Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-03T19:05:11.590Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Remembering in the wild: recontextualising and reconciling studies of media and memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2022

Tim Fawns*
Affiliation:
University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK
*
Corresponding author: Tim Fawns, email: [email protected]

Abstract

Studies that locate memory entirely within the head may pay less attention to the properties, practices or cultures of the media with which people remember than studies of ‘memory in the wild’, where memory is seen to extend beyond the individual, into the distributed activities of people and material things. While memory in the head is, apparently, individual and susceptible to universal effects, memory in the wild is emergent and relational. Studies of memory in the wild, therefore, produce results that are harder to pin down but may form a stronger basis for interpreting the importance of context. It is an important, interdisciplinary challenge to reconcile evidence from studies based on these different conceptions, so that we can better understand how people remember and forget, individually and collectively, and the relationship between context, environment, and memory. I argue that wherever memory is located or studied, all remembering can be framed as in the wild, and that doing so supports ecological validity, conceptual precision, reflexivity, and realistic application of conclusions beyond the research context. A key part of my argument is that the relationship between media, technology, and memory is situated, highly complex, and not easily generalisable. Remembering in the wild supports the conceptual precision needed to understand the subtle and entangled implications of technological change in relation to memory.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Wild views of memory and remembering

Where is memory? Barnier and Hoskins (Reference Barnier and Hoskins2018) argue that most memory research can be crudely divided into that which studies memory in the head, and that which studies memory in the wild. Memory in the head is individual, internal, self-contained, yet susceptible to universal effects. This is the focus of much of memory psychology. Examples include many studies of episodic and semantic memory (Tulving Reference Tulving, Tulving and Donaldson1972), mental time travel (Suddendorf et al Reference Suddendorf, Addis and Corballis2009), scene construction (Hassabis and Maguire Reference Hassabis and Maguire2007), source monitoring (Johnson et al Reference Johnson, Hashtroudi and Lindsay1993), and memory accuracy (Koriat et al Reference Koriat, Goldsmith and Pansky2000). Memory in the wild is, often, emergent, relational, and contextual; located out in the world, distributed across people, places, and things. Memory in the wild cannot be reduced to the individual (Barnier and Hoskins Reference Barnier and Hoskins2018, 389). It is social and material, and located in relation to cultural and personal, historical trajectories of practice, emotion, and belief. Examples of memory in the wild include work on transactive memory (Wegner Reference Wegner, Mullen and Goethals1987), collective memory (Halbwachs Reference Halbwachs1992), prosthetic memory (Landsberg Reference Landsberg2004), postmemory (Hirsch Reference Hirsch1997), mediated memory (Hoskins Reference Hoskins2011; van Dijck Reference van Dijck2007) and distributed, embodied, embedded, enacted, or extended remembering (e.g. Brown and Reavey Reference Brown and Reavey2018; Clark and Chalmers Reference Clark and Chalmers1998; Harris et al Reference Harris, Barnier, Sutton and Keil2014; Sutton et al Reference Sutton, Harris, Keil and Barnier2010; Tribble Reference Tribble2005). As even this very selective list demonstrates, there are many ways to conceive of memory in the wild.

When it comes to media and memory, this head/wild distinction can have profound implications. Generalisations appear simpler when memory is located in an individual's head because it is separated from the social and contextual influences that act on it. Laboratory experiments, for example, are designed to minimise ‘contextual noise’ so that memory can be measured in a ‘purer’ form. Yet, choices are still made, implicitly or explicitly, about what counts as neutral (e.g. the physical environment and materials, or the behaviour of the researchers) and representative of the wider population (e.g. participant engagement with tasks and media). The same can be true of naturalistic studies of individuals operating in more complex settings, where choices must be made (again, implicitly or explicitly) about what counts as ‘natural’.

Naturalistic enquiry may be more conducive than laboratory studies to studying memory in the wild for the simple reason that it is, ideally, designed not to control context but to embrace it, seeing it as inevitable and fundamental to everyday memory (Neisser Reference Neisser, Gruneberg, Morris and Sykes1978). However, naturalistic research does not necessarily study memory in the wild. Whether memory is in the head or wild depends on how memory is conceived rather than on the research setting. Outside of the laboratory, there is still an important distinction between whether context is an external factor that acts on a separate, internal memory, or whether context is a constituent element of an emergent, distributed remembering system that makes use of, but is not entirely contained within, the brain.

Perhaps, it is not so important where memory is, since we are not actually studying memory much of the time. The use of proxies like questionnaires, narratives, or brain scans, indicates the inaccessibility of internal memory processes. Their observable manifestations are always coupled with some form of externalisation (Tulving Reference Tulving1982). Any expression of memory as an object of study must be a form of remembering that is not just influenced by context, but produced in context. A participant recounts a narrative because a researcher has given particular instructions in a particular setting; another completes a questionnaire in response to being shown a particular photograph for a particular purpose. These instances of remembering happen because of the context, and they manifest in direct response to that context. I propose that recognising context as a constituent element, rather than an external influencer, is a framing of remembering in the wild.

In this commentary, I argue that wherever memory is located, all remembering can be framed as in the wild, and that doing so supports ecological validity, conceptual precision, reflexivity, and realistic application of conclusions beyond the research context. A key part of my argument is that the relationship between media, technology, and memory is situated, highly complex, and not easily generalisable. Only with remembering in the wild will we have sufficient conceptual precision to understand the subtle and entangled implications of technological change in relation to memory.

Appreciating context

As Tulving noted, all remembering is cued (Tulving Reference Tulving1984). His concept of synergistic ecphory (the coming together of internal memory trace and external retrieval information) conveys a rich sense of the distribution of remembering processes between brain and environment without implying that memory systems extend beyond an individual's head. However, this mix of information has important implications for how we understand research into memory and media. Through engagement with media, for example, we manipulate and configure our available retrieval information. This is a natural part of remembering: we have always incorporated elements from the world around us into our remembering activity (Donald Reference Donald1991), whether they be landmarks, cave paintings, post-it notes, conversations, photographs, or web pages. However, retrieval information is not static or inert – elements of our environment influence remembering processes as well as content. In making use of the world to help us remember, we distribute some of our remembering agency. The combinations of multiple media (e.g. photographs and mobile phones and social media platforms) and their entanglements in our social relations with other people are too complex for us to control, particularly if we look across longer time periods. Consider the use of photographs in remembering. We do not simply look at a photograph and remember. Photography practices are diverse and idiosyncratic, they often involve other people, and their effects are entangled with other technologies and media (e.g. cameras, albums, physical displays, and social media platforms) (Fawns Reference Fawns2020) as well as longer term social and cultural activities and values (Keightley and Pickering Reference Keightley and Pickering2014).

Recall during experiments, interviews and other research methods is situated within these wider histories, and ongoing reconfigurations of memory and identity (Fawns Reference Fawns2022). A thoroughly contextualised understanding of remembering also requires understanding the role of research (including settings, tasks, media) and researchers, as part of the situated remembering activity of participants. Whether it is acknowledged or not, there are always multiple media involved, research tasks are not neutral elicitations of memory, and researchers are not neutral implementers of methods from outside of the action.

Experimental studies seeking effects of one kind of medium, such as photographs, often ignore the influence of other media that are in play, or how multiple media combine in influencing acts of remembering. All media, including word lists, questionnaires, brain scans, etc., inevitably, are non-neutral (Grall and Finn Reference Grall and Finn2022). For example, the Memory Characteristics Questionnaire (MCQ), a commonly used tool for testing the phenomenological qualities of recall (Johnson et al Reference Johnson, Foley, Suengas and Raye1988), not only measures but also influences recall by prompting participants to consider past episodes in multiple ways. Constraints placed on the remembering context in the interests of standardisation or control should also be taken into account. Examples include: focusing on individual participants or prescribed groups; participants’ awareness of being researched; bounding of events, locations, and time periods; determining which media are to be used (e.g. specific photos or kinds of photo); determining legitimate forms of response (e.g. narratives, questionnaires, recognition, ‘free recall’); determining which forms of practice are included (e.g. looking at but not taking photos, or vice versa); and more. Researchers, themselves, cannot help being co-participants in their research. Through instructions, body language, power dynamics, etc., they contribute directly and indirectly to what is remembered. Of course, researchers often recognise these limitations, and there are pragmatic arguments for using the best methods and resources available. However, there is still an obligation to help readers meaningfully interpret results by thoroughly describing the factors that matter, and using a theoretical lens capable of making sense of the implications.

I argue that a theoretical lens that locates remembering in the wild is more powerful in relation to analysing these issues of context than one that locates remembering inside the head. If remembering happens in the head, media are more likely to be seen simply as tools or cues, external to memory processes and, therefore, to the primary expertise of the memory researcher. Media properties, practices, and cultures may, therefore, be largely ignored. On the other hand, a wild view of remembering can help us see that there is no hard boundary between remembering with and without media, and that media and technology are deeply embedded in our ways of thinking, experiencing, and bringing order to the world (Sutton Reference Sutton and Menary2010). Properly theorised, seeing remembering as in the wild can support a more meaningful account of the complex social and material relations in which remembering is entangled as part of a cognitive ecology (Hutchins Reference Hutchins2010).

This more complex account can be supported by generating research data that is directly about context and how it is entangled in memory-related activity. An example is the work of Kath Bicknell, John Sutton, Celia Harris, Amanda Barnier, and others (see Sutton et al Reference Sutton, Bicknell and Harris2020; or Bicknell and Sutton's Reference Bicknell and Sutton2022 book on Collaborative Embodied Performance: Ecologies of Skill, for more developed examples from a different field). Building on a research programme spanning more than a decade, which includes collaborative remembering experiments with couples in their homes (Barnier et al Reference Barnier, Harris, Morris and Savage2018; Harris et al Reference Harris, Barnier, Sutton and Keil2014, Reference Harris, Barnier, Sutton and Savage2018, Reference Harris, Sutton, Keil, McIlwain, Harris, Barnier, Savage and Dixon2022; Sutton et al Reference Sutton, Harris, Keil and Barnier2010), they simultaneously conducted an ethnographic study of two couples as they participated in those experiments. Participant observation, analysis of video footage and semi-structured interviews with these two couples generated insights beyond the experimental data. For example, participants’ activity and the recall strategies they used in a given test were shaped by seating arrangements, room layouts, fluctuating anxiety, familiarity with researchers, methods, current affairs, shared histories (including international travel), and so on. This does not mean that the experiment was invalid, but that all experiments are context-rich and differently meaningful to different participants. It is not a question of preventing the research environment from shaping results, but understanding how it does so.

Conceptual precision

A nuanced appreciation of the complex, contextual relationship between media and memory would caution researchers against extrapolating results of experimental studies to all humans, or all photographic media, technologies or practices. It would show, for example, that Henkel's (Reference Henkel2014) ‘photo-taking impairment effect’ (where taking photos was found to impair recall and recognition of photographed objects under certain conditions) cannot be generalised to photography and memory more widely without acknowledging the limitations of neglecting historical context or limiting photographic practices (Fawns Reference Fawns2022). Similarly, a more thorough appreciation of the role of context would illuminate the problem with Sparrow et al (Reference Sparrow, Liu and Wegner2011) proposing a very broad ‘Google effect’ without that search engine, nor the Internet, featuring in their methods or results (Heersmink Reference Heersmink2016). These studies suggest something interesting about how we pay attention that is worthy of further exploration, but it is premature to make claims beyond the limited forms of media engagement featured in the research. Controlled media-memory studies can help us understand something about memory mechanisms, but not much about generalisation to wider populations (Mook Reference Mook2013). As Mook argues, our goal is often not to generalise to a wider population but to better understand how something (remembering, in this case) works. Media and technology are not homogeneous things with fixed properties, where one instance can stand in for all related instances and produce the same results. Understanding the role of media in relation to memory requires paying attention to specific media properties, practices, and cultures.

Choices relating to media must be carefully explained and justified, since they are important to the activity that takes place (Grall and Finn Reference Grall and Finn2022). There are simply no abstract, objective, or archetypal media-memory practices, and media are never representative of everyday experience (Grall and Finn Reference Grall and Finn2022). These challenges reinforce the importance of describing and analysing the role of context. While it is not possible to find media and tasks that are meaningful to all people and situations (Kvavilashvili and Ellis Reference Kvavilashvili and Ellis2004), whichever media and tasks feature in our research become meaningful to these participants in relation to this particular research context. Understanding this situated combination of factors is an important aim for research into media and memory. Furthermore, researchers must take care to consider the wider social and material contexts in which media feature in research. For example, we cannot understand everyday photography without understanding the technologies of camera, photographs, albums, and social media platforms, and how they are integrated into broader life.

Ecological validity

Brown and Reavey (Reference Brown and Reavey2015) proposed an expanded view of memory that allows for connections between individual and collective; and between cognitive, social, and material forms of remembering. This move from memory to remembering is crucial because it is the situated activity, rather than the system itself, about which researchers from different disciplines are most likely to find commonality. In relation to the activity of remembering, different perspectives are not equivalent, but nor are they incompatible. For example, in the psychology of memory, ecological validity calls for us to interpret remembering as embedded in an ecology where context is not noise but a constituent element (Neisser Reference Neisser, Gruneberg, Morris and Sykes1978). Drawing on a tradition of work in media studies on ‘media ecologies’, Brown and Hoskins (Reference Brown and Hoskins2010) and Hoskins (Reference Hoskins2016) develop the idea of a ‘memory ecology’, foregrounding the interdependence of material and cultural environments with cognition and emotion. While these different views may lead to different conclusions, they find common ground in their focus on the emergent, contextualised articulation of memory. Perhaps, then, the answer to Barnier and Hoskins's (Reference Barnier and Hoskins2018, 389) question, ‘how are memories in the head and in the wild related’ lies in expressions of ecological validity around remembering, whether or not memory is conceived of as individual and internal, or collective and distributed. The relation between head and wild is that, no matter where memory is, all remembering is always in the wild.

This reframing can explain why there is no clear-cut, binary distinction between laboratory and naturalistic methods (Winograd Reference Winograd, Neisser and Winograd1988). Remembering in lab studies cannot be disconnected from the natural world, and field studies cannot avoid influencing that which they seek to study (Kvavilashvili and Ellis Reference Kvavilashvili and Ellis2004). These issues do not invalidate the results of memory research. Even outside of research, people are never free to do exactly as they want because they are part of ecologies that constrain possibilities for action. What is important is how we theorise and take account of these limitations to representativeness and generalisability. ‘Wild’ views allow us to reflexively reconcile lab and natural settings as just another remembering context. A lab is, potentially, just as ecologically valid as family reunions, conversations in a park, or watching TV after work, as long as we recognise that it is not context-free, and that prescriptive and highly-constraining choices of event-type, location, timing, media, and options for response all contribute to the emergent remembering activity. Indeed, it is impossible to choose a representative context for everyday remembering due to the diversity of ways in which everyday life emerges in socially and materially situated activity. I do not mean that labs are somehow equivalent to family homes in relation to remembering, but that we cannot take any context for granted as representing the kinds of remembering people do.

More important than how we label a research study (e.g. as lab or naturalistic) is its utility (Winograd Reference Winograd, Neisser and Winograd1988), which is contingent on our justifications for applying our conclusions to other situations. If context plays a constituent, rather than merely an influencing, role, then the qualities of remembering are not transferable in any straightforward way. They are differently produced in relation to whatever conditions are present at the time. This makes blanket claims such as that Google is ‘making us stupid’ (Carr Reference Carr2010) or that taking photos ‘impairs’ memory (Henkel Reference Henkel2014) problematic because they do not take into account the importance of particular situations, individual practices, or cultures of use. Of course, there are patterns of commonality across situations and across individuals, even if these may be founded upon, or entangled in, layers of culture (Bartlett Reference Bartlett1932; Wang Reference Wang and Mace2019). However, in relation to digital media and technology, the slowly emerging evidence does not support simple conclusions. Instead, it paints a complex picture of different effects that are dependent on different factors (Schacter Reference Schacter2022).

Seeing remembering as inevitably situated can generate richer understandings but make it harder to extrapolate those understandings to human memory more generally. This may seem like a serious limitation to many researchers, but, I argue, it gives us a more honest and sophisticated understanding of how remembering actually works in relation to the complexities of media and the world around us. Wild views support greater reflexivity around the remembering context and, therefore, more precise and appropriate application of results and conclusions. More nuanced understandings may help us to elaborate or critique other generalisations, particularly those featuring linear, causal affects: if this then that. For remembering in the wild, this is always an entangled construct. Remembering is never free from the intertwining of individual, social, cultural, political, economic, and environmental factors, and this is heightened when we turn our attention to the role of media and technology.

The results of any memory study can be reframed as a particular case of wild remembering through interpretation of the environment and research practices as non-neutral, and as socially, materially, and culturally situated. Methodology sections of experimental papers, in particular, are often rich with mechanical details but lacking in conceptual precision about media and reflexivity about context. Yet, if we are interested in particular individuals, groups, and situations, we need to go beyond statistics and interrogate the heterogeneity of contexts, samples, and populations. A necessary ingredient in this analysis is a clearly articulated theory of remembering that takes the external world, including media and technology practices, meaningfully into account.

Mixing disciplines for a richer picture

It is an important, interdisciplinary challenge to reconcile evidence from different methods and methodologies (Barnier and Hoskins Reference Barnier and Hoskins2018; Brown and Reavey Reference Brown and Reavey2015; Sutton Reference Sutton and Menary2010). To see the richness and variety of lived experience, we need multiple approaches working together (not always in harmony, for disagreement, tension and debate are constructive aspects of interdisciplinary work). This kind of cross-fertilisation poses some significant quandaries. How do we work across disciplines and perspectives without getting bogged down in longstanding ontological or disciplinary debates (e.g. what kinds of knowledge are valid; what counts as evidence, or, indeed, the mind)? I argue that any philosophical divide between head and wild research will be more easily negotiated with a focus on remembering rather than memory.

Locating remembering in the wild, rather than in the head, provides a more versatile conception with which to cross the divide because it can allow for memory systems to remain firmly in the head. Wild remembering still involves brains, and can accommodate internal constructs such as episodic or semantic memory, associated feelings, emotions and imagery, as long as these are understood as part of a wider, distributed activity (Fawns Reference Fawns2022; Sutton Reference Sutton and Menary2010). Thus, it does not stop researchers from collaborating with, or engaging with, those who locate and study memory in the head. As Tulving (Reference Tulving1991, 41) argued, ‘memory, like countless other objects of scientific curiosity, can be studied and described at many different levels, from many different perspectives, using many different approaches and methods’. Tulving, the originator of the episodic/semantic distinction (Tulving Reference Tulving, Tulving and Donaldson1972), exemplifies an openness to reframings of memory. His work, although it cautiously suggests a view of memory in the head and has primarily featured word lists and the study of apparently independent memory systems in labs, demonstrates a profound respect for the influence of the environment on memory. Tulving recognises that we must extract memory from the head to study it – it must always be converted via proxies (e.g. verbal expressions of recall) that can be observed by researchers (Tulving Reference Tulving1982). Through such conversions, memory is performed out in the wild (even where the wild is partly constituted by a lab) because performance is necessarily situated in context. Nor does Tulving strictly confine memory systems to the brain, taking care to clarify that what we are studying is the convergence of cognitive processes grouped together under the label of ‘memory’ (Tulving Reference Tulving2002). For him, what matters is not a strong commitment to established conceptions of memory but the potential value of different conceptions and approaches to our greater understanding.

Conclusion

A key challenge for the field of memory studies (and thus a core provocation for Memory, Mind & Media) is to reconcile understandings of memory in the head and in the wild. An important aspect of this is to reconcile evidence from experimental studies that, for the most part, pay little attention to the contexts, practices, and cultures of media engagement, with studies (including those from other disciplines) in which such things are part of what it means to remember. In this commentary, I have argued that the distinction between head and wild (Barnier and Hoskins Reference Barnier and Hoskins2018) can be explained in ecologically valid terms by reframing all remembering as in the wild. Even when the memory system is considered to be in the head, and even when that head is in a lab, the system still remembers in the wild. This can help us to take account of the complex contexts and relations in which empirical results are produced, and help us not to jump too far ahead of the evidence or to accept sensationalised claims (e.g. Google is making you stupid, photos are destroying memory, etc.). It can also create the versatility we need for interdisciplinary debate and collaboration, by allowing for different conceptions of memory systems while taking account of contextual factors and media and technology practices as constituent elements of remembering.

Data availability statement

This article contains no empirical data.

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Kath Bicknell, Celia Harris, and John Sutton for insights into the ethnography of experiments work that, along with others, they have been involved in. Thanks to Andy Hoskins and Amanda Barnier for the invitation to write this commentary, and for their guidance along the way. Thanks to Hamish Macleod and Ethel Quayle for their invaluable contributions to my thinking.

Financial Support

This work received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares none.

Tim Fawns (University of Edinburgh) is a senior lecturer of clinical education at the University of Edinburgh. His research interests include clinical and digital education, technology, photography, and autobiographical memory.

References

Barnier, AJ and Hoskins, A (2018) Is there memory in the head, in the wild? Memory Studies 11(4), 386390. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698018806440CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnier, AJ, Harris, CB, Morris, T and Savage, G (2018) Collaborative facilitation in older couples: Successful joint remembering across memory tasks. Frontiers in Psychology 9, 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02385CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartlett, FC (1932) Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bicknell, K and Sutton, J (2022) Collaborative Embodied Performance: Ecologies of Skill. London: Bloomsbury.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, SD and Hoskins, A (2010) Terrorism in the new memory ecology: Mediating and remembering the 2005 London Bombings. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression 2(2), 87107. https://doi.org/10.1080/19434471003597399CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, SD and Reavey, P (2015) Turning around on experience: The ‘expanded view’ of memory within psychology. Memory Studies 8(2), 131150. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698014558660CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, SD and Reavey, P (2018) Embodiment and place in autobiographical remembering a relational-material approach. Journal of Consciousness Studies 25(7–8), 200224.Google Scholar
Carr, NG (2010) The Shallows: How the Internet Is Changing the Way We Think, Read and Remember. London: Atlantic.Google Scholar
Clark, A and Chalmers, D (1998) The extended mind. Analysis 58(1), 719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donald, M (1991) Origins of the Modern Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fawns, T (2020) Blended memory: A framework for understanding distributed autobiographical remembering with photography. Memory Studies 13(6), 901916. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698019829891CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fawns, T (2022) Cued recall: Using photo-elicitation to examine the distributed processes of remembering with photographs. Memory Studies. https://doi.org/10.1177/17506980211073093Google Scholar
Grall, C and Finn, ES (2022) Leveraging the power of media to drive cognition: A media-informed approach to naturalistic neuroscience. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsac019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halbwachs, M (1992) On Collective Memory. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, CB, Barnier, AJ, Sutton, J and Keil, PG (2014) Couples as socially distributed cognitive systems: Remembering in everyday social and material contexts. Memory Studies 7(3), 285297. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698014530619CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, CB, Barnier, AJ, Sutton, J and Savage, G (2018) Features of successful and unsuccessful collaborative memory conversations in long-married couples. Topics in Cognitive Science 11, 668686. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12350CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, CB, Sutton, J, Keil, PG, McIlwain, N, Harris, SA, Barnier, AJ, Savage, G and Dixon, RA (2022) Ageing together: Interdependence in the memory compensation strategies of long-married older couples. Frontiers in Psychology 13, 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.854051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassabis, D and Maguire, EA (2007) Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11(7), 299306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heersmink, R (2016) The internet, cognitive enhancement, and the values of cognition. Minds and Machines 26(4), 389407. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11023-016-9404-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henkel, LA (2014) Point-and-shoot memories: The influence of taking photos on memory for a museum tour. Psychological Science 25(2), 396402. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613504438CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsch, M (1997) Family Frames: Photography, Narrative, and Postmemory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hoskins, A (2011) Media, memory, metaphor: Remembering and the connective turn. Parallax 17(4), 1931. https://doi.org/10.1080/13534645.2011.605573CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoskins, A (2016) Memory ecologies. Memory Studies 9(3), 348357. https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698016645274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E (2010) Cognitive ecology. Topics in Cognitive Science 2(4), 705715. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01089.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, MK, Foley, MA, Suengas, AG and Raye, CL (1988) Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117(4), 371376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, MK, Hashtroudi, S and Lindsay, DS (1993) Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin 114(1), 328. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keightley, E and Pickering, M (2014) Technologies of memory: Practices of remembering in analogue and digital photography. New Media & Society 16(4), 576593. https://doi.org/10.1177/1461444814532062CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koriat, A, Goldsmith, M and Pansky, A (2000) Toward a psychology of memory accuracy. Annual Review of Psychology 51, 481537. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kvavilashvili, L and Ellis, J (2004) Ecological validity and the real-life/laboratory controversy in memory research: A critical and historical review. History & Philosophy of Psychology 6, 5980.Google Scholar
Landsberg, A (2004) Prosthetic Memory: The Transformation of American Remembrance in the Age of Mass Culture. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Mook, DG (2013) The myth of external validity. Everyday Cognition in Adulthood and Late Life 1984, 2543. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511759390.004Google Scholar
Neisser, U (1978) Memory: What are the important questions? In Gruneberg, MM, Morris, PE and Sykes, RN (eds), Practical Aspects of Memory. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, 319.Google Scholar
Schacter, DL (2022) Media, technology, and the sins of memory. Memory, Mind & Media. https://doi.org/10.1017/mem.2021.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sparrow, B, Liu, J and Wegner, DM (2011) Google effects on memory: Cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science 333(6043), 776778. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1207745CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, T, Addis, DR and Corballis, MC (2009) Mental time travel and the shaping of the human mind. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 364(1521), 13171324. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0301CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, J (2010) Exograms and interdisciplinarity: History, the extended mind, and the civilizing process. In Menary, R (ed), The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 189225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton, J, Harris, CB, Keil, PG and Barnier, AJ (2010) The psychology of memory, extended cognition, and socially distributed remembering. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9(4), 521560. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9182-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sutton, J, Bicknell, K and Harris, C (2020) The Wisconsin moment: A cognitive ethnography of collaborative recall experiments. Paper presented at the Conference on Interactivity, Language, & Cognition (CILC), 20 September, Warsaw.Google Scholar
Tribble, EB (2005) Distributing cognition in the globe. Shakespeare Quarterly 56(2), 135155. https://doi.org/10.1353/shq.2005.0065CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In Tulving, E and Donaldson, W (eds), Organization of Memory. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press, 381402.Google Scholar
Tulving, E (1982) Synergistic ecphory in recall and recognition. Canadian Journal of Psychology 36(2), 130147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E (1984) Précis of elements of episodic memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7(2), 223268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E (1991) Memory research is not a zero-sum game. American Psychologist 46(1), 4142. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.46.1.41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tulving, E (2002) Episodic memory: From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology 53(1), 125. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Dijck, J (2007) Mediated Memories in the Digital Age. Redwood City, CA: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Q (2019) Culture in the organization of autobiographical memory. In Mace, J (ed), The Organization and Structure of Autobiographical Memory. Oxford: Oxford Scholarship Online, 7292. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198784845.003.0005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegner, DM (1987) Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In Mullen, B and Goethals, GR (eds), Theories of Group Behavior. London: Springer, 185208. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-4634-3_9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winograd, E (1988) Continuities between ecological and laboratory approaches to memory. In Neisser, U and Winograd, E (eds), Remembering Reconsidered. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1120. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511664014.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar