Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T05:30:21.782Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Food choice and intake: towards a unifying framework of learning and feeding motivation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 December 2007

Jon E. L. Day*
Affiliation:
Institute of Food Research, Reading Laboratory, Earley Gate, Reading, RG6 6BZ, UK.
Ilias Kyriazakis
Affiliation:
Scottish Agricultural College, West Mains Road, Edinburgh, EH9 3JG, UK.
Peter J. Rogers
Affiliation:
Institute of Food Research, Reading Laboratory, Earley Gate, Reading, RG6 6BZ, UK.
*
Address to which all correspondence should be sent: Dr. J. E. L. Day, Consumer Sciences Department, Institute of Food Research, Reading Laboratory, Earley Gate, Reading, RG6 6BZ. Telephone: 0118 935 7000 Facsimile: 0118 926 7917 E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

The food choice and intake of animals (including humans) has typically been studied using frameworks of learning and feeding motivation. When used in isolation such frameworks could be criticized because learning paradigms give little consideration to how new food items are included or excluded from an individual's diet, and motivational paradigms do not explain how individuals decide which food to eat when given a choice. Consequently we are posed with the question of whether individuals actively interact with the food items present in their environment to learn about their nutritional properties? The thesis of this review is that individuals are motivated to actively sample food items in order to assess whether they are nutritionally beneficial or harmful. We offer a unifying framework, centred upon the concept of exploratory motivation, which is a synthesis of learning and paradigms of feeding motivation. In this framework information gathering occurs on two levels through exploratory behaviour: (i) the discrimination of food from nonfood items, and (ii) the continued monitoring and storage of information concerning the nutritional properties of these food items. We expect that this framework will advance our understanding of the behavioural control of nutrient intake by explaining how new food items are identified in the environment, and how individuals are able to monitor changes in the nutritional content of their food resource.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © The Nutrition Society 1998

References

Ackroff, K., Manza, L. & Sclafani, A. (1993). The rat's preference for sucrose, polycose and their mixtures. Appetite 21, 6980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Almeida, S. S., Garcia, R. A., Cibien, M. M. R., DeAraujo, M., Moreira, G. M. S. & DeOliveira, L. M. (1994). The ontogeny of exploratory behaviors in early-protein-malnourished rats exposed to the elevated plus-maze test. Psychobiology 22, 283288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asahida, Y. & Mimura, K. (1972). Eating behavior of chicks to 4 weeks of age. Journal of the Faculty of Fisheries and Animal Husbandry, Hiroshima University 11, 1522.Google Scholar
Baker, B.J. & Booth, D.A. (1989). Genuinely Olfactory preferences conditioned by protein repletion. Appetite 13, 223227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, B. J., Booth, D. A., Duggan, J. P. & Gibson, E. L. (1987). Protein appetite demonstrated: learned specificity of protein-cue preference to protein need in adult rats. Nutrition Research 7, 481487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balleine, B. (1992). Instrumental performance following a shift in primary motivation depends on incentive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes 18, 236250.Google ScholarPubMed
Bence, J. R. & Murdoch, W. W. (1986). Prey size selection by the mosquitofish: relation to optimal diet theory. Ecology 67, 324336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. New York: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biquand, S. & Biquand-Guyot, V., (1992). The influence of peers, lineage and environment on food selection of the criollo goat (Capra hircus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science 34, 231245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birch, L. L. & Marlin, D. W. (1982). I don't like it; I never tried it: effects of exposure on two-year-old children's food preferences. Appetite 3, 353360.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolen, R. H. & Green, S. M. (1997). Use of olfactory cues in foraging by owl monkeys (Aotus nancymai) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Comparative Psychology 111, 152158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolles, R. C. (1975). Theory of Motivation, 2nd edn. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Booth, D. A. (1985). Food conditioned eating preferences and aversions with introceptive elements: conditioned appetites and satiety. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 443, 2237 (Experimental Assessments and Clinical Applications of Conditioned Food Aversions [Braveman, N.S. & Bronstein, P, editors]).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, D. A. (1992). Integration of internal and external signals in intake control. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 51, 2128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brigham, A. J. & Sibly, R. M. (1998). A review of the phenomenon of neophobia. In Advances in Vertebrate Pest Management, pp. [Cowan, D.P., & Feare, C. J., editors]. Furth: Filander Verlag GmbH. (in press)Google Scholar
Castonguay, T. W. (1985). Studies of food choice: the nutritional challenge. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8, 334335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charnov, E. L. (1976). Optimal foraging, the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology 9, 129136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheney, C. D. & Miller, E. R. (1997). Effects of forced flavor exposure on food neophobia. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53, 213217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coil, J. D., Hankins, W. G., Jenden, D. J. & Garcia, J. (1978). The attenuation of a specific cue-to-consequence association by antiemetic agents. Psychopharmacology 56, 2125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowans, P. E. (1983). Exploration in small mammals: ethology and ecology. In Exploration in Animals and Humans, pp. 147175 [Archer, J. and Birke, L. I. A., editors]. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
Dardaillon, M. (1989). Age-class influences on feeding choices of free-ranging wild boars (Sus scrofa). Canadian Journal of Zoology 67, 27922796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Day, J. E. L., Kyriazakis, I. & Lawrence, A. B. (1995). The effect of food deprivation on the expression of foraging and exploratory behaviour in the growing pig. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 42, 193206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Day, J. E. L., Kyriazakis, I. & Lawrence, A. B. (1996 a). The use of a second-order schedule to assess the effect of food bulk on the feeding motivation of growing pigs. Animal Science 63, 447455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Day, J. E. L., Kyriazakis, I. & Lawrence, A. B. (1996 b). An investigation into the causation of chewing behaviour in growing pigs: the role of exploration and feeding motivation. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 48, 4759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Day, J. E. L., Kyriazakis, I. & Lawrence, A. B. (1996 c). The use of a second-order schedule to measure feeding motivation in the pig. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 50, 1531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Day, J. E. L., Kyriazakis, I. & Rogers, P. J. (1997). Feeding motivation in animals and man: a comparative review of its measurement and uses. Nutrition Abstracts and Reviews A 67, 107117.Google Scholar
Day, J. E. L., Rogers, P. J., Finch, G. M., & Gaffan, E. A., (1998). Do food related cues elicit ‘general’ or ‘specific’ appetites in the rat? Proceedings of the British Society of Animal Science, in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, A. J., Frutos, P., & Young, S. A. (1997). Rates of oxalic acid degradation in the rumen of sheep and goats in response to different levels of oxalic acid administration. Animal Science 65, 451456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, I. J. H., & Hughes, B. O. (1972). Free and operant feeding in domestic fowls. Animal Behaviour 20, 775777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, G. R., Newman, J. A., Parsons, A. J., & Krebs, J. R. (1997). Use of cues by grazing animals to locate food patches: an example with sheep. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 51, 5968.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmans, G. C. (1991). Diet selection by animals: theory and experimental design. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 50, 5964.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmans, G. C. & Kyriazakis, I. (1995). The idea of optimization in animals—uses and dangers. Livestock Production Science 44, 189197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, J. M. (1992). Metabolic aspects of satiety. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 51, 1319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forbes, J. M. & Kyriazakis, I. (1995). Food preferences in farm animals: why don't they always choose wisely. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 54, 429440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forkman, B. A. (1993). Self-reinforced behaviour does not explain contra-freeloading in the Mongolian gerbil. Ethology 94, 109112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frädrich, H. (1974). A Comparison of Behavior in the Suidae. The Behavior of Ungulates and its Relation to Management, pp. 133143. Morges: International Union for Conservation of Nature and Natural Resources.Google Scholar
Fraser, A. F. & Broom, D. M. (1990). Farm Animal Behaviour and Welfare, 3rd edn, p.34. London: Baillière Tindall.Google Scholar
Freeland, W. J. & Janzen, D. H., (1974). Strategies in herbivory by mammals: the role of plant secondary compounds. American Naturalist 108, 269289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galef, B. G. (1977). Mechanisms for the social transmission of food preferences from adult to weanling rats. In Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection, pp. 123150 [Barker, L. M., Best, M. and Domjan, M., editors]. Waco: Baylor University Press.Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. (1996). Food selection: problems in understanding how we choose foods to eat. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 20, 6773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, J. (1989). Food for Tolman: cognition and cathexis in concert. In Aversion, Avoidance and Anxiety, pp. 4585 [Archer, T. and Nilsson, L., editors]. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Garcia, J., Ervin, F. R., Yorke, C. H. & Koelling, R. A. (1967). Conditioning with delayed vitamin injections. Science 155, 716718.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, E. L. & Booth, D. A. (1989). Dependence of carbohydrate-conditioned flavor preference on internal state in rats. Learning and Motivation 20, 3647.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, K. F. & Garcia, J. (1971). Recuperation from illness: flavor enhancement for rats. Science 173, 749751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, G. C., Elwin, R. L., Mottershead, B. E., Keogh, R. G. & Lynch, J. J. (1984). Long tern effects of early experience to supplementary feeding in sheep. Proceedings of the Australian Society of Animal Production 15, 373375.Google Scholar
Hill, A. J. & Blundell, J. E. (1990). Sensitivity of the appetite control system in obese subjects to nutritional and serotoninergic challenges. International Journal of Obesity 14, 219233.Google ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R. A. (1970). Animal Behaviour: a synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology, 2nd edn. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Hocking, P. M. & McCormack, H. A., (1995). Differential sensitivity of ovarian follicles to gonadotrophin stimulation in broiler and layer lines of domestic fowl. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility 105, 4955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, J. A. (1973 a). Development of food recognition in young chicks. I. Maturation and nutrition. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 83, 355366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, J. A. (1973 b). Development of food recognition in young chicks. II. Learned associations over long delays. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 83, 367373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holman, E. W. (1973). Temporal properties of gustatory spontaneous alternation in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 85, 536539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houston, A. & Sumida, B. (1985). A positive feedback model for switching between two activities. Animal Behaviour 33, 315325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, X. F., Zhang, G. X. & Herring, S. W. (1994). Age changes in mastication in the pig. Comparative Biochemistry & Physiology 107A, 647654.Google Scholar
Hughes, R. N. (1993). Introduction. In Diet Selection, pp. 19 [Hughes, R. N, editor]. London: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Illius, A. W. & Gordon, I. J. (1993). Diet selection in mammalian herbivores: constraints and tactics. In Diet Selection, pp. 157181 [Hughes, R. N., editor]. London: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Inglis, I. R. (1983). Towards a cognitive theory of exploratory behaviour. In Exploration in Animals and Humans, pp. 72115 [Archer, J. and Birke, L. I. A, editors]. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
Inglis, I. R. & Ferguson, N. J. K., (1986). Starlings search for food rather than eat freely available identical food. Animal Behaviour 34, 614616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglis, I. R., Forkman, B. & Lazarus, J. (1997). Free food or earned food? A review and fuzzy model of contrafreeloading. Animal Behaviour 53, 11711191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inglis, I. R., Shepherd, D. S., Smith, P., Haynes, P. J., Bull, D. S., Cowan, D. P. & Whitehead, D. (1996). Foraging behavior of wild rats (Rattus norvegicus) towards new foods and bait containers. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 47, 175190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, G. D. (1963). Preference for bar pressing over ‘freeloading’ as a function of number of rewarded presses. Journal of Experimental Psychology 65, 451454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, M. B., Kyriazakis, I. & Lawrence, A. B. (1993). The activity and straw directed behaviour of pigs offered foods with different crude protein content. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 37, 211221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalat, J. W. (1974). Taste salience depends on novelty, not concentration, in taste-aversion learning in the rat. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 86, 4750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyriazakis, I. (1994). The voluntary food intake and diet selection of pigs. In Principles of Pig Science, pp. 85105 [Cole, D.J.A.Wiseman, J. and Varley, M., editors]. Nottingham: Nottingham University Press.Google Scholar
Kyriazakis, I., (1997). The nutritional choices of farm animals: to eat or what to eat? In Animal Choices, pp. 5565 [Forbes, J.M., Lawrence, T. L. J., Rodway, R. G. and Varley, M. A., editors]. Edinburgh: British Society of Animal Science.Google Scholar
Kyriazakis, I., Anderson, D. H., & Duncan, A. J., (1998). Conditioned flavour aversion in sheep: the relationship between the dose of a plant secondary compound and the acquisition and persistence of aversion. British Journal of Nutrition 79, 5562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyriazakis, I., Emmans, G. C. & Whittemore, C. T. (1990). Diet selection in pigs: choices made by growing pigs given foods of different protein concentrations. Animal Production 51, 189199.Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. (1992). A theoretical investigation of the role of social transmission in evolution. Ethological Sociobiology 13, 87113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Plotkin, H. C., (1990). Social learning and social transmission of foraging information in Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). Animal Learning and Behavior 18, 246251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, A. B., Terlouw, E. M. C., & Kyriazakis, I. (1993). The behavioural effects of undernutrition in confined farm animals. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 52, 219229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leathwood, P. D. & Ashley, D. K. M. (1983). Behavioural strategies in the regulation of food choice. Experientia 44 (Suppl.) 171196.Google ScholarPubMed
Le Magnen, J. (1986). Hunger. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Levitsky, D. A. (1979). Malnutrition and the hunger to learn. In Malnutrition, Environment and Behavior, pp. 161179.[Levitsky, D. A., editor]. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
McFarland, D. J. (1971). Feedback Mechanisms in Animal Behaviour. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McFarland, D. J. & Sibly, R. M., (1975). The behavioural final common path. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B270, 265293.Google ScholarPubMed
Mehiel, R. & Bolles, R. C. (1984). Learned flavor preferences based on caloric outcome. Animal Learning and Behavior 12, 421427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mela, D. & Rogers, P. J. (1998). Food, Eating and Obesity: the psychobiological basis of appetite and weight control. London: Chapman and Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mizra, S. N. & Provenza, F. D. (1990). Preference of the mother affects selection and avoidance of foods by lambs differing in age. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 28, 255263.Google Scholar
Mizra, S. N. & Provenza, F. D. (1992). Effects of age and conditions of exposure on maternally mediated food selection by lambs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 33, 3542.Google Scholar
Morrison, G. R. (1974). Alterations in palatability of nutrients for the rat as a result of prior tasting. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 86, 5661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicol, C. J. & Pope, S. J. (1994). Social-learning in sibling pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 40, 3143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olsen, J. D. & Ralphs, M. H. (1986). Feed aversion induced by intraruminal infusion with larkspur extract in cattle. American Journal of Veterinary Research 47, 18291833.Google ScholarPubMed
Pelchat, M. L. & Rozin, P. (1982). The special role of nausea in the acquisition of food dislikes by humans. Appetite 3, 341351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, D. M. (1987). Optimal diet theory: behavior of a starved predatory snail. Oecologia 72, 360365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pliner, P., Eng, A. & Krishnan, K. (1995). The effects of fear and hunger on food neophobia in humans. Appetite 25, 7787.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Provenza, F. D. (1995). Postingestive feedback as an elementary determinant of food preference and intake in ruminants. Journal of Range Management 48, 217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Provenza, F. D. (1996). Acquired aversions as the basis for varied diets of ruminants foraging on rangelands. Journal of Animal Science 74, 2010–2020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Provenza, F. D. & Burritt, E. A. (1991). Socially-induced diet preference ameliorates conditioned food aversion in lambs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 31, 229236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Provenza, F. D. & Cincotta, R. P. (1993). Foraging as a self-organizational learning process: accepting adaptability at the expense of predictability. In Diet Selection, pp. 78101. [Hughes, R. N.,editor]. London: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Provenza, F. D., Ortega-Reyes, L., Scott, C. B., Lynch, J. J. & Burritt, E. A. (1994). Antiemetic drugs attenuate food aversions in sheep. Journal of Animal Science 72, 19891994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ralphs, M. H. (1997). Persistence of aversions to larkspur in naive and native cattle. Journal of Range Management 50, 367370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rechten, C., Avery, M. & Stevens, A. (1983). Optimal prey selection: why do great tits show partial preferences? Animal Behaviour 31, 576584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renner, M. J. (1988). Learning during exploration: the role of behavioral topography during exploration in determining subsequent adaptive behavior. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 2, 4356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renner, M. J. (1990). Neglected aspects of exploratory and investigatory behavior. Psychobiology 18, 1622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renner, M. J. & Seltzer, C. P. (1994). Sequential structure in behavioral components of object investigation by Long-Evans rats (Rattus norvegicus). Journal of Comparative Psychology 108, 335343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Revusky, S. H. & Bedarf, E. W. (1967). Association of illness with prior ingestion of novel foods. Science 155, 219220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richman, C. L., Dember, W. N. & Kim, P. (19861987). Spontaneous alternation behaviour in animals: a review. Current Psychological Research and Reviews 5, 358391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, P. J. & Blundell, J. E. (1984). Meal patterns and food selection during the development of obesity in rats fed a cafeteria diet. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 8, 441453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, P. J. & Blundell, J. E. (1990). Psychobiological bases of food choice. British Nutrition Foundation Bulletin 15, Suppl, 1, 3140.Google Scholar
Rogers, P. J. & Blundell, J. E. (1991). Mechanisms of diet selection: the translation of needs into behaviour. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 50, 6570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, P. J., Tonkiss, T. & Smart, J. L. (1986). Incidental learning is impaired during early life undernutrition. Developmental Psychobiology 19, 113124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, S. P. & Kyriazakis, I. (1991). Diet selection of pigs and poultry. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 50, 8798.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P. (1969). Adaptive food sampling patterns in vitamin deficient rats. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 69, 126132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvatierra, N. A., Torre, R. B. & Arce, A. (1997). Learning and novelty induced increase of central benzodiazepine receptors from chick forebrain, in a food discrimination task. Brain Research 757, 7984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sibly, R. M. (1975). How incentive and deficit determine feeding tendency. Animal Behaviour 23, 437446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, P. B. (1993). Behaviour-genetic analyses and poultry husbandry. Poultry Science 72, l6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyderman, M. (1983). Optimal prey selection: the effects of food deprivation. Behaviour Analysis Letters 3, 359369.Google Scholar
Steiner, J. E., (1983). What the neonate can tell us about umami. In Umami: a basic taste, pp. 97123. [Kawamura, Y. and Kare, M. R., editors]. New York: Marcel Dekker Inc.Google Scholar
Stephens, D. W., & Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging Theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Thorhallsdottir, A. G., Provenza, F. D. & Balph, D. F. (1987). Food aversion learning in lambs with or without a mother: discrimination, novelty and persistence. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 18, 327340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toates, F. M. (1980). Animal Behaviour—a systems approach. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Toates, F. M.. (1981). The control of ingestive behaviour by internal and external stimuli—a theoretical review. Appetite 2, 2550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toates, F. M. (1983). Exploration as a motivational and learning system: a cognitive incentive view. In Exploration in Animals and Humans, pp. 5571. [Archer, J and Birke, L. I. A., editors]. New YorkVan Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
Toates, F. M. (1986). Motivational Systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Toates, F. M. (1997). Has pure ethology the right foundations for applied ethology? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 53, 113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tolkamp, B. J., Day, J. E. L. & Kyriazakis, I. (1998). Measuring nutrient intake in farm and laboratory animals. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society, in press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolkamp, B. J. & Ketelaars, J. J. M. H. (1992). Toward a new theory of feed intake regulation in ruminants. 2. Costs and benefits of feed consumption: an optimization approach. Livestock Production Science 30, 297317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treit, D.Spetch, M. L. & Deutsch, J. A. (1983). Variety in the flavor of food enhances eating in the rat: a controlled demonstration. Physiology and Behavior 30, 207211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, S. (1997). Visual discrimination of real objects and pictures in pigeons. Animal Learning and Behavior 25, 185192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weingarten, H. P. (1983). Conditioned cues elicit feeding in sated rats: a role for learning in meal initiation. Science 220, 431433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weingarten, H. P. (1984). Meal initiation controlled by learned cues: basic behavioral properties. Appetite 5, 147158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weingarten, H. P. (1985). Stimulus control of eating: implications for a two-factor theory of hunger. Appetite 6, 387401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiting, J. W. M. & Mowrer, O. H. (1943). Habit progression and regression—a laboratory study of some factors relevant to human socialization. Journal of Comparative Psychology 36, 229253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiepkema, P. R. (1971). Positive feedbacks at work during feeding. Behaviour 39, 266273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood-Gush, D. G. M., Stolba, A. & Miller, C. (1983). Exploration in farm animals and animal husbandry. In Exploration in Animals and Humans, pp. 198209. [Archer, J. and Birke, L. I. A., editors]. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
Wood-Gush, D. G. M. & Vestergaard, K. (1989). Exploratory behavior and the welfare of intensively kept animals. Journal of Agricultural Ethics 2, 161169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood-Gush, D. G. M. & Vestergaard, K. (1991). The seeking of novelty and its relation to play. Animal Behaviour 42, 599606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, P. (1991). Development of food choice during infancy. Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 50, 107113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeomans, M. R. (1996). Palatability and the micro-structure of feeding in humans: the appetizer effect. Appetite 27, 119133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, P. T. (1940). Reversal of food preferences of the white rat through controlled pre-feeding. Journal of General Psychology 22, 3366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahorik, D. M., Maier, S. F. & Pies, R. W. (1974). Preferences for tastes paired with recovery from thiamine deficiency in rats: appetitive conditioning or learned safety? Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology 87, 10831091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed