Gietzen3) and man(Reference Porrini, Santangelo and Crovetti10), protein is currently believed to present the greatest appetite-suppressing effect of the three macronutrients. In these human studies ingestion of a HP load induces a difference in the decrease in the total meal or daily food intake in comparison with carbohydrate(Reference Bertenshaw, Lluch and Yeomans13) or lipid loads(Reference Porrini, Santangelo and Crovetti10) or both(Reference Poppitt, McCormack and Buffenstein2, Reference Latner and Schwartz4). However, the commonly acknowledged hierarchy proteins>carbohydrates>lipids with respect to satiety is not always observed in rats(Reference Geliebter, Liang and Van Itallie14), or in human subjects(Reference Marmonier, Chapelot and Fantino15, Reference Raben, Agerholm-Larsen and Flint16).

Many of these discrepancies have been shown to originate from the physiological status of the subjects, the duration of eating and the method of nutrient administration (meaning oral, intra-gastric or intravenous) and the nature of the load(Reference Reid and Hetherington17). Nowadays, special attention must be given to the characteristics of the protein load: structure, texture, volume, energy density and palatability can influence the satiation or satiety induced by it. We must also take care in the selection of the subjects according to their physiological status and function (such as BMI) in conjunction with their cognitive restraint and disinhibition scores.

It has been claimed that beverages elicit weaker appetitive and/or dietary responses than solid foods(Reference Mattes18). Leidy has suggested that differences in satiety and food intake between protein and other macronutrients are only observed when protein is consumed as a solid and not liquid. Leidy et al. (Reference Leidy, Bales-Voelker and Harris19) showed that a protein-rich beverage consumed as a breakfast meal leads to weaker appetitive and dietary responses v. a protein-rich solid breakfast meal in adolescents. Martens et al. (Reference Martens, Lemmens and Born20) also investigated the differences in appetite profile and physiological parameters after consumption of a single-macronutrient, subject-specific, HP meal in liquefied v. solid form, controlled for energy density, weight and volume in ten lean male subjects. They observed a stronger suppression of hunger and desire to eat with the solid protein than liquefied protein. However, the hypothesis that energy consumed from solid-protein foods evokes a greater satiety response and suppresses energy intake at a subsequent meal compared with liquid foods is still unresolved. The studies of Akhavan et al. (Reference Akhavan, Luhovyy and Anderson21) tested the hypothesis that liquid energy is less satiating than solid energy by using pure macronutrients and similar tastes, ingredients, volumes and matrices to eliminate the confounding factors of taste, texture, smell, structure and familiarity of foods. Akhavan et al. (Reference Akhavan, Luhovyy and Anderson21) showed that macronutrient composition is more important than the physical state of foods in determining subjective appetite. In respect to this question, in a previous human study(Reference Potier, Fromentin and Lesdema22) we compared the effect on satiety of different liquid preloads consisting only of protein, fat or carbohydrate in very controlled conditions; the preloads had the same volume, energy content, energy density, viscosity and palatability, since it had been shown that all these properties of food can affect food intake. Our results did not confirm the highest satiety effect of proteins, maybe because we used liquid, novel and unusual foods as preloads.

As with all foods, consumption of those containing proteins will generate valuable oro-sensory information due to their organoleptic properties; the latter are probably influenced by the presence of proteins in the food but are not specific. Indeed, mixtures containing proteins will have different final food textures when conditions of food processing (conditions of heating, for instance) are modified. Oro-sensory properties of foods containing proteins will be used by animals and man in learning processes such as conditioned food satiety(Reference Fromentin, Feurte and Nicolaidis23). These learning processes are likely to be involved in the satiety effect of dietary proteins. After several times of eating foods containing proteins, the subject will learn to associate the food oro-sensory properties with the pre- and post-ingestive effects of eating such a food. This has been shown in rats by Bensaid et al. (Reference Bensaid, Tome and Gietzen3). It was shown that 2 d were necessary before a significant decrease was seen in the energy intake of the next protein meal, which means that as soon as the second load day, rats have learned to associate the sensory properties of the protein load with its post-ingestive consequences. In contrast, in human subjects only a few studies have shown that repeated consumption of the same food over a few days constitutes a learning phase for the effects on appetite of this food, which then determines food intake during the post-training test(Reference Booth, Lee and McAleavey24Reference Yeomans, Weinberg and James26). In all the articles the effect of repeated consumption on short-term food intake was studied using loads varying in energy density. However, the effect of protein was not determined.

Finally, protein type can affect satiety in rats (for instance, yeast proteins)(Reference Faipoux, Tome and Bensaid27) and in human subjects (whey or soya protein v. albumin)(Reference Anderson, Tecimer and Shah28). According to Veldhorst et al. (Reference Veldhorst, Nieuwenhuizen and Hochstenbach-Waelen29), the protein type effect could be more efficient when the protein level in the diet is not too high: differences in appetite ratings between types of protein appear when certain amino acids are above or below particular threshold values (10 and 25 % of the protein:energy ratio, respectively). However, the effect of the nature of the proteins was not present in all experiments. Indeed, various proteins have been used in other experiments described in the literature, and it is still not clear if the nature of the protein can significantly affect the feeding response studied (egg albumin, casein, soya or whey or pea proteins in human subjects)(Reference Lang, Bellisle and Alamowitch30, Reference Lang, Bellisle and Oppert31). The results observed with biochemically very different proteins strongly suggest that the satiating effect of protein is primarily a very general property that does not depend on such specific characteristics of one or another protein(Reference Bensaid, Tome and Gietzen3). Nevertheless, if protein type can influence the satiating effect, the mechanisms involved remain unclear. For instance, it could be related to a slowdown of the gastric emptying, an increase in brain amino acids, the presence of specific peptides or the presence of certain specific amino acids (see below). However, in such studies the dietary protein used to formulate the food has to be well equilibrated in its essential amino acid composition. Otherwise, eating a protein- or amino acid-imbalanced diet induces a depression in food intake induced by a conditioned food aversion(Reference Fromentin, Feurte and Nicolaidis32, Reference Gietzen, Hao and Anthony33).

Protein diets

The consumption of a HP diet quickly induces a strong and immediate depression in food intake followed by a progressive but not complete return to the level of energy intake of the control diet in animals(Reference Jean, Rome and Mathe1). Harper & Peters studied this phenomenon in rats by using diets whose protein content ranged from 5 to 75 %(Reference Harper and Peters34). The reduction of food intake occurred when the protein content of the diet was greater than 40 % (as protein:energy ratio) and was even more pronounced as it rose higher.

Taking into consideration the depression in food intake and the induced active avoidance, it has been claimed that eating a HP diet probably induces a conditioned food aversion(Reference Tews, Repa and Harper35). The respective roles of conditioned food aversion, satiety and palatability in the depression of food intake induced by a HP diet were studied by analysing the behavioural responses in comparison with a normal-protein (NP) diet(Reference Bensaid, Tome and L'Heureux-Bourdon36Reference Ropelle, Pauli and Fernandes39). Conditioned food aversion is an acquired mechanism allowing a subject to avoid consuming a food when the post-ingestive consequences are remembered as harmful. Conditioned food aversion requires that the subject links one or more oro-sensory characteristics of the food to unpleasant post-ingestive consequences. Our own experiments(Reference Bensaid, Tome and L'Heureux-Bourdon36, Reference L'Heureux-Bouron, Tome and Bensaid37) showed that only behavioural and food intake parameters were disturbed during day 1 when an animal ate the HP diet, and that most parameters returned to baseline values as soon as day 2 of the HP diet. Rats adapted to the HP diet did not acquire a conditioned food aversion but exhibited satiety, and a normal behavioural satiety sequence. Similar to ours, other studies(Reference Kinzig, Hargrave and Hyun38, Reference Ropelle, Pauli and Fernandes39) did not confirm a conditioned food aversion hypothesis.

Also, it has been shown that different protein sources may differently affect food intake in a context of HP diets in rats(Reference Pichon, Potier and Tome40). Whatever the protein type used in the HP diet, these diets decrease average energy intake more than the NP diets. This effect is modulated by the ratio between carbohydrate and fat and by the protein type (total milk protein, whey protein or β-lactoglobulin)(Reference Pichon, Potier and Tome40). For example, whey-derived protein sources, and particularly β-lactoglobulin, reduce food intake, body-weight gain and the adiposity index more than total milk protein. However, we also observed that biochemically very different proteins such as total milk and soya protein induced no differences in depression of energy intake(Reference Pichon41). As in the case of protein snacks, this result suggests that the satiating effect of protein used in a HP diet is primarily a very general property that does not depend on specific characteristics of one or another protein, which in contrast could explain the modulation of food intake observed when different types of proteins are used.

It has been suggested that the sensing of protein ingestion by animals might be linked to the l-glutamate (free+protein bound) content in foods(Reference Kondoh, Mallick and Torii42). This might provide a reasonable index of protein ingestion because l-glutamate is the most abundant amino acid in almost all dietary proteins. Nevertheless, 15 d of eating a NP diet (total milk protein, protein:energy of 14 %) enriched or not in glutamate (2 %) did not induce a decrease in food intake or in weight gain(Reference Boutry, Bos and Matsumoto43). Glutamic acid is well tolerated by the rat in amounts as high as 5 %, and probably up to 10 %, in diets with low to moderate protein content(Reference Harper, Benevenga and Wohlhueter44).

The poor palatability of HP diets has been documented(Reference McArthur, Kelly and Gietzen45), but with respect to protein intake its relative importance remains unclear. It is possible that the appetite-suppressing effect of dietary protein is partially induced by poor palatability. In order to study the role of oro-sensorial factors, food intake was measured after modifying the composition of the HP diet, meaning the type of proteins or carbohydrates present(Reference L'Heureux-Bouron, Tome and Bensaid46). However, we were unable to modify the depression of food intake induced by a HP diet. Taken together, our experiments indicate that the overall behavioural response more probably originated from an initial lower palatability of the food combined with an enhanced satiety effect of the HP diet and a delay required for the metabolic adaptation.

Even if several studies, conducted in human subjects fed low-energy diets, have shown that using one or two HP meal replacements per d might lead to a decrease in energy intake and body weight(Reference Ashley, St Jeor and Perumean-Chaney47, Reference Noakes, Foster and Keogh48), the long-term consequences of the consumption of protein meals in non-restricted subjects remain unknown.

The usefulness of protein diets may be questioned in relation to weight loss and maintenance of lean body mass in two different situations: that of energy restriction and that of ad libitum feeding. The effect of diet composition on weight loss during energy restriction has been widely studied and the additional effect of macronutrient composition above energy restriction was not clearly demonstrated. The absence of any effect of diet composition in human subjects (apart from energy restriction), such as the protein:carbohydrate ratio, has been reported by several authors(Reference Sacks, Bray and Carey49, Reference Gilbert, Bendsen and Tremblay50), although not all(Reference Treyzon, Chen and Hong51, Reference Layman, Evans and Erickson52). The loss of lean body mass accompanying energy restriction is a major obstacle to successful long-term dieting. One important suggestion relative to increasing dietary protein during energy restriction is that it might prevent a loss of lean body mass(Reference Chaston, Dixon and O'Brien53). In rats, eating a HP diet ad libitum mainly induced a reduction in the fat mass of rats but also a higher ratio of lean:fat mass(Reference Jean, Rome and Mathe1). In human subjects only a few studies have been carried out in order to analyse the long-term effects on body-weight gain and adiposity of eating a diet rich in protein ad libitum. Using two isoenergetic diets in overweight subjects, Weigle et al. (Reference Weigle, Breen and Matthys54) showed that an increase in dietary protein from 15 to 30 % of energy at a constant carbohydrate intake resulted in rapid losses of weight and body fat due to a sustained decrease in appetite and energy intake.

Peripheral control of amino acids and protein intake

During digestion, proteins produce several pre- and post-absorptive signals that play a role in the control of food intake (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1 Mechanisms responsible for the protein-induced suppression of food intake. BCAA, branched-chain amino acids; AMPK, AMP-activated protein kinase; mTOR, mammalian target of rapamycin; NPY, neuropeptide Y; AgRP, Agouti-related peptide; POMC, pro-opiomelanocortin; CART, cocaine and amphetamine-regulated transcript; AA, amino acids; CCK, cholecystokinin; GLP-1, glucagon-like peptide-1; PYY, peptide YY.

Detection of protein and amino acids during digestion and control of food intake by feedback signals

Amino acid detection occurs as early as within the oral cavity. Indeed, several amino acids taste sweet, bitter or umami to man and are attractive to rodents and other animals(Reference Beauchamp55). Taste receptors (namely T1R1 and T1R3 heterodimers) are present on the tongue and detect most of the twenty amino acids. Some, such as glutamate, can be detected specifically through the means of metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGluR1 and mGluR4), while others, such as glycine, taste sweet to man. There are different sources of l-glutamate in food: either protein-bound or in free form. When glutamate is protein-bound, it is tasteless and does not provide an umami taste to food. l-Glutamate is present as a free amino acid in various food products such as seaweed, soya sauce, fermented beans, an extract made from beef meat, aged cheeses, cured ham and tomatoes. Thus, it is unlikely that free glutamate is a major taste marker of dietary protein to the individual(Reference Kondoh, Mallick and Torii42, Reference Beauchamp55).

Satiation feedback signals originating from the stomach are the result of volumetric signals produced by mechanoreceptors(Reference Powley and Phillips56). The initial increase in gastric volume subsequent to the ingestion of dietary protein is probably due to increased gastric secretions and increased water intake(Reference L'Heureux-Bouron, Tome and Bensaid46). As explained below, dietary proteins and amino acids are detected within the duodenum and this detection delays gastric emptying(Reference Ma, Stevens and Cukier57), hence prolonging gastric distension satiation signals. As proposed by Janssen et al. (Reference Janssen, Vanden Berghe and Verschueren58), gastric emptying is a key mediator of hunger satiation and satiety. Thus, signals originating from the stomach are likely to contribute to satiation signalling provoked by protein intake.

A strong contributor to the effect of protein and amino acids on food intake is the upper intestine. There is evidence that protein and protein digestion products, such as amino acids and oligopeptides, are detected within the lumen of the duodenum. While a first proposed mechanism for this detection is linked to protein and amino acid absorption and processing by enterocytes(Reference Raybould, Glatzle and Freeman59), other groups have shown that nutrient-specific receptors exist on the apical side of enterocyte and enteroendocrine cells, being similar to either lingual taste receptors(Reference Lindemann60) or functional oligopeptide transporters(Reference Liou, Chavez and Espero61, Reference Darcel, Liou and Tome62). Ultimately, amino acid and oligopeptide detection in the intestinal wall is dependent upon the release of cholecystokinin (CCK) by enteroendocrine cells(Reference Conigrave, Quinn and Brown63). Duodenal CCK then increases the firing rate of vagus nerve afferents that extend terminals to the close vicinity of the brush border, and that convey information to the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS) in the brainstem. The implication of this detection on food intake seems to be restricted to short-term food intake control(Reference Raybould64).

Within the lower intestine, the ileal brake is a feedback mechanism that results in the inhibition of proximal gastrointestinal motility and secretion. Animal and human studies have shown that activation of it by local nutrient perfusions increases feelings of satiety and reduces ad libitum food intake(Reference Maljaars, Peters and Mela65). These results point to a potential role for the ileal brake in the regulation of digestion, exerting a direct or indirect impact on eating behaviour and satiety. Ileal protein infusions in both human subjects and animals activate the ileal brake(Reference Meyer, Hlinka and Tabrizi66). It is notably mediated by peptide YY (PYY), which is released by L cells located in the mucosa of the ileum. According to Moran & Dailey(Reference Moran and Dailey67), the pattern of secretion in plasma can remain elevated for up to 6 h following meal termination. This long-lasting pattern of release suggests roles for this mediator that extend beyond the meal that originally stimulated its release. According to Batterham et al. (Reference Batterham, Heffron and Kapoor68), a HP diet intake induced the greatest release of PYY and the most pronounced satiety in normal-weight and obese human subjects. A long-term augmentation of dietary protein in mice induced elevated plasma PYY levels, and reduced food intake and adiposity. In order to determine directly the role of PYY in mediating the satiating effects of protein, Batterham et al. (Reference Batterham, Heffron and Kapoor68) generated PYY-null mice that were selectively resistant to the satiating and weight-reducing effects of protein; these animals developed marked obesity that was reversed by exogenous PYY treatment.

In addition to CCK and PYY, a wide range of intestinal mediators have been described to be linked to dietary protein intake, as reviewed by Karhunen et al. (Reference Karhunen, Juvonen and Huotari69). Ghrelin, an orexigenic peptide, is decreased after consumption of a HP breakfast in lean subjects(Reference Blom, Lluch and Stafleu70). This result was confirmed by Bowen et al. (Reference Bowen, Noakes and Clifton71) in lean and overweight subjects but not by Westerterp's studies (reviewed in Veldhorst et al. (Reference Veldhorst, Smeets and Soenen72)). It is also possible that HP diet but not HP meal intake influences ghrelin responses(Reference Smeets, Soenen and Luscombe-Marsh73). Also, compared with other macronutrients, dietary protein is also a very strong stimulus for gastric inhibitory polypeptide(Reference Hall, Millward and Long74) and glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1) release by the small intestine(Reference Bowen, Noakes and Clifton71). Since GLP-1 receptors are expressed in vagal afferent neurons it is likely that GLP-1 acts on vagal afferent terminals in close vicinity to the enteroendocrine L cells(Reference Moran and Dailey67). Complex cooperative interplay might occur between ghrelin and CCK as described by de Lartigue et al. (Reference de Lartigue, Lur and Dimaline75), and also between CCK, gastric inhibitory polypeptide and GLP-1, suggesting critical synergistic effects(Reference Rehfeld76).

The participation of hepatic portal vein vagal afferents in protein sensing and signalling to the brain is notably supported by electrophysiological recordings showing that hepatic portal vein perfusion of amino acids activates vagal afferent fibres(Reference Darcel, Liou and Tome62). However, hepatic vagal afferents do not seem essential to the peripheral detection of a HP diet(Reference Tome, Schwarz and Darcel6). Others have proposed the hepatic portal vein as being the critical and necessary site for HP diets to alter food intake(Reference Penhoat, Mutel and Correig77), but this view seems rather unlikely considering the high level of redundancy present in gut–brain axis satiety signalling(Reference Berthoud, Shin and Zheng78).

Such generated signals (gut peptides but also circulating amino acids) have a dual mode of action on the central nervous system: (i) at the gastrointestinal tract via the vagus nerve to the brainstem within the NTS (the first central relay for afferent vagal fibres); and (ii) directly at the hypothalamus (arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus; ARC) and brainstem via the bloodstream (notably through hormones and circulating nutrients acting as mediators) and an indirect transmission through the nervous system which innervates the gastrointestinal tract, namely the vagus nerve and splanchnic nerves (though sub-diaphragmatic vagotomy fails to suppress the depression of food intake induced by the administration of a HP diet in rats(Reference L'Heureux-Bouron, Tome and Rampin79)).

Detection of protein and amino acids, post-absorptive signals and feedback signals controlling food intake

Since the 2000s, many studies in human subjects have been conducted to examine differences in postprandial hormone profiles that could be the cause of satiety induced by proteins. Studies have focused on CCK, GLP-1, amylin, ghrelin, leptin, insulin and glucagon, but very often no clear correlation emerged between these hormones and satiety(Reference Bowen, Noakes and Clifton71, Reference Veldhorst, Smeets and Soenen72). As for individual mediators, studies have shown that ghrelin is primarily blood-borne and alters hypothalamic function, whereas the mode of action of CCK and GLP-1 is, in contrast, primarily vagally mediated(Reference Moran and Dailey67). Plasma CCK levels are unlikely to be a relevant signal(Reference Brenner, Yox and Ritter80).

It must be mentioned that an increase in energy expenditure could also be the cause of peripheral signals of satiety induced by proteins. The role of dietary protein in body-weight control could be a direct consequence of thermogenesis. Proteins are thought to produce a greater thermogenic effect than other macronutrients participating in protein-induced satiety. This is due in part to the energetic cost necessary to incorporate each amino acid into protein and for catabolism of the amino acid in excess. In human subjects(Reference Latner and Schwartz4, Reference Westerterp-Plantenga, Nieuwenhuizen and Tome5) but not in rats(Reference Stepien, Gaudichon and Azzout-Marniche81), ingestion of proteins stimulates postprandial thermogenesis at a higher level than other macronutrients. Conversely, it has never been shown that an increase in postprandial thermogenesis was the direct cause of satiety induced by the ingestion of food proteins. At most, correlations between dietary protein intakes, an effect on satiety and increased thermogenesis were highlighted. Furthermore, central mechanisms linking the direct effect of increased thermogenesis and postprandial satiety remain to be demonstrated.

Amino acid-induced gluconeogenesis could also prevent a decrease in glycaemia that could contribute to satiety(Reference Veldhorst, Westerterp-Plantenga and Westerterp82). The fact that gluconeogenesis in man is thought to remain relatively stable in varying metabolic conditions is still a matter of debate(Reference Nuttall, Ngo and Gannon83). However, Velhorst et al. (Reference Veldhorst, Westerterp and Westerterp-Plantenga84) has shown that after a HP diet, gluconeogenesis was increased and appetite was lower compared with a NP diet; however, these were unrelated to each other. The glucostatic theory indicates that a drop of plasma glucose level precedes the start of the following meal. However, no one has shown that the flow of plasma glucose induced by the ingestion of a meal rich in protein lasts longer than one that follows a NP meal(Reference Marmonier, Chapelot and Fantino15).

The high plasma concentration of amino acids after the ingestion of a protein diet could be the cause of peripheral signals that would be detected in some specific regions in the hypothalamus(Reference Choi, Fletcher and Anderson85). Hall et al. (Reference Hall, Millward and Long74) explained that their results on the satiating effect of whey protein being greater than that of casein were in agreement with the work of Boirie et al. (Reference Boirie, Dangin and Gachon86), originally the conceiver of ‘slow and fast proteins’, and the aminostatic theory(Reference Mellinkoff, Frankland and Boyle87). These authors concluded that the massive influx of amino acids following ingestion of whey proteins could lead to higher satiating power of these proteins as compared with other, slower, sources of amino acids (such as casein). Finally, the presence of some specific amino acids such as glutamate or leucine could also play a role in the central control of appetite (see below).

The general hypothesis that the specific role of certain amino acid precursors of neurotransmitters would explain the decrease in food intake induced by proteins is still a matter of debate. In particular, the amount of tryptophan (the precursor of serotonin and a mediator known to inhibit appetite(Reference Blundell and Latham88)) was used to demonstrate this phenomenon. The high tryptophan content of α-lactalbumin has been suggested to explain the satiating effect of this protein(Reference Veldhorst, Nieuwenhuizen and Hochstenbach-Waelen29, Reference Nieuwenhuizen, Hochstenbach-Waelen and Veldhorst89). Indeed, the ingestion of α-lactalbumin increases the plasma level of tryptophan and the tryptophan:branched-chain amino acids ratio more than other proteins such as gelatin (which is devoid of tryptophan). However, the addition of free tryptophan to gelatin at the same level as that of α-lactalbumin does not affect responses on scales of hunger(Reference Veldhorst, Nieuwenhuizen and Hochstenbach-Waelen29). Similarly, tyrosine, a precursor of dopamine, added at a 5 % level in the diet of rats had no influence on the level of intake(Reference Bassil, Hwalla and Obeid90). Because the ingestion of foods high in protein (and thus l-glutamate) does not lead to appreciable changes in plasma l-glutamate concentrations(Reference Kondoh, Tsurugizawa and Torii91), the body (the brain) is unlikely to monitor protein intake via meal- or diet-related variations in plasma l-glutamate. In the same way, results concerning the addition of histidine, a precursor of histamine, to the diet are conflicting. Chronic ingestion of histidine added at a 5 % level in the diet of rats had no influence on the level of intake(Reference Bassil, Hwalla and Obeid90). At the same time a 8 d ingestion of histidine added at a 1, 2·5 or 5 % level in the diet of rats decreased food intake(Reference Kasaoka, Tsuboyama-Kasaoka and Kawahara92), so the role of central histamine in the depression of food intake has been suggested(Reference Mercer93).

Protein-induced reduction in eating and central neuronal pathways

It should be mentioned that brain recognition of deficiency in indispensable amino acid diets is out of the scope of the present review(Reference Fromentin, Feurte and Nicolaidis32, Reference Gietzen, Hao and Anthony33). Well-equilibrated dietary proteins induce a reduction in food intake during a subsequent meal in parallel with activation of neuronal populations in the NTS and the hypothalamus. In addition, a HP diet inhibits the activation of opioid and GABAergic neurons in the nucleus accumbens, and thus inhibits food intake by reducing the hedonic response to food, presumably because of its low palatability (Fig. 1).

The NTS is the main entry point of the vagus nerve in the central nervous system and thus receives afferent projections from most of the organs of the gastrointestinal tract(Reference Berthoud94). In addition, the NTS receives some cranial nerve afferents that convey, from the oro-sensory area, extensive information on food texture, taste, smell, appearance and palatability.

The involvement of vagal afferent pathways in protein sensing and signalling to the brain is supported by results showing that intraduodenal protein activates vagal afferent fibres, and HP feeding induces c-Fos expression in neurons within the NTS(Reference Darcel, Fromentin and Raybould95, Reference Phifer and Berthoud96). Faipoux et al. (Reference Faipoux, Tome and Gougis97) showed that a reduction in food intake after a HP load (v. a NP load) resulted from activation of the noradrenergic neurons related to CCK-induced anorexia. This study also showed that neurons expressing GLP-1 were not activated, which is consistent with the fact that protein-induced reduction in eating is not associated with conditioned food aversion(Reference Bensaid, Tome and L'Heureux-Bourdon36).

The hypothalamus is the focus of much peripheral information sensing and participates in the control of body energy homeostasis and food intake. This vast area has many nuclei that are in constant interaction(Reference Berthoud94). The hypothalamus contains a number of discrete neuronal populations or nuclei, including the ARC, the paraventricular nucleus (PVN), the ventromedial nucleus, the dorsomedial nucleus and the lateral hypothalamic area. Energy homeostasis-regulating circuits are found within and connecting these nuclei. In the ARC, pro-opiomelanocortin (POMC) neuron activation induces a reduction in food intake. The activation of POMC neurons is also inseparable from the behaviour of another population in the ARC, neuropeptide Y (NPY)/Agouti-related peptide (AgRP) neurons, activation of which is potent in increasing food intake and inhibiting POMC neuron activation(Reference Cowley, Smart and Rubinstein98).

We have shown that after the ingestion of a HP meal, the numbers of double-labelled Fos and α-melanocyte-stimulating hormone (a marker of POMC neuron activation) marked cells increased, concomitantly with a reduction in the activation of non-POMC neurons(Reference Faipoux, Tome and Gougis97). This result was less pronounced when a HP diet had been served chronically (21 d) than in an acute setting. Moreover, because arcuate neurons mainly exhibit a POMC or NPY phenotype, it could be hypothesised that NPY neurons are less strongly activated after HP meals. Similarly, rats or obese mice fed a HP diet ad libitum decreased their hypothalamic NPY mRNA levels and increased their hypothalamic mRNA POMC levels to a greater extent than those fed the NP diet(Reference Ropelle, Pauli and Fernandes39). Using Sprague–Dawley rats maintained on a HP–high-fat diet also resulted in significant increases in POMC expression over that of controls, with no effect on AgRP or NPY expression levels in the ARC, and increased dorsomedial nucleus NPY expression(Reference Kinzig, Hargrave and Hyun38). Surprisingly, a recent study reported that in wild mice fed a HP diet for 3 d (or more until 30 d) there was a paradoxical decrease in expression of the hypothalamic POMC, and an increase in expression of the gene encoding AgRP(Reference Pillot, Duraffourd and Begeot99). However, they did not discuss their results with respect to those previously presented in this review.

It has been shown that AMP-activated protein kinase (AMPK) and the mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR) are involved in the reduction in eating induced by HP diets. Here, an increase in dietary leucine(Reference Ropelle, Pauli and Fernandes39) or the intra-cerebroventricular administration of amino acids (or leucine only)(Reference Cota, Proulx and Smith100, Reference Morrison, Xi and White101) reduces food intake and body weight. AMPK is the downstream component of a kinase cascade that acts as a sensor for cellular energy charge, being activated by an increase in the AMP:ATP ratio. Once activated, AMPK phosphorylates acetyl-CoA carboxylase (ACC) and switches on energy-producing pathways at the expense of energy-depleting processes. Ropelle et al. (Reference Ropelle, Pauli and Fernandes39) showed that both a HP diet and intra-cerebroventricular leucine administration suppressed AMPK and ACC phosphorylation in the rat hypothalamus, this being concomitant with a reduced AMP:ATP ratio. In parallel, there has been growing interest in mTOR, an intracellular signalling molecule sensitive to both amino acids and growth factors, which is also described as a metabolic sensor. Both a HP diet and the intra-cerebroventricular administration of free amino acids, or leucine only, led to mTOR activation in the hypothalamus(Reference Cota, Proulx and Smith100, Reference Morrison, Xi and White101). Moreover, HP diets modulate AMPK and mTOR in the same specific neuronal subsets, the ARC and paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus. AMPK and mTOR may have overlapping and reciprocal functions(Reference Cota, Proulx and Smith100, Reference Morrison, Xi and White101). Finally, the activation of mTOR and the suppression of AMPK phosphorylation activity seem to modulate hypothalamic neuropeptides: they reduce levels of orexigenic NPY and AgRP neuropeptides and increase the expression of POMC, which exerts an anorexigenic effect(Reference Ropelle, Pauli and Fernandes39, Reference Morrison, Xi and White101).

This effect of HP diets seems to be leucine-specific, because intracerebral leucine alone exerts the same effect on food intake as a mixture of amino acids(Reference Ropelle, Pauli and Fernandes39). Moreover, using a HP diet or a NP diet enriched in leucine to the same level found in a HP diet (containing 50 g free leucine per kg diet) Ropelle et al. (Reference Ropelle, Pauli and Fernandes39) obtained the same results without causing a conditioned taste aversion. Nevertheless, such a comparison has some limits because a NP diet containing an amino acid in excess and a HP diet have different types of peripheral catabolism(Reference Harper, Benevenga and Wohlhueter44).

The nucleus accumbens (NAcc) is a structure of the ventral striatum which is pivotal in the modulation of hedonic control of eating behaviour. It is divided into two parts, the core (AccCo) and shell (AccSh)(Reference Berthoud94). It was identified early on as the primary interface between motivation and action in the brain, because of its many afferents (mostly glutamatergic) from regions involved in cognitive processes and learning, and its efferents to areas of motor control, mostly GABAergic. The AccCo is involved in the learning process and implementation of adaptive mechanical actions, while AccSh is more involved as a relay between cortical regions and other regions of the brain in behavioural aspects, in particular food(Reference Kelley102). As previously discussed, dietary proteins have a low palatability. Palatability has a major impact on the reward system but the expression of liking cannot alone explain the anorexigenic effect of proteins(Reference Bensaid, Tome and L'Heureux-Bourdon36). Proteins inhibit the activation of opioid and GABAergic neurons in the NAcc and thus could inhibit food intake by reducing the hedonic response to food(Reference Faipoux103). It is well known that neurons in the AccSh project directly to the lateral hypothalamic area(Reference Kirouac and Ganguly104) and the stimulation of orexigenic lateral hypothalamic area neurons can induce robust feeding(Reference Stratford and Kelley105). Chemical manipulation of the AccSh has been shown to elicit robust feeding and Fos expression in the hypothalamus, in particular in the ARC. Fos activation was significantly lower in POMC/cocaine and amphetamine-regulated transcript (CART) neurons and higher in NPY neurons(Reference Zheng, Corkern and Stoyanova106). Recently we have shown that postprandial activation of NAcc is decreased by a HP diet given during 2 d or after a 15 d period(Reference Chaumontet, Darcel and Tomé107) and could in turn inhibit NPY neurons and activate POMC/CART neurons in ARC.

Conclusion

Extensive studies of HP diets have demonstrated their ability to limit total energy intake, body weight and lipid deposition. They have become popular, probably because they increase satiety and aid in ensuring better compliance with a reduced-energy diet and/or contribute to a spontaneous reduction in energy intake(Reference Anderson and Moore108).

Nowadays we know that the protein and amino acid content of food is a determinant of control of food intake and the amount that will be eaten. The peripheral hormones CCK, GLP-1 and PYY have been shown to be involved in the mechanism of protein-induced satiety. Specific amino acids such as the branched-chain amino acid leucine, or other neuropeptide precursors (perhaps histidine), can contribute to the protein-induced control of food intake via central mechanisms. Several brain regions are involved in the central control of food intake, and more specifically it has been shown that proteins may affect them by acting in different parts of the brainstem or hypothalamus.

In the future we will have to study more precisely the interaction between different brain areas, and the adaptation of different neural circuits following the ingestion of protein diets. Also, when eating a HP diet, a fraction of dietary protein and endogenous protein escapes digestion in the small intestine and passes the ileo-caecal junction where it undergoes the action of bacterial proteases and the residual fraction of pancreatic proteases. Free amino acids are substrates used by the microbiota to generate metabolites of diverse structure, some of which have effects on cells. The role of dietary protein on the control of food intake via the microbiota remains unknown.

Acknowledgements

The present review was supported by funds from our laboratory. We acknowledge the contribution of all authors in the writing of the present review and G. F. and D. T. for conceiving the article.

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

References

1Jean, C, Rome, S, Mathe, V, et al. . (2001) Metabolic evidence for adaptation to a high protein diet in rats. J Nutr 131, 9198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
2Poppitt, SD, McCormack, D & Buffenstein, R (1998) Short-term effects of macronutrient preloads on appetite and energy intake in lean women. Physiol Behav 64, 279285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3Bensaid, A, Tome, D, Gietzen, D, et al. . (2002) Protein is more potent than carbohydrate for reducing appetite in rats. Physiol Behav 75, 577582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4Latner, JD & Schwartz, M (1999) The effects of a high-carbohydrate, high-protein or balanced lunch upon later food intake and hunger ratings. Appetite 33, 119128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5Westerterp-Plantenga, MS, Nieuwenhuizen, A, Tome, D, et al. . (2009) Dietary protein, weight loss, and weight maintenance. Annu Rev Nutr 29, 2141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6Tome, D, Schwarz, J, Darcel, N, et al. . (2009) Protein, amino acids, vagus nerve signaling, and the brain. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 838S843S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7Schwartz, MW, Woods, SC, Porte, D Jr, et al. . (2000) Central nervous system control of food intake. Nature 404, 661671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8Berthoud, HR (2011) Metabolic and hedonic drives in the neural control of appetite: who is the boss? Curr Opin Neurobiol 21, 888896.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9Trigazis, L, Orttmann, A & Anderson, GH (1997) Effect of a cholecystokinin-A receptor blocker on protein-induced food intake suppression in rats. Am J Physiol 272, R1826R1833.Google ScholarPubMed
10Porrini, M, Santangelo, A, Crovetti, R, et al. . (1997) Weight, protein, fat, and timing of preloads affect food intake. Physiol Behav 62, 563570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11Marmonier, C, Chapelot, D & Louis-Sylvestre, J (2000) Effects of macronutrient content and energy density of snacks consumed in a satiety state on the onset of the next meal. Appetite 34, 161168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12Burton-Freeman, B, Gietzen, DW & Schneeman, BO (1997) Meal pattern analysis to investigate the satiating potential of fat, carbohydrate, and protein in rats. Am J Physiol 273, R1916R1922.Google ScholarPubMed
13Bertenshaw, EJ, Lluch, A & Yeomans, MR (2008) Satiating effects of protein but not carbohydrate consumed in a between-meal beverage context. Physiol Behav 93, 427436.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14Geliebter, A, Liang, JT & Van Itallie, TB (1984) Effects of repeated isocaloric macronutrient loads on daily food intake of rats. Am J Physiol 247, R387R392.Google ScholarPubMed
15Marmonier, C, Chapelot, D, Fantino, M, et al. . (2002) Snacks consumed in a nonhungry state have poor satiating efficiency: influence of snack composition on substrate utilization and hunger. Am J Clin Nutr 76, 518528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
16Raben, A, Agerholm-Larsen, L, Flint, A, et al. . (2003) Meals with similar energy densities but rich in protein, fat, carbohydrate, or alcohol have different effects on energy expenditure and substrate metabolism but not on appetite and energy intake. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 91100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
17Reid, M & Hetherington, M (1997) Relative effects of carbohydrates and protein on satiety – a review of methodology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 21, 295308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
18Mattes, RD (2006) Fluid energy – where's the problem? J Am Diet Assoc 106, 19561961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19Leidy, HJ, Bales-Voelker, LI & Harris, CT (2011) A protein-rich beverage consumed as a breakfast meal leads to weaker appetitive and dietary responses v. a protein-rich solid breakfast meal in adolescents. Br J Nutr 106, 3741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
20Martens, MJ, Lemmens, SG, Born, JM, et al. . (2011) A solid high-protein meal evokes stronger hunger suppression than a liquefied high-protein meal. Obesity (Silver Spring) 19, 522527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21Akhavan, T, Luhovyy, BL & Anderson, GH (2011) Effect of drinking compared with eating sugars or whey protein on short-term appetite and food intake. Int J Obes (Lond) 35, 562569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22Potier, M, Fromentin, G, Lesdema, A, et al. . (2010) The satiety effect of disguised liquid preloads administered acutely and differing only in their nutrient content tended to be weaker for lipids but did not differ between proteins and carbohydrates in human subjects. Br J Nutr 104, 14061414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23Fromentin, G, Feurte, S & Nicolaidis, S (1998) Spatial cues are relevant for learned preference/aversion shifts due to amino-acid deficiencies. Appetite 30, 223234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24Booth, DA, Lee, M & McAleavey, C (1976) Acquired sensory control of satiation in man. Br J Psychol 67, 137147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25Louis-Sylvestre, J, Tournier, A, Verger, P, et al. . (1989) Learned caloric adjustment of human intake. Appetite 12, 95103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26Yeomans, MR, Weinberg, L & James, S (2005) Effects of palatability and learned satiety on energy density influences on breakfast intake in humans. Physiol Behav 86, 487499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27Faipoux, R, Tome, D, Bensaid, A, et al. . (2006) Yeast proteins enhance satiety in rats. J Nutr 136, 23502356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28Anderson, GH, Tecimer, SN, Shah, D, et al. . (2004) Protein source, quantity, and time of consumption determine the effect of proteins on short-term food intake in young men. J Nutr 134, 30113015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29Veldhorst, MA, Nieuwenhuizen, AG, Hochstenbach-Waelen, A, et al. . (2009) A breakfast with α-lactalbumin, gelatin, or gelatin + TRP lowers energy intake at lunch compared with a breakfast with casein, soy, whey, or whey-GMP. Clin Nutr 28, 147155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
30Lang, V, Bellisle, F, Alamowitch, C, et al. . (1999) Varying the protein source in mixed meal modifies glucose, insulin and glucagon kinetics in healthy men, has weak effects on subjective satiety and fails to affect food intake. Eur J Clin Nutr 53, 959965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31Lang, V, Bellisle, F, Oppert, JM, et al. . (1998) Satiating effect of proteins in healthy subjects: a comparison of egg albumin, casein, gelatin, soy protein, pea protein, and wheat gluten. Am J Clin Nutr 67, 11971204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32Fromentin, G, Feurte, S, Nicolaidis, S, et al. . (2000) Parabrachial lesions disrupt responses of rats to amino acid devoid diets, to protein-free diets, but not to high-protein diets. Physiol Behav 70, 381389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33Gietzen, DW, Hao, S & Anthony, TG (2007) Mechanisms of food intake repression in indispensable amino acid deficiency. Annu Rev Nutr 27, 6378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
34Harper, AE & Peters, JC (1989) Protein intake, brain amino acid and serotonin concentrations and protein self-selection. J Nutr 119, 677689.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35Tews, JK, Repa, JJ & Harper, AE (1992) Protein selection by rats adapted to high or moderately low levels of dietary protein. Physiol Behav 51, 699712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36Bensaid, A, Tome, D, L'Heureux-Bourdon, D, et al. . (2003) A high-protein diet enhances satiety without conditioned taste aversion in the rat. Physiol Behav 78, 311320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37L'Heureux-Bouron, D, Tome, D, Bensaid, A, et al. . (2004) A very high 70 %-protein diet does not induce conditioned taste aversion in rats. J Nutr 134, 15121515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
38Kinzig, KP, Hargrave, SL, Hyun, J, et al. . (2007) Energy balance and hypothalamic effects of a high-protein/low-carbohydrate diet. Physiol Behav 92, 454460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
39Ropelle, ER, Pauli, JR, Fernandes, MF, et al. . (2008) A central role for neuronal AMP-activated protein kinase (AMPK) and mammalian target of rapamycin (mTOR) in high-protein diet-induced weight loss. Diabetes 57, 594605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
40Pichon, L, Potier, M, Tome, D, et al. . (2008) High-protein diets containing different milk protein fractions differently influence energy intake and adiposity in the rat. Br J Nutr 99, 739748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
41Pichon, L (2008) Rôles de la composition en macronutriments et de la nature protéique du régime dans le comportement alimentaire et le métabolisme protéino-énergétique chez le rat (Roles of macronutrient composition and protein nature of the regime in feeding behavior and protein–energy metabolism in rats). Thesis Abies. Paris: Agroparistech.Google Scholar
42Kondoh, T, Mallick, HN & Torii, K (2009) Activation of the gut–brain axis by dietary glutamate and physiologic significance in energy homeostasis. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 832S837S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43Boutry, C, Bos, C, Matsumoto, H, et al. . (2011) Effects of monosodium glutamate supplementation on glutamine metabolism in adult rats. Front Biosci (Elite Ed) 3, 279290.Google ScholarPubMed
44Harper, AE, Benevenga, NJ & Wohlhueter, RM (1970) Effects of ingestion of disproportionate amounts of amino acids. Physiol Rev 50, 428558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45McArthur, LH, Kelly, WF, Gietzen, DW, et al. . (1993) The role of palatability in the food intake response of rats fed high-protein diets. Appetite 20, 181196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46L'Heureux-Bouron, D, Tome, D, Bensaid, A, et al. . (2004) Preabsorptive factors are not the main determinants of intake depression induced by a high-protein diet in the rat. Physiol Behav 81, 499504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
47Ashley, JM, St Jeor, ST, Perumean-Chaney, S, et al. . (2001) Meal replacements in weight intervention. Obes Res 9, Suppl. 4, 312S320S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
48Noakes, M, Foster, PR, Keogh, JB, et al. . (2004) Meal replacements are as effective as structured weight-loss diets for treating obesity in adults with features of metabolic syndrome. J Nutr 134, 18941899.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
49Sacks, FM, Bray, GA, Carey, VJ, et al. . (2009) Comparison of weight-loss diets with different compositions of fat, protein, and carbohydrates. N Engl J Med 360, 859873.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50Gilbert, JA, Bendsen, NT, Tremblay, A, et al. . (2011) Effect of proteins from different sources on body composition. Nutr Metab Cardiovasc Dis 21, Suppl. 2, B16B31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
51Treyzon, L, Chen, S, Hong, K, et al. . (2008) A controlled trial of protein enrichment of meal replacements for weight reduction with retention of lean body mass. Nutr J 7, 23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
52Layman, DK, Evans, EM, Erickson, D, et al. . (2009) A moderate-protein diet produces sustained weight loss and long-term changes in body composition and blood lipids in obese adults. J Nutr 139, 514521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
53Chaston, TB, Dixon, JB & O'Brien, PE (2007) Changes in fat-free mass during significant weight loss: a systematic review. Int J Obes (Lond) 31, 743750.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
54Weigle, DS, Breen, PA, Matthys, CC, et al. . (2005) A high-protein diet induces sustained reductions in appetite, ad libitum caloric intake, and body weight despite compensatory changes in diurnal plasma leptin and ghrelin concentrations. Am J Clin Nutr 82, 4148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
55Beauchamp, GK (2009) Sensory and receptor responses to umami: an overview of pioneering work. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 723S727S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
56Powley, TL & Phillips, RJ (2004) Gastric satiation is volumetric, intestinal satiation is nutritive. Physiol Behav 82, 6974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
57Ma, J, Stevens, JE, Cukier, K, et al. . (2009) Effects of a protein preload on gastric emptying, glycemia, and gut hormones after a carbohydrate meal in diet-controlled type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 32, 16001602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
58Janssen, P, Vanden Berghe, P, Verschueren, S, et al. . (2011) Review Article: the role of gastric motility in the control of food intake. Aliment Pharmacol Ther 33, 880894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
59Raybould, HE, Glatzle, J, Freeman, SL, et al. . (2006) Detection of macronutrients in the intestinal wall. Auton Neurosci 125, 2833.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
60Lindemann, B (2001) Receptors and transduction in taste. Nature 413, 219225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
61Liou, AP, Chavez, DI, Espero, E, et al. . (2011) Protein hydrolysate-induced cholecystokinin secretion from enteroendocrine cells is indirectly mediated by the intestinal oligopeptide transporter PepT1. Am J Physiol Gastrointest Liver Physiol 300, G895G902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
62Darcel, NP, Liou, AP, Tome, D, et al. . (2005) Activation of vagal afferents in the rat duodenum by protein digests requires PepT1. J Nutr 135, 14911495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
63Conigrave, AD, Quinn, SJ & Brown, EM (2000) l-Amino acid sensing by the extracellular Ca2+-sensing receptor. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97, 48144819.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
64Raybould, HE (1991) Capsaicin-sensitive vagal afferents and CCK in inhibition of gastric motor function induced by intestinal nutrients. Peptides 12, 12791283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
65Maljaars, PW, Peters, HP, Mela, DJ, et al. . (2008) Ileal brake: a sensible food target for appetite control. A review. Physiol Behav 95, 271281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
66Meyer, JH, Hlinka, M, Tabrizi, Y, et al. . (1998) Chemical specificities and intestinal distributions of nutrient-driven satiety. Am J Physiol 275, R1293R1307.Google ScholarPubMed
67Moran, TH & Dailey, MJ (2011) Intestinal feedback signaling and satiety. Physiol Behav 105, 7781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
68Batterham, RL, Heffron, H, Kapoor, S, et al. . (2006) Critical role for peptide YY in protein-mediated satiation and body-weight regulation. Cell Metab 4, 223233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
69Karhunen, LJ, Juvonen, KR, Huotari, A, et al. . (2008) Effect of protein, fat, carbohydrate and fibre on gastrointestinal peptide release in humans. Regul Pept 149, 7078.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
70Blom, WA, Lluch, A, Stafleu, A, et al. . (2006) Effect of a high-protein breakfast on the postprandial ghrelin response. Am J Clin Nutr 83, 211220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
71Bowen, J, Noakes, M & Clifton, PM (2006) Appetite regulatory hormone responses to various dietary proteins differ by body mass index status despite similar reductions in ad libitum energy intake. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 91, 29132919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
72Veldhorst, M, Smeets, A, Soenen, S, et al. . (2008) Protein-induced satiety: effects and mechanisms of different proteins. Physiol Behav 94, 300307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
73Smeets, AJ, Soenen, S, Luscombe-Marsh, ND, et al. . (2008) Energy expenditure, satiety, and plasma ghrelin, glucagon-like peptide 1, and peptide tyrosine-tyrosine concentrations following a single high-protein lunch. J Nutr 138, 698702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
74Hall, WL, Millward, DJ, Long, SJ, et al. . (2003) Casein and whey exert different effects on plasma amino acid profiles, gastrointestinal hormone secretion and appetite. Br J Nutr 89, 239248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
75de Lartigue, G, Lur, G, Dimaline, R, et al. . (2010) EGR1 is a target for cooperative interactions between cholecystokinin and leptin, and inhibition by ghrelin, in vagal afferent neurons. Endocrinology 151, 35893599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
76Rehfeld, JF (2011) Incretin physiology beyond glucagon-like peptide 1 and glucose-dependent insulinotropic polypeptide: cholecystokinin and gastrin peptides. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 201, 405411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
77Penhoat, A, Mutel, E, Correig, MA, et al. . (2011) Protein-induced satiety is abolished in the absence of intestinal gluconeogenesis. Physiol Behav 105, 8993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
78Berthoud, HR, Shin, AC & Zheng, H (2011) Obesity surgery and gut–brain communication. Physiol Behav 105, 106119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
79L'Heureux-Bouron, D, Tome, D, Rampin, O, et al. . (2003) Total subdiaphragmatic vagotomy does not suppress high protein diet-induced food intake depression in rats. J Nutr 133, 26392642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
80Brenner, L, Yox, DP & Ritter, RC (1993) Suppression of sham feeding by intraintestinal nutrients is not correlated with plasma cholecystokinin elevation. Am J Physiol 264, R972R976.Google Scholar
81Stepien, M, Gaudichon, C, Azzout-Marniche, D, et al. . (2010) Postprandial nutrient partitioning but not energy expenditure is modified in growing rats during adaptation to a high-protein diet. J Nutr 140, 939945.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
82Veldhorst, MA, Westerterp-Plantenga, MS & Westerterp, KR (2009) Gluconeogenesis and energy expenditure after a high-protein, carbohydrate-free diet. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 519526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
83Nuttall, FQ, Ngo, A & Gannon, MC (2008) Regulation of hepatic glucose production and the role of gluconeogenesis in humans: is the rate of gluconeogenesis constant? Diabetes Metab Res Rev 24, 438458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
84Veldhorst, MA, Westerterp, KR & Westerterp-Plantenga, MS (2012) Gluconeogenesis and protein-induced satiety. Br J Nutr 107, 595600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
85Choi, YH, Fletcher, PJ & Anderson, GH (2001) Extracellular amino acid profiles in the paraventricular nucleus of the rat hypothalamus are influenced by diet composition. Brain Res 892, 320328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
86Boirie, Y, Dangin, M, Gachon, P, et al. . (1997) Slow and fast dietary proteins differently modulate postprandial protein accretion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94, 1493014935.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
87Mellinkoff, SM, Frankland, M, Boyle, D, et al. . (1956) Relationship between serum amino acid concentration and fluctuations in appetite. J Appl Physiol 8, 535538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
88Blundell, JE & Latham, CJ (1979) Serotonergic influences on food intake: effect of 5-hydroxytryptophan on parameters of feeding behaviour in deprived and free-feeding rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 11, 431437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
89Nieuwenhuizen, AG, Hochstenbach-Waelen, A, Veldhorst, MA, et al. . (2009) Acute effects of breakfasts containing α-lactalbumin, or gelatin with or without added tryptophan, on hunger, ‘satiety’ hormones and amino acid profiles. Br J Nutr 101, 18591866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
90Bassil, MS, Hwalla, N & Obeid, OA (2007) Meal pattern of male rats maintained on histidine-, leucine-, or tyrosine-supplemented diet. Obesity (Silver Spring) 15, 616623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
91Kondoh, T, Tsurugizawa, T & Torii, K (2009) Brain functional changes in rats administered with monosodium l-glutamate in the stomach. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1170, 7781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
92Kasaoka, S, Tsuboyama-Kasaoka, N, Kawahara, Y, et al. . (2004) Histidine supplementation suppresses food intake and fat accumulation in rats. Nutrition 20, 991996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
93Mercer, LP (1997) Histamine and the neuroregulation of food intake. Nutrition 13, 581582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
94Berthoud, HR (2002) Multiple neural systems controlling food intake and body weight. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26, 393428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
95Darcel, N, Fromentin, G, Raybould, HE, et al. . (2005) Fos-positive neurons are increased in the nucleus of the solitary tract and decreased in the ventromedial hypothalamus and amygdala by a high-protein diet in rats. J Nutr 135, 14861490.Google ScholarPubMed
96Phifer, CB & Berthoud, HR (1998) Duodenal nutrient infusions differentially affect sham feeding and Fos expression in rat brain stem. Am J Physiol 274, R1725R1733.Google ScholarPubMed
97Faipoux, R, Tome, D, Gougis, S, et al. . (2008) Proteins activate satiety-related neuronal pathways in the brainstem and hypothalamus of rats. J Nutr 138, 11721178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
98Cowley, MA, Smart, JL, Rubinstein, M, et al. . (2001) Leptin activates anorexigenic POMC neurons through a neural network in the arcuate nucleus. Nature 411, 480484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
99Pillot, B, Duraffourd, C, Begeot, M, et al. . (2011) Role of hypothalamic melanocortin system in adaptation of food intake to food protein increase in mice. PLoS One 6, e19107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
100Cota, D, Proulx, K, Smith, KA, et al. . (2006) Hypothalamic mTOR signaling regulates food intake. Science 312, 927930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
101Morrison, CD, Xi, X, White, CL, et al. . (2007) Amino acids inhibit Agrp gene expression via an mTOR-dependent mechanism. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab 293, E165E171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
102Kelley, AE (2004) Ventral striatal control of appetitive motivation: role in ingestive behavior and reward-related learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 27, 765776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
103Faipoux, R (2007) Caractérisation de l'effet satiétogène des protéines et mécanismes centraux impliqués; cas particulier des protéines de levures (Characterising the satiety effect of proteins and central mechanisms involved; particular case of yeast proteins). Thesis Abies. Paris Agroparistech.Google Scholar
104Kirouac, GJ & Ganguly, PK (1995) Topographical organization in the nucleus accumbens of afferents from the basolateral amygdala and efferents to the lateral hypothalamus. Neuroscience 67, 625630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
105Stratford, TR & Kelley, AE (1999) Evidence of a functional relationship between the nucleus accumbens shell and lateral hypothalamus subserving the control of feeding behavior. J Neurosci 19, 1104011048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
106Zheng, H, Corkern, M, Stoyanova, I, et al. . (2003) Peptides that regulate food intake: appetite-inducing accumbens manipulation activates hypothalamic orexin neurons and inhibits POMC neurons. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 284, R1436R1444.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
107Chaumontet, C, Darcel, N, Tomé, D, et al. . (2011) Postprandial activation of accumbens nucleus, brain area involved in hedonism, is decreased by high protein diet. FASEB J 25, 328.1.Google Scholar
108Anderson, GH & Moore, SE (2004) Dietary proteins in the regulation of food intake and body weight in humans. J Nutr 134, 974S979S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Mechanisms responsible for the protein-induced suppression of food intake. BCAA, branched-chain amino acids; AMPK, AMP-activated protein kinase; mTOR, mammalian target of rapamycin; NPY, neuropeptide Y; AgRP, Agouti-related peptide; POMC, pro-opiomelanocortin; CART, cocaine and amphetamine-regulated transcript; AA, amino acids; CCK, cholecystokinin; GLP-1, glucagon-like peptide-1; PYY, peptide YY.