Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T14:28:56.590Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sandwich EPP hypothesis: Evidence from child Finnish

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 April 2010

Pauli Brattico
Affiliation:
Department of Computer Science and Information Systems, PL35, 40014, University of Jyväskylä, Finland. [email protected]
Taija Saikkonen
Affiliation:
Cognitive Science Unit, Institute of Behavioral Sciences, PL 9, 00014, University of Helsinki, Finland. [email protected]
Get access

Abstract

It is well-known that grammatical movement is somehow linked to functional heads. There is less agreement on the excact nature of this correlation. According to one view, phrases are moved to the specifier positions of functional heads because functional heads attract them. According to another view, movement is not triggered by functional heads alone, but depends on the larger grammatical context. For instance, one such proposal says that T (tense) becomes attractive only when selected by finite C (complementizer), while V becomes attractive when selected by v* (transitivizer). What attracts phrases are therefore the C–T system and the v*–V system as a whole, not the individual functional heads; moved phrases are then sandwitched between the two heads. In this article, we present evidence in favor of this view by looking at first language acquisition. The data shows that in child Finnish, subject determiner phrases (DPs) move into the position of grammatical subject if and only if the full complementizer system has matured. Movement to the (Spec, TP) subject position therefore depends on the presence of C.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Nordic Association of Linguistics 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Alexiadou, Artemis & Anagnostopoulou, Elana. 1998. Parametrizing Agr: Word order, V-movement and EPP-checking. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 16 491539.Google Scholar
Baker, Mark C. 2008. The Syntax of Agreement and Concord. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bellugi, Ursula. 1967. The Acquisition of Negation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Blake, Barry J. 2001. Case. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, Louis. 1970. Language Development: Form and Function in Emerging Grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Boeckx, Cedric. 2008. Bare Syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bošković, Željko. 2002. A-movement and the EPP. Syntax 5 167218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa. 1973. Early Semantic Development: A Cross-linguistic Study with Special Reference to Finnish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Braine, Martin D. S. 1963. The ontogeny of English phrase structure: The first phase. Language 30 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brattico, Pauli & Huhmarniemi, Saara. 2006. Finnish negation, EPP-principle and the valuation theory of morphosyntax. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 29 (1), 544.Google Scholar
Brown, Roger. 1973. A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Cheng, Lisa Lai-Shen & Corver, Norbert (eds.). 2006. Wh-movement: Moving on. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1981. Lectures in Government and Binding: The Pisa Lectures. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1982. Some Concepts and Consequences of the Theory of Government and Binding. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Martin, Roger, Michaels, Davic & Uriagereka, Juan (eds.), Step by Step: Essays on Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik, 89156. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Kenstowicz, Michael (ed.), Ken Hale: A Life in Language, 152. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2004. Beyond explanatory adequacy. In Belletti, Adriana (ed.), Structures and Beyond: The Cartography of Syntactic Structures, vol. 3, 104131. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2008. On phases. In Freidin et al. (eds.), 133–166.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Lasnik, Howard. 1993. The theory of principles and parameters. In Jacobs, Joachim, von Stechow, Arnim, Sternefeld, Wolfgang & Venneman, Theo (eds.), Syntax: An International Handbook of Contemporary Research, 506569. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, Harald. 1991. Constraints on parameter setting: A grammatical analysis of some acquisition stages in German child language. Language Acquisition 1 361391.Google Scholar
Clahsen, Harald, Eisenbeiss, Sonja & Vainikka, Anne. 1994. The seeds of structure: A syntactic analysis of the acquisition of Case marking. In Hoekstra & Schwartz (eds.), 85–118.Google Scholar
Clahsen, Harald, Penke, Martina & Parodi, Teresa. 1993. Functional categories in early child German. Language acquisition 3 395429.Google Scholar
Collins, Chris. 1997. Local Economy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Déprez, Vivian & Pierce, Amy E.. 1993. Negation and functional projections in early grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 24 2567.Google Scholar
Déprez, Vivian & Pierce, Amy E.. 1994. Crosslinguistic evidence for functional projections in early child grammar. In Hoekstra & Schwartz (eds.), 57–84.Google Scholar
Drozd, Kenneth F. 1995. Child English pre-sentential negation as metalinguistic exclamatory sentence negation. Journal of Child Language 22 583610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drozd, Kenneth F. 2002. Negative DPs and elliptical negation in child English. Language Acquisition 10 77122.Google Scholar
Freidin, Robert, Otero, Carlos P. & Zubizarreta, Maria Luisa (eds.). 2008. Foundational Issues in Linguistic Theory: Essays in Honor of Jean-Roger Vergnaud. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grinsted, Jane. 2004. Subjects and interface delay in child Spanish and Catalan. Language 80 4072.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, Tony & Wexler, Kenneth. 1996. The optional-infinitive stage in child English. In Clahsen, Harald (ed.), Generative Perspectives on Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Hiraiwa, Ken. 2005. Dimensions of Symmetry in Syntax: Agreement and Clausal Architecture. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Hoekstra, Teun & Schwartz, Bonnie D. (eds). 1994. Language Acquisition Studies in Generative Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Holmberg, Anders. 2000. Scandinavian stylistic fronting: How any category can become an expletive. Linguistic Inquiry 31 441484.Google Scholar
Holmberg, Anders. 2005. Is there a little pro? Evidence from Finnish. Linguistic Inquiry 36 533564.Google Scholar
Holmberg, Anders & Nikanne, Urpo. 1993. Introduction. In Holmberg, Anders & Nikanne, Urpo (eds.), Case and Other Functional Categories in Finnish Syntax, 122. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Holmberg, Anders & Nikanne, Urpo. 2002. Expletives, subjects, and topics in Finnish. In Svenonius (ed.), 71–106.Google Scholar
Hyams, Nina. 1994. V2, null arguments and COMP projections. In Hoekstra & Schwartz (eds.), 21–56.Google Scholar
Joseph, Kate L. & Pine, Julian M.. 2002. Does error-free use of French negation constitute evidence for very early parameter setting. Journal of Child Language 29 7186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaiser, Elsi. 2006. Negation and the left periphery in Finnish. Lingua 116 214350.Google Scholar
Kauppinen, Anneli. 1982. Kuinka negaatio kasvaa? Virittäjä 2 140163.Google Scholar
Klima, Edward S. & Bellugi, Ursula. 1967. Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In Lyons, John & Wales, Roger J. (eds.), Psycholinguistic Papers, 183–208. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Landau, Idan. 2007. EPP extensions. Linguistic Inquiry 38 485523.Google Scholar
Lasnik, Howard & Saito, Mamoru. 1992. Move-α. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lebeaux, David. 1988. Language Acquisition and the Form of the Grammar. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherts.Google Scholar
Manzini, Rita M. 1995. From Merge and Move to form dependency. UCLAWorking Papers in Linguistics 7 323346.Google Scholar
Martin, Roger. 1999. Case, the Extended Projetion Principle, and Minimalism. In Epstein, Samuel David & Hornstein, Norbert (eds.), Working Minimalism, 125. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, David. 1970. The Acquisition of Language. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Meisel, Jürgen M. & Müller, Natascha. 1992. Finiteness and Verb Placement in early child grammars. Evidence from simultaneous acquisition of two first languages: French and German. In Meisel, Jürgen M. (ed.), The Acquisition of Verb Placement: Functional Categories and V2 Phenomena in Language Development, 109138. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Miyagawa, Shigeru. 2001. The EPP, scrambling, and wh-in-situ. In Kenstowicz, Michael (ed.), Ken Hale: A Life in Language, 293338. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ouhalla, Jamal. 1991. Functional Categories and Parametric Variation. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Penner, Zvi. 1994. Asking questions without CPs? On the acquisition of root wh-questions in Bernese Swiss German and Standard German. In Hoekstra & Schwartz (eds.), 177–213.Google Scholar
Pierce, Amy. 1992. Language Acquisition and Syntactic Theory: A Comparative Analysis of French and English Child Grammars. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Pierce, Amy & Deprez, Viviane. 1993. Negation and functional projections in early child grammar. Linguistic Inquiry 24 2567.Google Scholar
Poeppel, David & Wexler, Kenneth. 1993. The full competence hypothesis of clause structure in early German. Language 69 365424.Google Scholar
Radford, Andrew. 1990. Syntactic Theory and the Acquisition of English Syntax: The Nature of Early Child Grammars of English. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Radford, Andrew. 1998. Towards a structure-building model of acquisition. In Harris, Tony & Wexler, Kenneth (eds.), Generative Perspectives on Language Acquisition, 4389. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Reintges, Chris H., LeSourd, Philip & Chung, Sandra. 2006. Movement, wh-agreement, and apparent wh-in-situ. In Cheng & Corver (eds.), 165–194.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 1993. Some notes on linguistic theory and language development: The case of root infinitives. Language Acquisition 3 371393.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 1994. Early null subjects and root null subjects. In Hoekstra & Schwartz (eds.), 151–176.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 1997. The fine structure of the left periphery. In Haegemann, Liliane (ed.), Elements of Grammar, 289330. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 2004. Locality and left periphery. In Belletti, Adriana (ed.), Structures and Beyond: The Cartography of Syntactic Structures, vol. 3, 223251. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 2006. On the form of chains: Criterial positions and ECP effects. In Cheng & Corver (eds.), 97–133.Google Scholar
Roberts, Ian & Roussou, Anna. 2002. The Extended Projection Principle as a condition on the tense dependency. In Svenonius (ed.), 125–155.Google Scholar
Ross, John Robert. 1967. Constraints on variables in syntax. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT.Google Scholar
Stromswold, Karen. 1996. Does the VP-internal subject stage really exist? Presented at the Twenty-first Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development.Google Scholar
Stromswold, Karen. 1997. The acquisition of inversion and negation in English: A reply to Deprez and Pierce (1993). Ms., Rutgers University.Google Scholar
Svenonius, Peter (ed.). 2002. Subjects, Expletives, and the EPP. Oxford & New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Toivainen, Jouni. 1980. Inflectional Affixes Used by Finnish-speaking Children Aged 1–3 Years. Helsinki: SKS.Google Scholar
Valian, Virginia. 1991. Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition 40 2181.Google Scholar
Vergnaud, Jean-Roger. 2008. Letter to Noam Chomsky and Howard Lasnik on ‘Filters and Control’, April 17, 1977. In Freidin et al. (eds.), 3–15.Google Scholar
Wexler, Kenneth. 1994. Finiteness and head movement in early child grammars. In Lightfoot, David & Hornestein, Norbert (eds.), Verb Movement, 305350. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zanuttini, Raffaella. 1997. Negation and Clausal Structure: A Comparative Study of Romance Languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar