1. Introduction
The perception of similarity is a marker of social identification (Jetten et al., Reference Jetten, Spears and Manstead2001). Accordingly, expressions of similarity provide information on social identification. Similarity can manifest itself across various dimensions, including gender, ethnicity, appearance, social class, culture, behavior and attitude (McCroskey et al., Reference McCroskey, Richmond and Daly1975; Lee et al., Reference Lee, Hutchens, George, Wilson-Howard, Cooks and Krieger2022). By expressing similarity, individuals define their social identity and place within a particular social environment. Sociolinguistic research has expanded this concept to include language, which is defined here as linguistic similarity, or the perception that others speak similarly to oneself (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Hutchens, George, Wilson-Howard, Cooks and Krieger2022). Language plays a dual role in identity formation, serving as both a marker of belonging and a vehicle for reinforcing identity (Kiesling, Reference Kiesling, Chambers and Schilling2018; Joseph, Reference Joseph, Llamans and Watt2012; Bell, Reference Bell1984).
Although it is acknowledged that similarity is a marker of identification, there is limited evidence on why people identify with others. This study builds on the concept of homophily. Homophily, or the fact that similar individuals are more likely to connect with one another (McPherson et al., Reference McPherson, Smith-Lovin and Cook2001), operates through two intertwined processes: social influence and social selection (Bidart et al., Reference Bidart, Degenne and Grossetti2020). Social selection suggests that individuals will interact with others they perceive as similar to themselves, while repeated interactions tend to reinforce this similarity. These two joint processes are cyclical and self-reinforcing.
Homophily also fosters group belonging, encouraging individuals to adopt positive attitudes toward fellow group members (Mok et al., Reference Mok, Morris, Benet-Martinez and Karakitapoglu-Aygün2007; Leszczensky, Reference Leszczensky2013; Repke and Benet-Martinez, Reference Repke and Benet-Martínez2017). These positive attitudes are reflected in feelings of similarity (Gallois et al., Reference Gallois, Gasiorek, Giles, Soliz and Ed2016). However, the broad concept of ‘interaction’ needs further refinement to identify the underlying dimensions that influence social identification (Stryker, Reference Stryker2008; Kiesling, Reference Kiesling, Chambers and Schilling2018). Adopting an Eliassian approach, this study considers interactions anchored in chains of interdependent exchanges that enable the functioning of the broader social network. These functional exchanges refer, for instance, to chains of support, conflict, and a mixture of both, i.e. ambivalence (Widmer, Reference Widmer2010). The distribution of such functional exchanges creates tensions and opportunities for the network’s members, making the network unpredictable and dynamic. It is therefore the interplay between such chains of interdependence that fosters social identification (Stryker, Reference Stryker2008).
Our theoretical framework emphasizes the roles of social support, conflict and ambivalence in the process of social identification, expressed through linguistic similarity. Furthermore, we explore how these dynamics differ across social domains, such as family and friends, and their role in either reinforcing or reducing social identification. Empirically, our hypotheses are tested using data from a sample of college 126 students and their personal relations (n = 1035) in the French-speaking region of Switzerland. The analysis employs logistic multilevel regression models to account for the hierarchical structure of dyadic ties.
2. The social identification process
Social Identity Theory or SIT (Tajfel, Reference Tajfel1981) elucidates how individuals internalize their membership of social groups and categories by incorporating them into their self-conceptions. This process generates a sense of self that is shaped by both enduring and transient group affiliations, facilitated through identification with these groups (Haslam et al., Reference Haslam, Cruwys, Milne, Kan and Haslam2016; Postmes and Jetten, Reference Postmes and Jetten2006). From this perspective, any social category has the potential to assume a defining role in an individual’s identity within a given context or life stage. As a result, social identity reflects and prescribes group norms, conventions and sanctions, which in turn shape individual behavior, decisions and choices.
Traditionally, studying the link between language and social identification from a quantitative perspective falls within this approach (Kiesling, Reference Kiesling, Chambers and Schilling2018). Linguistic forms serve as social markers that express social identities and can be used to infer them (Hernández-Campoy, Reference Hernández-Campoy Juan2016). Social categories, such as gender, class and ethnicity, are central to the process of linguistic identification (Ash, Reference Ash, Chambers and Schilling2018; Queen, Reference Queen, Chambers and Schilling2018; Fought, Reference Fought, Chambers and Schilling2018). These categories, while not always explicitly salient, are critical in shaping how communities define themselves and compel individuals to position themselves in relation to them (Kiesling, Reference Kiesling, Chambers and Schilling2018).
In this regard, language serves as a means to convey a sense of belonging and signals social proximity (Bell, Reference Bell1984). People use the language of the group they identify with (Kiesling, Reference Kiesling, Chambers and Schilling2018). Furthermore, when an individual shows linguistic similarity toward another individual. it is a marker of a positive attitude (Gallois et al., Reference Gallois, Gasiorek, Giles, Soliz and Ed2016; Welkowitz and Feldstein, Reference Welkowitz and Feldstein1969) and an acknowledgment of the other as belonging to the same group (Giles, Reference Giles2016). Labov (Reference Labov1976), in his seminal New York City project, showed that the social categories of class, gender, ethnicity and age were all crucial categories of identification and drivers of linguistic choices and attitudes. Similarly, Sharma (Reference Sharma2017) showed that individuals adapt their language when they identify with the ethnicity of their interlocutors.
While the variationist and sociolinguistic traditions have focused on social categories, there are challenges to this approach (Kiesling, Reference Kiesling, Chambers and Schilling2018). It remains unclear whether the use of a linguistic variant and the perception of similarity arise from identification with a category, or if they are driven by opportunities for interaction, which, in accordance with the social influence of homophily, lead individuals to become more similar.
A complementary view of social identification arises from symbolic interactionism within structuralist approaches (Stets and Serpe, Reference Stets and Serpe2016; Stryker, Reference Stryker2008; Brenner et al., Reference Brenner2021; Burke and Stets, Reference Burke and Stets2023). This structuralist view of identification is aligned with the framework proposed by Bucholtz and Hall (Reference Bucholtz2005), which states that identity is the product of interactions. Their fourth principle suggests that the linguistic expression of identity is a relational process. In her seminal work, Milroy (Reference Milroy1987) demonstrated that individuals with multiplex relationships in their networks were more likely to adopt linguistic variants that were characteristic of their community. At the dyadic level, Kovács and Kleinbaum (Reference Kovács and Kleinbaum2020) showed that friends within a network tend to use similar pronouns over time.
2.1 Social support, conflict and ambivalence
Interactions and social relationships are crucial in the link between language and identification. It is well established that social relationships and interactions enable similarity, but not all social ties have the same influence. Failing to account for the content and strength of these relationships risks overlooking important effects. For example, Fernandez (Reference Fernandez2023) showed that only reciprocal ties and the most frequent primary source of discussions were related to the emergence of shared linguistic practices. Therefore, as has been argued (Fernandez, Reference Fernandez2023; Kiesling, Reference Kiesling, Chambers and Schilling2018), a nuanced understanding of social ties is necessary to fully grasp the connection between social relations and linguistic identification.
From an Eliassian perspective, interactions are embedded in chains of interdependence within social networks. These interdependencies—whether supportive, conflictual, or ambivalent—are crucial to the network’s functioning (Widmer, Reference Widmer2010). The distribution of these exchanges generates both opportunities and tensions, contributing to the dynamic and sometimes unpredictable nature of the network. Functional exchanges are essential for fostering social identification (Stryker, Reference Stryker2008; Burke ands Stets, 2023). Normative behavior provides information about group membership (Häusser et al., Reference Häusser, Abdel Hadi, Reichelt and Mojzisch2023). Thus, social identification with a specific group should be reinforced by the perceived social support provided by its members. A cohesive, supportive group fosters a sense of community, increasing the likelihood of social identification (McKimmie et al., Reference McKimmie, Butler, Chan, Rogers and Jimmieson2019; Doucerain et al., Reference Doucerain, Amiot and Ryder2022).
The position within the network of support is also a driver of identification. In the tradition of social network analysis, central individuals are considered to be prestigious (Wasserman and Faust, Reference Wasserman and Faust2018). Alters are more influential when they hold a central position (Aral and Walker, Reference Aral and Walker2014). Moreover, the act of providing support signals to recipients that they are valued members of the group, thereby reinforcing shared goals and values (Häusser et al., Reference Häusser, Abdel Hadi, Reichelt and Mojzisch2023). When individuals receive support, they are more likely to evaluate their alters positively and develop a sense of shared attitudes (Stets and Serpe, Reference Stets and Serpe2016). Therefore, central actors—those who provide support to many others—are perceived as key contributors to group cohesion and well-being (Cruwys et al., Reference Cruwys, Haslam, Dingle, Haslam and Jetten2014). As a result, they are often seen as representative of the group, promoting identification with them. For instance, Rodrigues et al. (Reference Rodrigues, Evans and Galatti2019) demonstrated that persons with a higher degree of centrality were more likely to identify with their sports-team peers. Similarly, Graupensperger et al. (Reference Graupensperger, Panza and Evans2021) provided evidence that out-degree centrality in friendship predicted stronger ingroup affects. Degree centrality has also been linked to identification stability over time (McFarland and Pals, Reference McFarland and Pals2005). In line with this insight, alters may be more influential when they hold a central position (Aral and Walker, Reference Aral and Walker2014).
Sociolinguistics also emphasizes central actors as drivers of linguistic identification. Central individuals, who are highly connected and have prestige among their peers, exert both direct and indirect influence over others (Fagyal et al., Reference Fagyal, Swarup and Escobar2010; Dodsworth and Benton, Reference Dodsworth and Benton2020). Labov (Reference Labov2001) argued that individuals who occupy a central position within the network were those who were able to spread a linguistic practice within the network because they were seen as more prestigious and therefore more influential.
Negative exchanges, such as conflicts, are perceived as threats to group cohesion and identity formation (Bochatay et al., Reference Bochatay, Bajwa, Blondon, Junod Perron, Cullati and Nendaz2019). Conflict serves as a marker of group boundaries, signaling divergence from group norms and values. At the linguistic level, evidence suggests that individuals who distance themselves and have negative attitudes toward others will be likely to diverge in their language (Giles, Reference Giles2016). This manifests itself in individuals deliberately adopting different ways of speaking to distance themselves from others. Therefore, when an alter is central to a conflict dynamic, they are less likely to be linguistically aligned with others in the network. For example, Felder (Reference Felder, Rüdiger and Dayter2020) analyzed WhatsApp conversations and found that some participants intentionally diverged in their use of emojis compared to their alters over time. Two distinct patterns of divergence emerged: participants either maintained their original usage patterns or adjusted their emoji use in the opposite direction, increasing or decreasing their usage counter to their alters.
However, social relations are rarely purely positive or negative. More often, they consist in a complex interplay of both, leading to ambivalence (Lüscher, Reference Lüscher2011). Ambivalence arises from the emotional dynamics of close relationships, where support and frequent interaction can give rise to conflict (Connidis, Reference Connidis2015). Interpersonal ambivalence exists when both positive and negative feelings toward an alter coexist or when there is a contradiction between the behavior and the sentiments (Connidis, Reference Connidis2015; Lüscher, Reference Lüscher2011). Ambivalence is not confined to dyadic processes but can extend to meso-level networks. The distribution of ambivalence within the network exerts a negative influence on the individual (Girardin et al., Reference Girardin, Widmer, Connidis, Castrén, Gouveia and Masotti2018), and alter’s well-being (de Bel and Widmer, Reference de Bel and Widmer2024, Conndis, Reference Connidis2015). A systematic review of the effect of ambivalence in interpersonal relations indicates that ambivalent ties have a stronger detrimental impact on the well-being of individuals than exclusively negative social relations (Zoppolat et al., Reference Zoppolat, Righetti, Faure and Schneider2024). Consequently, ambivalence toward the ingroup mitigates the achievement of social identification (Costarelli and Palmonari, Reference Costarelli and Palmonari2003).
2.2 Social domains
Social identification is domain-specific, meaning that individuals’ sense of belonging and identity is shaped not only by their perception of group membership, but also by the roles they occupy within their social networks (Stets and Serpe, Reference Stets and Serpe2016; Stryker, Reference Stryker2008; Brenner et al., Reference Brenner2021; Burke and Stets, Reference Burke and Stets2023). This perspective emphasizes the immediate social context, or personal network, as critical for validating identity (Markowski, Reference Markowski, Brenner, Stets and Serpe2021). More precisely, personal networks are composed of individuals who are assigned to different roles (mother, colleague, friend, etc.). Identification with a specific role depends on its prominence within the social network (Stryker, Reference Stryker2008). For example, if an individual is a manager in an organization, and most of the significant social relationships are entertained with colleagues, then there is a high probability that that person identifies with the role of ‘boss’.
Kiesling (Reference Kiesling, Chambers and Schilling2018) emphasizes the importance of social domains and roles in linguistic identification, showing that individuals adjust their stance and communication patterns depending on the role they perform. For example, Penelope (Reference Penelope2000) demonstrated that individuals tend to adjust their language to align with the speech patterns of members within their social networks and communities of practice.
Two of the most fundamental domains of socialization and social identification are friends and family. Empirical evidence demonstrates that the relationship between social ties and identification is domain-specific, with relationships in a given domain fostering identification with that particular group (Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Postmes and Stroebe2022; Häusser et al., Reference Häusser, Abdel Hadi, Reichelt and Mojzisch2023). That phenomenon is nevertheless not expected to be consistent across all domains of socialization. For instance, as adolescents transition into adulthood, they become less dependent on their parents and begin to derive socialization and reference points from other relationships, such as friends, colleagues, and peers (Chow et al., Reference Chow, Roelse, Buhrmester, Underwood, Laursen and Collins2012; Umaña-Taylor et al., Reference Umaña-Taylor, Kornienko, McDermott and Motti-Stefanidi2020; Repke and Benet-Martinez, Reference Repke and Benet-Martínez2017). Dense friendship networks in particular are proved to foster strong social identification (Markowski, Reference Markowski, Brenner, Stets and Serpe2021), and friendships have been identified as predictors of linguistic similarity (Kovacs and Kleinbaum, Reference Kovács and Kleinbaum2020). Similarly, support received from family members often leads to stronger identification with the family compared to support from neighbors or others in the community (Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Postmes and Stroebe2022).
Although it is logical to assume that supportive ties are likely to be maintained, it is equally plausible to argue that negative or ambivalent ties should lead to the dissolution of relationships. However, this is not always the case, especially in relationships that are not fully voluntary, such as those with family, neighbors, colleagues, or friends (Offer and Fischer, Reference Offer and Fischer2018), where interdependencies make dissolution of the relationship difficult. Moreover, negative valences are sometimes essential in intimate relationships, suggesting that negative and positive feelings often coexist. Ambivalence is inherent in many family relationships, for example (Connidis, Reference Connidis2015). Some negative and ambivalent ties are considered ‘natural and accepted’ (de Bel and Widmer, Reference de Bel and Widmer2024, p. 185), meaning that, while these relationships may be undesirable, they are not necessarily dysfunctional.
Importantly, ambivalent ties in family relationships do not yield the same outcomes as in non-family relationships. Ambivalence with friends, for example, tends to affect well-being negatively, whereas ambivalence in family ties does not have the same detrimental effect (Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Lansford and Volling2006; de Bel and Widmer, Reference de Bel and Widmer2024). Ambivalent ties along with positive ones are also involved in the process of both autonomy and social identity development (Arnett, Reference Arnett2000). Mixed-feeling relationships with family members are more socially accepted among young adults, as ambivalence may be perceived as part of the process of developing autonomy, while friendships often demand clearer valence for group belonging (de Bel and Widmer, Reference de Bel and Widmer2024). In friendships, ambivalence or negative ties are viewed as threats to the formation of a shared identity (Postmes and Jetten, Reference Postmes and Jetten2006).
In the context of linguistic identification, it is important to recognize that functional exchanges—whether supportive, conflictual, or ambivalent—may not be played out similarly across different social domains. For instance, ambivalence may negatively affect friendships but could have a positive association with family interactions. The domain-specific nature of these functional exchanges must therefore be taken into account when examining the role of social identification through language.
2.3 Hypotheses
Based on the literature reviewed above, we propose several hypotheses regarding the relationship between social identification and function-based exchanges, particularly as they relate to linguistic identification. These exchanges—whether supportive, conflictual, or ambivalent—are expected to influence social identification in distinct ways. Moreover, the role of social identification is influenced by an alter’s position within the relational dynamic. We therefore expect that (H1a) the more the alter entertains supportive ties with other members of the personal network, the more ego will perceive a link of linguistic similarity; conversely (H1b), the more alter entertains conflictual ties with other members of the personal network, the less ego will perceive a link of linguistic similarity. Less clear valence, or mixed-feeling, is also expected to have a specific association due to its inherent nature. Unlikely what has been hypothesized for the negative dynamic, alters involved at the core of ambivalence dynamics would presumably be seen as undesirable, thus favoring divergence. Thus, one may expect that the more active and central the alter in terms of ambivalent ties, the less ego will perceive a link of linguistic similarity.To that end, we assume that (H2) the more the alter entertains ambivalent ties with other members of the personal network, the less ego will perceive a link of linguistic similarity.
In addition to the functional dynamics of relationships, we also expect that the influence of these exchanges will vary by social domain. Different social contexts, such as family or friendship, come with unique norms and expectations that influence the effects of supportive, conflictual and ambivalent ties on identification. As these expectations are domain-dependent, we expect that (H3) the associations of functional-based exchanges on the perception of linguistic similarity will vary from one domain to another. In particular, we expect that ambivalence will be negatively associated with friendship, whereas we could expect a positive association for the interaction with family members.
3. Methods
We consider language perception at the dyadic level. Dyads can be considered as the most fundamental and basic social unit and form the smallest social element that can be defined as a ‘group’ (Wasserman and Faust, Reference Wasserman and Faust2018). However, dyads are nested in larger social networks. Given this hierarchical structure, we employed a multilevel modeling approach. To constitute the personal network of ego, we used the name generator inspired by the Family Network Method (Widmer, Reference Widmer1999): ego is asked to name the persons who have been important in the past six months, whether positively or negatively.
For the name-interpreter phase, basic sociodemographic information about each alter was asked. For the edge interpreter, ego was asked to tell to what extent each alter would give (i) emotional support or (ii) would be a source of annoyance and conflict. We also asked who, within the list of contacts previously identified, would give emotional support to other members of the network. In using this procedure, therefore, we are not trying to reconstitute the objective social network within which the individual is embedded but rather the subjective one. As has been argued (de Bel and Widmer, Reference de Bel and Widmer2024), these self-reported interdependencies probably mirror recurrent past engagements with individuals who have exerted various (positive, negative, or ambivalent) influences on individuals’ lives.
The focus is therefore not on the ‘actual’ structure of the network of interdependencies, but rather on the perceived structure. Individuals tend to imagine relationships that, in reality, do not exist, either by creating non-existent connections or by not perceiving existing ones, which even prompts some researchers to reconsider the relevance of relying on self-reported data when conducting relational research (Bernard et al., Reference Bernard, Killworth and Sailer1980). Nevertheless, the perception of reality motivates actions (Poupart, Reference Poupart2011) and can have real consequences on cognitive and emotional states (Holt-Lunstad et al., Reference Holt-Lunstad, Smith, Baker, Harris and Stephenson2015; Krackhardt and Kilduff, Reference Krackhardt and Kilduff1999).
3.1 Measurements
3.1.1 The dependent variable is the perception of similarity
To measure it, the respondents were asked to assess to what extent they perceive themselves to speak similarly to each alter, including aspects such as word choice, expressions and general speaking style. For each alter, respondents answered either ‘1’ if they perceived linguistic similarity or ‘0’ if they did not. This measure is adapted from Lee et al. (Reference Lee, Hutchens, George, Wilson-Howard, Cooks and Krieger2022) but differs from it as here it denotes a binary scale instead of a 7-point Likert Scale ranging from 1 (‘Does not speak like me’) to 7 (‘Does speak like me’). The binary scale was chosen to limit respondent fatigue and to maintain consistency with other network measures. A binary measure of the perception of similarity has been used elsewhere (Remez et al., Reference Remez2007), and no incidence was reported for the reliability and validity of the construct. This procedure allows the straightforward and accurate capture of linguistic similarity perception at the dyadic level without complex and composite measures, which could lead to response bias due to fatigue.
3.1.2 The centrality of the alter in the respondent’s network
To capture to what extent the alters are embedded in ego’s network, we used in-degree centrality. For supportive relationships, in-degree centrality indicates the extent to which an alter provides emotional support to others in the network. In conflict networks, in-degree centrality reflects the extent to which an alter is a source of conflict for other network members. For the ambivalence of centrality, we use the in-degree of the ambivalent network, or the support and the conflict networks multiplied together.
For social domains, alters were categorized into three main social domains: family members (including parents, grandparents, siblings, uncles, aunts, cousins and children), friends, and non-family relations (e.g., colleagues or neighbors).
Several control variables were included in the analysis to account for potential confounding factors. Because homophily suggests that the frequency and duration of interactions are functions of linguistic convergence (Fernandez, Reference Fernandez2023), we added a control variable that measures the frequency of contact: ‘How often are you in contact with [alter’s name], whether face-to-face, by phone, internet, or another communication device (5 = every day; 4 = almost every day; 3 = several times a week; 2 = several times per month; 1 = several times per year; 0 = less than several times per year)?’ Another quantitative control variable measured how long ego and alter have known each other.
We also introduce gender homophily as a control variable, indicating whether ego shares the same gender as alter. This addition stems from literature suggesting that gender homophily is a significant influence, as women are more likely to adapt themselves to their interlocutors (Van Hofwegen, Reference Van Hofwegen2015). Age is another potentially influential factor. Within the age cohort, language is often employed to delineate clear social boundaries, particularly during adolescence and early adulthood (Kirkham and Moore, Reference Kirkham, Moore, Chambers and Schilling2018). The sample is nevertheless highly biased towards the 20–29-year-old group because of the nature of our population. This results in age categories that are too small for performing statistical analyses. To account for this limitation, we include a homophily metric, indicating whether ego and alter belong to the same age group, scoring 1 for yes and 0 for no. Addtionally, because the perception of linguistic similarity may simply emerge from the fact of sharing the same linguistic background, we add a variable that measures the number of common mother tongues ego shares with the alters. Finally, to control for hierarchical and socioeconomic influences, respondents were asked to rate each alter’s position on the socioeconomic hierarchy using a 10-point Likert scale (Sapin et al., Reference Sapin, Joye and Wolf2020). This control aimed to capture any prestige-based effects on linguistic identification, given that social status can interact with network dynamics to shape identification processes (Labov, Reference Labov1976).
At the network level, we controlled for network size, as the number of alters in a personal network can influence both the perception of linguistic similarity and social identification (Lev-Ari, Reference Lev-Ari2018; Walker and Lynn, Reference Walker and Lynn2013; Leszczensky, Reference Leszczensky2013). Larger networks may also dilute the strength of identification processes, so network size was included as a confounder in the models.
3.2 Sample
This study investigates linguistic similarity in personal networks using a sample of 126 college students from the French-speaking region of Switzerland. These students provided information about their social ties, resulting in data on 1035 unique social relationships. This research received ethical approval from the ethical committee of the author’s university.Footnote 1
Table 1 shows the sample characteristics for both respondents and their alters. The sample consists of 61.9% women, with the majority of respondents falling into the 20–29 age group, which aligns with the characteristics of a college student population. Gender and age homophily were prominent, as 61.7% of the respondents’ ties were with individuals of the same gender, and 58.5% were with individuals from the same age group. The average size of the network is 9.25 individuals (S.D. = 2.63), and the average duration of the relationship is 16.81 years (S.D. = 11.55), suggesting that many of these ties were of longstanding. On average, respondents interacted with their alters several times per week (mean = 2.99, SD = 1.33). Alters’ socioeconomic prestige was rated at a mean score of 5.86 (SD = 1.66). In terms of network dynamics, alters had more central positions in the supportive network (mean = 2.81, SD = 2.21) than in the conflictual network (mean = 0.95, SD = 1.31). Ambivalent centrality was the lowest (mean = 0.62, SD = 1.03). Friends and family members share an even proportion (41.7%), and other types of relationship represent 16.5% of the ties. Overall, 46.1% of alters were perceived as linguistically similar to the respondents.
Table 1. Sample characteristics

3.3 Analytical strategy
As already stated, the data are hierarchically structured. Multilevel models are regression models that have been developed to take into account the nested nature of the data (Snijders and Bosker, Reference Snijders and Bosker2012). Because the output variable is binary, we use logistic multilevel regressions. To mitigate bias due to the relatively small number of level-1 units (mean = 9.2, range 1–11) within each level-2 unit, we opted for the Restricted Maximum Likelihood method (Hox and McNeish, Reference Hox, McNeish, van de Schoot and Miočevic2020). The models were implemented using the pymer4 Python package with the Lmer function.
4. Results
Table 2 presents the correlation matrix for each quantitative variable. We observe a high correlation between conflict centrality and ambivalence centrality, which is expected, since the ambivalence measure is derived from the interaction of the support and conflict networks. Therefore, by definition, interdependence exists between the constructs. This interdependence indicates that many individuals who provide support also cause friction, reflecting the complex nature of ambivalent relationships. Given this strong correlation, we tested conflict, support and ambivalence using separate models to avoid multicollinearity.
Table 2. Correlation matrix

Note: Bold characters are correlations with p-value < 0.05; for the association between binary and continuous variables, Point-Biserial correlations are computed.
Let us note that we observe quite small and non-significant bivariate associations for some of our variables of interest, although as Table 3 shows, in our bivariate models, larger and more significant effects are found. One reason for these differences may lie in the fact that the bivariate correlations do not take into account the nested structure of the data. However, the multilevel models allow within and between-group variation to be taken into account, which can clarify the effects of the binary associations.
The multilevel logistic regression results (Table 3) show the association between the centrality of alters in support, conflict and ambivalent networks and the perception of linguistic similarity. As suggested previously, in order to avoid any multicollinearity issues, support, conflict and ambivalence are treated in separate models. Model 1a refers to the bivariate association for the centrality in the support network, and Model 1b incorporates the control variables. Model 2a focuses on conflict centrality. Model 2b includes the control variables. The same applies to Model 3a and 3b.
The centrality of the alter in the supportive network is significantly and positively associated with linguistic identification. This effect holds even when control variables are added, underscoring the importance of supportive relationships in fostering linguistic alignment. Our results thus allow us to accept H1a.
In contrast, the relationship between conflict centrality and linguistic similarity is more nuanced. In simpler models (Model 2a, Table 3), centrality within conflict networks is negatively associated with linguistic similarity (OR = 0.88, p < 0.05), but this effect becomes non-significant once control variables are introduced (Model 2b). This suggests that the negative impact of conflict on linguistic identification may be influenced by other factors such as social domain or the nature of the relationships. Although the bivariate association goes in the expected direction, we must reject H1b, as the effect vanishes in more complex models.
Ambivalence centrality, contrary to expectations, shows a significant positive association with linguistic similarity when the control variables are included (OR = 1.37, p < 0.001). This indicates that in some contexts, ambivalent relationships—those marked by both support and conflict—may still foster identification, potentially reflecting the complexity of family or long-term relationships where ambivalence is more accepted. H2 is therefore rejected.
Table 3. Logistic multilevel regressions odd ratios for the link between linguistic similarity perception and functional-based centralities (N = 126, n = 1035)

Note: Odd ratios are shown, with 2.5 and 97.5 confidence intervals in brackets; ***p < 0.001 **p < 0.01 *p < 0.05 +p < 0.1; Intra Class Correlation = 0.22.
Table 4 shows the interaction between functional centrality (support, conflict, ambivalence) and social domains (family, friends, non-family) on linguistic similarity. Models ‘a’ focus on the link between the interactions and the perceptions of linguistic similarities, while the confounding variables are added in Models ‘b’. Models 1 focus on support, Models 2 emphasize conflict and Models 3 include ambivalence. Centrality in the supportive network enhances linguistic similarity across all social domains. Alters who provide support, whether family members or friends, are more likely to be perceived as linguistically similar (OR = 1.25, p < 0.05 for family; OR = 1.29, p < 0.05 for friends), compared to other domains.
Conflict centrality shows divergent effects across domains, but only in the simple models. While conflict within non-family networks decreases the likelihood of linguistic similarity (OR = 0.64, p < 0.05), within family networks, it has a positive association (OR = 1.76, p < 0.05). This finding suggests that in family settings, conflict may not entirely disrupt linguistic alignment, perhaps due to the enduring nature of family relationships, which often tolerate both conflict and support. However, the effect vanishes when control variables are included in the models.
Ambivalence centrality further supports the idea that family dynamics are unique. Ambivalence is positively associated with linguistic similarity when alter is a family member (OR = 1.93, p < 0.05), indicating that mixed feelings within family ties do not preclude identification and may even strengthen it. In contrast, ambivalence has no significant effect on linguistic similarity in friendships or non-family relationships, implying that ambivalent ties are more readily accepted in familial contexts.
To sum up, overall our results give validation to H3. We see first that not all function-based exchanges show similar patterns, and that these patterns are not consistent across all domains. We observe that conflict and ambivalence with friends are not related to linguistic identification, whereas that is the case for support, for which we observe that being either a supportive friend or a family member enables social identification.
Other dyadic factors also play a significant role in linguistic identification. More frequent interactions between ego and alter are positively associated with linguistic similarity (OR = 1.26, p < 0.001), suggesting that regular contact facilitates linguistic alignment. Age homophily is another significant predictor of linguistic similarity (OR = 3.32, p < 0.001), supporting the idea that shared demographic characteristics enhance identification through language.
When it comes to the impact of the structure of the personal network, it seems that none of the properties exert any influence. The structural effect of level-2 thus seems to be controlled by level-1 properties. However, when size is added to the model without the dyadic-level cofounder, we see a significant and negative relation (OR = 0.89, p < 0.05), suggesting that the more dispersed and extensive a network, the weaker the individual connections, thereby reducing the likelihood of linguistic alignment.
5. Discussion
This study contributes to our understanding of how social support, conflict and ambivalence within personal networks influence perceptions of linguistic similarity. The findings emphasize that the nature of social ties—whether supportive, conflictual, or ambivalent—significantly shapes perceptions of linguistic alignment, and that these effects differ across social domains.
Supportive ties, particularly when central within a network, foster perceived linguistic similarity. This supports existing sociological theories that positive social exchanges enhance cohesion and identification within groups (Doucerain et al., Reference Doucerain, Amiot and Ryder2022). Individuals who provide emotional support play a key role in facilitating the perception of linguistic convergence, reinforcing the idea that language is a marker of belonging and social identity (Joseph, Reference Joseph, Llamans and Watt2012). This aligns with previous findings that emphasize the role of supportive relationships in strengthening social bonds and shared identity (McKimmie et al., Reference McKimmie, Butler, Chan, Rogers and Jimmieson2019).
Table 4. Logistic multilevel regressions odd ratios for the link between linguistic similarity perception, domains, and functional-based centralities (N = 126, n = 1035)

Note: Odd ratios are shown, with 2.5 and 97.5 confidence intervals in brackets; ***p < 0.001 **p < 0.01 *p < 0.05 +p < 0.1.
Conflict introduces complexity into these dynamics. While conflict generally weakens identification, particularly in non-family relationships, the results in the simple models suggest that conflict within family networks does not necessarily disrupt linguistic similarity. This resilience in familial ties may be due to the enduring nature of family relationships, where conflict and support coexist, allowing for continued identification despite negative exchanges (Bochatay et al., Reference Bochatay, Bajwa, Blondon, Junod Perron, Cullati and Nendaz2019). Such findings challenge traditional assumptions that conflict inherently undermines cohesion, highlighting the need for a more nuanced understanding of how conflict operates within close social ties (Widmer, Reference Widmer2010).
Ambivalence extends this pattern, its effects varying with social role. Our results indicate that ambivalence has a positive association when alter is a family member. As previously suggested, mixed feelings are more acceptable and natural in family relationships, benefiting social identification. Ambivalence may be seen as a consequence of positive involvement that comes with emotional costs. As Girardin et al. (Reference Girardin, Widmer, Connidis, Castrén, Gouveia and Masotti2018) argue, negative feelings often stem from positive ones. Familial support therefore fosters stronger identification compared to other types of relationship (Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Postmes and Stroebe2022), compensating for the negative impact of ambivalence. Among young adults, ambivalent ties are more socially accepted in family contexts (de Bel and Widmer, Reference de Bel and Widmer2024) because ambivalence is often seen as part of the process of developing autonomy, making negative interactions more manageable within the complexity of familial dynamics. This effect of ambivalence, however, does not extend to friendships, contrary to prior expectations (Sherman et al., Reference Sherman, Lansford and Volling2006). While it was hypothesized that ambivalence would harm friendships, recent evidence shows that even less harmonious ties in friendships do not impact well-being (de Bel and Widmer, Reference de Bel and Widmer2024), and a similar association emerges for linguistic identification. These results are also aligned with the argument that ambivalent relations have a greater effect than univalent negative relations (Zoopolat et al., 2014; Costarelli and Palmonari, Reference Costarelli and Palmonari2003).
The domain-specific nature of these findings is critical. Family relationships appear to accommodate a wider range of emotional exchanges, both supportive and conflictual, while still maintaining identification. In contrast, non-family relationships may be less tolerant of conflict or ambivalence, requiring clearer valence for social identification to occur (Postmes and Jetten, Reference Postmes and Jetten2006). These results align with the idea that social identification is context-dependent, varying by social domain and by the expectations that govern relationships within each domain (Stets and Serpe, Reference Stets and Serpe2016; Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Postmes and Stroebe2022).
Our results are aligned with the wider literature linking homophily and social identification (McFarland and Pals, Reference McFarland and Pals2005; Graupensperger et al., Reference Graupensperger, Panza and Evans2021; Rodrigues et al., Reference Rodrigues, Evans and Galatti2019; Umaña-Taylor et al., Reference Umaña-Taylor, Kornienko, McDermott and Motti-Stefanidi2020). Increased contact frequency strengthens linguistic identification, consistent with the homophily and social influence theories, which suggests that similarity increases with more frequent interactions (McPherson and Smith-Lovin, Reference McPherson and Smith-Lovin1987). Interestingly, the duration of relationships shows no significant effect, implying that linguistic identification relies more on actively maintained ties rather than longer, but potentially dormant connections that act as social reserves (Cullati et al., Reference Cullati, Kliegel and Widmer2018). Age-based homophily also plays a crucial role in perceived linguistic similarity, supporting prior research that highlights age as a key boundary marker in social groups. Language often reflects and reinforces the age-based structure of social organization (Kirkham and Moore, Reference Kirkham, Moore, Chambers and Schilling2018).
5.1. Limitations
Despite the contributions of this study, several limitations must be acknowledged. One limitation is the absence of key variables such as the nationality or ethnicity of both ego and alter, as well as nationality homophily, which could influence perceptions of linguistic similarity. Our data only distinguish respondents and alters as Swiss, French, or other, which limits a more nuanced analysis. However, we do control for linguistic factors by including mother tongue and other languages spoken by both ego and alter. Given the strong correlation between nationality and language, this variable helps capture some of the potential national and ethnic influences that might otherwise be missed.
Our reliance on cross-sectional data limits the ability to draw causal inferences, as the temporal dynamics of social ties and linguistic similarity cannot be fully captured. We understand that the interplay between homophily and social identification is intricate, given its cyclical and self-reinforcing nature. Future research would benefit from longitudinal designs that track changes in social ties and perceptions of linguistic similarity over time.
The sample’s homogeneity, consisting mainly of college students from the French-speaking part of Switzerland, also limits the generalizability of the findings. Expanding the study to more diverse populations, both culturally and demographically, would enhance the broader applicability of the results. The choice of this age group is driven by the fact that this period is pivotal not only for the students themselves but also for their social relationships. As individuals navigate this transition, their participatory roles shift, affecting resource distribution and shaping future life trajectories (Levy and Bühlmann, Reference Levy and Bülhmann2016). It also significantly alters the composition of personal networks, often leading to the formation of new friendships at the expense of family ties (Bidart and Lavenu, Reference Bidart and Lavenu2005).
6. Conclusion
In this paper, we have examined how social similarity dynamics are shaped by broader social structures. Our findings show that dyadic attitudes are not isolated but are embedded within a larger social network. This suggests that identification processes depend not only on direct exchanges between ego and alter but also on alter’s centrality within the broader network. Thus, social identification is influenced by both dyadic interactions and the relational structure within the group.
This study also highlights the importance of considering different types of relations and incorporating the quality of ties. It extends prior research on social identification and language by taking a sociological perspective. While previous quantitative sociolinguistic research, often grounded in identity theory (Tajfel, Reference Tajfel1981), focuses on group-based identities and attributes, we emphasize relational patterns and roles in the emergence of social identification.
The study also opens several promising directions for future research in both sociolinguistics and social network analysis. The findings highlight the importance of ambivalence in relationships—especially within family networks—challenging the traditional view that only positive or supportive ties foster linguistic similarity. Future studies could delve more deeply into the role of ambivalent relationships in shaping language use, particularly across different social domains and cultural contexts, to understand the nuanced ways in which mixed emotions influence linguistic behavior.
Finally, the observed influence of alter centrality on linguistic practices suggests that future research should explore the interplay between network structure and language dynamics in more detail. Studies could investigate how different types of central actor (e.g., influential leaders vs. peripheral members) affect linguistic alignment within diverse groups, shedding light on the intersection of power, status and language use in social networks.
Competing interests
None.
Financial support
This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Data availability
The data is available upon request.