Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T17:46:47.410Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The hidden power of implicit collective memory

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 October 2022

Astrid Erll*
Affiliation:
Goethe University Frankfurt, Frankfurt, Germany

Abstract

Over the past decades, the field of memory studies has produced a wealth of research on explicit (conscious, commemorative, official) collective memory. But beyond this realm of the visible, there is a largely hidden world of ‘implicit collective memory’. Elements of this invisible world include narrative schemata, stereotypes, patterns of framing, or world models, which are usually not explicitly known or addressed, but get passed on from generation to generation – in order to shape perception and action in new situations. Implicit collective memory is pervasive and powerful. But it is difficult to trace. It is therefore time to join forces for its systematic study: Drawing on approaches from psychology, sociology, communication studies, anthropology, media culture studies, literary studies, and mnemohistory, this article proposes some building blocks for a future transdisciplinary field of research on implicit collective memory.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

‘Unusual suspects’: moving beyond commemorative memory

‘Round up the Unusual Suspects!’ Vered Vinitzky-Seroussi's (Reference Vinitzky-Seroussi, Neiger, Zandberg and Meyers2011) exhortation is the starting point of this article. Certain forms of collective memory are by now very well-researched. Among the ‘usual suspects’ of interdisciplinary memory studies are forms of explicit, identity-creating, and often official commemoration – acts of memory in any case, which actors are aware of. Such conscious acts of memory are an important, visible and much-discussed part of memory culture. But they do not represent the entire range of possible relations between minds, media communication, and (social) environments.

In fact, as Michael Schudson (Reference Schudson1997, 3) remarked, conducting memory studies exclusively as commemoration studies is a bit like the drunk man who only looks for his car keys under the street lamp. In other words: ‘Not all of what societies remember is recalled through or in relation to self-conscious or dedicated memory projects. Instead, the past is often incorporated into the present in ways that do not aim at commemoration’ (Schudson Reference Schudson, Zelizer and Tenenboim-Weinblatt2014, 85). Schudson thus draws an important distinction between ‘commemorative memory’, on the one hand, and a whole variety of other forms of collective memory, which he subsumes under the term ‘non-commemorative memory’.

The present article builds on such calls to move beyond the predominance of memory studies as commemoration studies. For reasons of terminological precision and interdisciplinary interconnectability (outlined below), it suggests a distinction between explicit and implicit forms of collective memory. It proposes ‘implicit collective memory’ as a cover term for the myriad possibilities of the past affecting the present in ways that most people remain unaware of.

Memory studies have not even begun to systematically address the usually hidden but powerful dynamics of implicit collective memory. In what follows, I will discuss the role of media communication for processes of remembering that remain non-conscious on a collective level. And to show that implicit phenomena are (and have always been) a(n) (equally quite hidden) key concern of memory studies, I will engage with perspectives from a wide range of memory research in various disciplines.

What does ‘implicit memory’ mean? Psychological perspectives

In cognitive psychology, where the term was coined (Graf and Schacter Reference Graf and Schacter1985),‘implicit memory’ refers to processes of individual memory that subjects are not conscious of. Cognitive psychology posits different systems of memory. Endel Tulving (Reference Tulving1983) famously differentiated between semantic memory (‘knowing that’) and episodic memory (‘remembering’). Both are forms of explicit memory, which enables conscious (declarative, intentional) recall. Implicit memory, on the other hand, is behind all non-conscious acts of memory, which can range from procedural memories (i.e., for binding shoelaces or riding a bike, ‘knowing how’) all the way to effects of perceptual and conceptual priming (see Schacter Reference Schacter1987; on procedural vs. declarative memory, see Squire Reference Squire1987).Footnote 1

Priming is a standard psychological method to make implicit memory empirically observable in the laboratory. One typical example of (perceptual) priming research in cognitive psychology is the word-stem completion task, where participants are first shown a list of words and later asked to quickly generate words from the first three letters (so that ele__ could be completed into elephant, elevator, etc.). The result is that participants tend to resort to the words seen earlier, but without being aware of the connection. Importantly, amnesiacs, who cannot have conscious memory for the previously shown lists of words, perform on this task just as well as healthy people. (For an overview, see Roediger Reference Roediger1990) Put in a nutshell, when seen through the lens of priming implicit memory appears, in the words of cognitive psychologist Henry L. Roediger (Reference Roediger1990), as a form of ‘retention without remembering’. Similarly, social psychologist Don Carlston (Reference Carlston, Gawronski and Payne2010, 4) explains in the Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: ‘Although measured in a variety of ways, implicit memory has been defined fairly consistently as influences of past experience on later performance, in the absence of conscious memory for the earlier experience’.

In a broader memory studies perspective, there are some particularly relevant effects of priming, among them the ‘mere exposure effect’ (Bornstein and Craver-Lemley Reference Bornstein, Craver-Lemley and Pohl2017), and the ‘validity effect’ (Renner Reference Renner and Pohl2017), i.e., the fact that what is seen and heard again and again, often without awareness, seems familiar, is liked, and is even considered true. Behavioural economist Daniel Kahneman (Reference Kahneman2011, 85) explains: ‘A reliable method to bring people to believe in false statements is frequent repetition, because familiarity cannot easily be distinguished from truth’. Priming can also lead to ‘mental contamination’ (Schacter Reference Schacter2002, 301) by sexist and racist stereotypes as well as to non-conscious forms of plagiarism – cryptomnesia.Footnote 2

One reason for all these effects is that implicit memory involves source amnesia. The source of a piece of information and its status (is it someone's lived experience or hearsay? Is the source reliable or unreliable, fictional or factual?) do not seem to play a great role whenever items are encoded and activated non-intentionally. What is retained in the framework of implicit memory feels familiar (pleasant, natural, true) and can even assume the subjective, ‘episodic’ quality of personal experience.Footnote 3

In a psychological perspective, implicit memory is an apparatus of automatic, ‘fast thinking’, as Kahneman calls it in Thinking, Fast and Slow (Reference Kahneman2011). He posits a psychological ‘double process model’ and distinguishes between controlled (conscious, intentional) and automatic (non-conscious) forms of human information processing. For the social psychologist John Bargh, automatic cognitive processes have four characteristics: ‘lack of awareness, lack of intentionality, lack of controllability, and high efficiency (nonreliance on cognitive resources)’ (Bargh Reference Bargh, Wyer and Srull1994; Carlston Reference Carlston, Gawronski and Payne2010, 40).

‘High efficiency’ – this is a central attribute of implicit memory. At the same time, such powerful ‘fast’ forms of memory often remain entirely hidden. They are non-conscious, non-intentional, and uncontrollable. This is why, in his now-classic introduction to the psychology and neuroscience of memory, Searching for Memory (Reference Schacter1996, 161), Daniel Schacter writes about ‘the hidden world of implicit memory’. Moving on to the broader field of memory studies, and with a nod to Schacter, I point to ‘the hidden power of implicit collective memory’, thus accentuating not only the invisibility, but also the forward-pushing, future-making capacities of implicit collective memory.

A hidden power: implicit memory as a collective phenomenon

But how can we start thinking about implicit memory as a collective phenomenon? To begin with, it is worth recalling what is meant by ‘remembering’ and ‘forgetting’, when these processes are understood not just as phenomena of individual psychology but seen in their (actual) distribution across assemblages of biological, mental, sociocultural, and material elements. For memory studies, ‘memory’ exists only in this extended form as an ‘ecology of memory’ (Hoskins Reference Hoskins2016; see also Sutton et al Reference Sutton, Harris, Keil and Barnier2010). ‘Collective memory’ is therefore a tautology. I use the term only to be clear and also with a nod to a terminological tradition going back to Maurice Halbwachs (Reference Halbwachs1925), who claimed that all memory is always already collective memory.Footnote 4

Collective remembering does not mean that all individuals would have identical mental representations in their minds. Instead, it means that certain versions of the past are actualised again and again within social groups (via discourses, media, practices), and that they are well-networked with other topics. Similarly, collective forgetting does not mean, as Guy Beiner (Reference Beiner2018) has shown, that all traces, all knowledge of a past event would be lost. It means that within certain social frameworks, there are no acts of remembering traceable. For example, memories of particular past events can be avoided, kept secret, tabooed, or may seem difficult to articulate in public. But often, these events will ‘live on’ as familial or local memories – or within the semantic collective memory of science (as has been the case with the Spanish Flu, see Beiner Reference Beiner2022). The logic of collective memory thus implies, in Barry Schwartz’ (Reference Schwartz2009, 23) words, that ‘remembering and forgetting are distributed unevenly among different communities, groups, and individuals’.

Implicit collective memory has to be conceptualised according to this logic, too. What the majority of people remain unaware of can be quite obvious to some observers: Cultural stereotypes, tabooed pasts, emotional regimes, non-conscious master narratives – for some people, such as newcomers to a society or critical observers, the daily activations and effects of implicit collective memory immediately catch the eye. Certain actors will moreover work quite intentionally with implicit collective memory: journalists using framing techniques; politicians who play with historical allusions; film industries and advertising agencies who exploit cultural schemata and narrative habits. Last not least, it belongs to the tasks of critical media, art, and literature to make the dynamics of implicit collective memory visible – and thus potentially transformable.Footnote 5

How can we then draw a distinction between forms of memory that are collectively non-conscious and those that belong to explicit knowledge? Here as elsewhere in memory studies functional definitions are key: Within social groups, the explicit and the implicit are particular usages and effects of collective memory, and not separate systems. (Whether forms of explicit and implicit memory are located within different regions of the brain and thus dissociable is still a question under discussion; see Carlston Reference Carlston, Gawronski and Payne2010.) This means that certain memorata can be the object of knowledge or official commemoration at one point of time, while at another point of time they unfold their world-making power in an unnoticed way.

A striking example is the presence of colonial monuments or racist forms of speaking in post-colonial societies (for their role in Brexit, see Ward and Rasch Reference Ward and Rasch2019). Such ‘colonial remains’ can stay implicit – unnoticed by most – for a long time, until their power of stabilising and transmitting forms of (direct and structural) violence across generations is exposed. In this way, memory activists (such as Black Lives Matter) have made explicit in recent years what had long been a powerful, but largely implicit presence (see Otele et al Reference Otele, Gandolfo and Galai2021; Rigney Reference Rigney, Gutman and Wüstenberg2022). In our self-reflective memory cultures of what Ulrich Beck (Reference Beck2006) has termed ‘second’ or ‘reflexive modernity’, the transformation of implicit collective memory into explicit knowledge and commemoration has been a key concern. This operation was already at the heart of Victor Klemperer's (Reference Klemperer2013 [1947]) analysis of the language of the ‘Third Reich’ (LTI, lingua tertii imperii) and its detrimental non-conscious afterlives. Another example of a transforming agent between implicit and explicit collective memory is all feminist work that exposes and seeks to rectify the (let's hopefully say: often) non-conscious gender biases of historiography and science (Reading Reference Reading2016).

But how is implicit collective memory transmitted from generation to generation when it is never explicitly addressed? Schematisation seems to be a powerful process in the travels of implicit collective memory. Examples of schematised memorata are visual icons, narrative patterns, stereotypes, metaphors, world models, values and norms, or certain ways of acting or ‘doing’ things (on habit memory, see Connerton Reference Connerton1989).Footnote 6 They are often loaded with affect. Think of the discursive formula ‘dying for the fatherland’, the millennia-old iconic image of the Pietà, the binary of light-versus-darkness, the narrative pattern of ‘rise and fall’, the stereotype of the ‘yellow peril’ (actualised again in the Coronavirus pandemic). Such potentially perilous packages of schema-cum-affect are passed on – often (though not exclusively) non-consciously – from human to human, from generation to generation. Media in all their forms and appearances play a decisive role in the process: Implicit collective memory is mediated and remediated via gesture and mimics, via orality and literacy, via analogue and digital media (on remediation as the process of conveying memory contents again and again, across time, cultures, and different media, see Erll and Rigney Reference Erll and Rigney2009).

With implicit collective memory, I describe the recurrent use of mostly schematised memorata, which remains – for a majority of the group or society – not intentional, non-conscious, and not visible. As the examples above show, with the collective dynamics of non-awareness come questions of ethics. What responsibilities arise from not-remembering and not-knowing? (On ‘agnotology’, see Proctor and Schiebinger Reference Proctor and Schiebinger2008). Such ethical questions can be addressed with Michael Rothberg's (Reference Rothberg2019) concept of the ‘implicated subject’, which helps describe actors’ differentiated ability and willingness to be aware of the past and its continuous presence. The accent of the present intervention, however, is on the generative power of implicit memory: while remaining unexposed, it is likely to produce more of the same in the future.

How has research in different disciplines addressed the phenomena that I suggest bringing and thinking together under the banner of ‘implicit collective memory’? In what follows, I will discuss narrative templates, media framing and priming, and premediation as concepts for the study of implicit memory – thus staging a dialogue between psychology, anthropology, sociology, communication studies, media culture studies, and mnemohistorical research.

A perspective from anthropology: National narratives

National narratives are a striking example of the hidden power of implicit collective memory.Footnote 7 In How Nations Remember (Reference Wertsch2021), anthropologist James Wertsch studied the differences in national narrative templates between Russia and the United States. Collaborating with cognitive psychologists on an interdisciplinary survey study (Abel et al Reference Abel, Umanath, Fairfield, Takahashi, Roediger and Wertsch2019), Wertsch and his colleagues could show how Russian narrative templates about the Second World War are fundamentally different from those not only of the US, but also most other nations. Abel et al (Reference Abel, Umanath, Fairfield, Takahashi, Roediger and Wertsch2019) asked more than 100 people from each of 11 countries to state what they thought were the ten most important events of the Second World War. The results are striking: Even in China and Japan, subjects produced items that are largely in consensus with the American perspective on the Second World War. Among the most important events range Pearl Harbor, D-Day, the Holocaust, and Hiroshima and Nagasaki. In Russia, however, the results are very different: With an extraordinary level of consensus (and in combination with the greatest general knowledge about the war, which was also tested), Russian subjects usually did not mention Pearl Harbor or the dropping of atomic bombs, but came up with the following core set of events: The Battle of Stalingrad, the Battle of Kursk, the Siege of Leningrad, the Battle of Moscow, the German invasion of the USSR, and the Battle of Berlin.

How can such differences be explained? And what holds this stable set of remembered events together? Analysing Putin's uses of historical memory, Wertsch (Reference Wertsch2021, 25) argues that these are basic elements of a story that ‘positions Russia as a victim of attacks by alien enemies’. Importantly, this is a narrative pattern that most people in Russia do not (always) seem to be aware of. But it guides not only the way they select remembered events, but also how they imagine the future. According to Wertsch, for Putin the narrative template of Russian victimhood is a ‘fast cognitive tool’: ‘It is a narrative tool that could almost be said to be doing some of his thinking and speaking for him’ (Wertsch Reference Wertsch2021, 24). Wertsch's book came out just a couple of months before Russia's invasion of Ukraine. It shows not only the extent to which the war in Ukraine is a war of differing collective memories, but also how memory studies contributes to a better understanding of how implicit memory operates through narrative and can be used and abused as part of aggressive politics.

But how do such largely invisible narrative templates circulate in society? How are they passed on across decades and centuries? In the case of Russia, Wertsch (Reference Wertsch2021, 14) discerns a dynamics where the template of national victimhood permeates major societal spheres and communication media (from family conversations to textbooks to popular culture and the national press). But what about the striking transnational consensus on the ‘American narrative’? With regard to subjects in Germany, Italy, or China, it remains a conundrum why they should remember ‘Pearl Harbor’ and not, say, the ‘Battle of Kursk’. At least part of the answer must lie in globalising media culture, where American historical narratives have been widely disseminated via Hollywood films such as Pearl Harbor (2001, dir. Michael Bay). The crucial point here is that their enormous influence on viewers’ images of history remains, in the sense of a ‘collective source amnesia’, largely unknown to audiences and under-researched in media studies (on the logic of the ‘memory film’, see Erll Reference Erll, Erll and Nünning2010, Reference Erll2012).

What remains equally unknown to most people is the selectivity, narrativity, and perspectivity of their own images of history. Wertsch (Reference Wertsch2021, 13) emphasises that ‘narrative tools often operate under the radar of conscious reflection, leaving us with the impression that we have a direct, unmediated picture of reality’. He reminds us of the words of narrative psychologist Jerome Bruner: ‘Common sense stoutly holds that the story form is a transparent window on reality, not a cookie cutter imposing a shape on it’ (Bruner Reference Bruner2003, 6–7). Most of the time, we do not even see which cutter we hold in our hands. But the fact that we successfully used it yesterday and the day before makes it more likely that we will use it tomorrow, too. This is the forward-facing power of implicit collective memory.

Perspectives from sociology and communication studies: Framing and priming

As we've seen, anthropology and cultural studies can contribute their insights about narrative patterns to the project of tracing the hidden power of implicit collective memory. Promising perspectives from sociologyFootnote 8 and communication studies include research on framing and priming. While communication studies work on priming is based on the psychological research outlined above, the concept of framing has its own tradition in sociology, going back all the way to Erving Goffman, Emile Durkheim, and Georg Simmel. Metaphors of the frame and framing are used in many different ways today: In communication studies (as ‘media frames’), in political philosophy (Judith Butler's ‘frames of war’, Reference Butler2009), and last but not least in memory studies, where Halbwachs’ cadres sociaux (social frameworks) remains a key concept.

For Erving Goffman, the founding figure of sociological frame analysis, ‘framing’ means ‘the organization of experience’ (Goffman Reference Goffman1986 [1974], 11). ‘Observers actively project their frames of reference into the world immediately around them’ (Goffman Reference Goffman1986 [1974], 39). Such mental frames are acquired in the socialisation process. The production of realities by means of framing is an active process, but not necessarily a conscious one. On the contrary, our functioning in the everyday world is contingent on our automatic usage of frames.

This understanding resonates strongly with Maurice Halbwachs’ theory of collective memory. The concept of the frame was so important to Halbwachs that he devoted an entire monograph to it: Les cadres sociaux de la mémoire (Reference Halbwachs1925, The social frameworks of memory). Halbwachs’ metaphor of the cadres sociaux merges into one term the social and mental properties of the ‘frame’: It is via the people who surround us (French: cadres sociaux) that we acquire our socially shaped mental frames, scaffolds, or schemata of remembering (again, French: cadres sociaux). But these twofold frameworks not only enable and shape acts of remembering. They also – and this is what Goffman will accentuate 50 years later – lead the understanding of new experience along certain paths. For Halbwachs, frames remain largely a hidden power. In La mémoire collective (Reference Halbwachs1997 [1950], 70), he states that a ‘social current of thought’ is ‘ordinarily as invisible as the air that we breathe. In normal life we recognise its existence only when we resist it’ (my translation).

Being a sociologist, Halbwachs directs his attention to the social dimension of memory. But in a hypothetical anecdote about a ‘walk through London’, he imagines how media such as the words of his architect friend or Charles Dickens's novels shape his current experience as he is walking through the city (Halbwachs Reference Halbwachs1997 [1950], 52–53). Halbwachs thus exemplifies something that media memory studies (e.g., Edy Reference Edy2006; Erll Reference Erll2011, 129) have emphasised again and again: Social frames are mediated phenomena. They are medial frames, cadres médiaux. Social relations and meanings are constituted and transmitted through oral speech, letters, or books; and in the age of digital media they have acquired an intrinsically mediated form (Hoskins Reference Hoskins2018; van Dijck Reference Van Dijck2007). Medial frames both shape collective remembering of the past and have a forward-facing power, as we see in Halbwachs’ anecdote, where media preform his perception of London. It can be supposed that in everyday life (i.e., without the sociologist's introspection) they will do so in a mainly non-conscious way.

In the late twentieth century, communication studies adopted the concept of framing in order to understand the logic of the news. For Robert M. Entman (Reference Entman1993, 52), ‘[f]raming essentially involves selection and salience. To frame is to select some aspects of a perceived reality and make them more salient in a communicating text in such a way as to promote a particular problem definition, causal interpretation, moral evaluation, and/or treatment recommendation for the item described’. In this usage, framing becomes a conscious strategy on the production side. But its power nonetheless resides in the non-conscious effects of news framings on the reception side.

Framing and priming have become key concepts of communication studies and media effects research (see Lecheler and De Vreese Reference Lecheler and de Vreese2019; Roskos-Ewoldsen et al Reference Roskos-Ewoldsen, Roskos-Ewoldsen, Dillman Carpentier, Bryant and Oliver2009; Tewksbury and Scheufele Reference Tewksbury, Scheufele, Bryant and Oliver2009). The two terms describe different phenomena involved in the dynamics of implicit collective memory. Both are metaphors. John Sonnett (Reference Sonnett, Brekhus and Ignatow2019, 227) explains that ‘framing’ is based on a spatial and visual metaphor (the framing or arranging of paintings), while ‘priming’ is based on a temporal and sequential metaphor (only after the presentation of a piece of information can certain effects come to pass). Therefore, research on framing is typically interested in the ‘how’ of communication, while research on priming asks about the ‘what’.

Vincent Price and David Tewksbury (Reference Price, Tewksbury, Barnett and Boster1997) differentiate between ‘applicability’ and ‘accessibility’. ‘Applicability’ is concerned with the semantic uses and usability of a frame (how?), while ‘accessibility’ points to the temporally restricted activation potentials of a prime (what? when?) (see also Scheufele Reference Scheufele1999). In communication studies, framing is thus about the ways in which mediations can shape and change perceiving and understanding, while priming is about the automatic activation of non-consciously existent memorata, which may include frames. Media can prime people on certain frames.

The challenge: Implicit collective memory across the longue durée

Psychology, sociology, and communication studies have shown that framing and priming are social and medial phenomena, and that they remain implicit for most of the actors involved. The possibility of linking these approaches with memory studies depends on questions of time: It is only when their power to frame and to prime unfolds long-term effects that mediations can shape collective memory. But the problem is that in most psychological experiments priming effects are counted merely in minutes and hours.Footnote 9

Communication studies have tried to model the temporal stability of frames as ‘chronic accessibility’, which can be heightened by ‘frequent priming’ or ‘repetitive framing’ (Roskos-Ewoldsen et al Reference Roskos-Ewoldsen, Roskos-Ewoldsen, Dillman Carpentier, Bryant and Oliver2009, 83). Christian Baden and Sophie Lecheler (Reference Baden and Lecheler2012, 359) have made an important foray into the theoretical modelling of the duration of framing effects. They emphasise, much in the sense of memory studies, that ‘the social relevance of framing effects hinges upon their ability to persist’. But empirical research on media framing so far has only provided evidence for effects that last 10 days to a maximum of 3 weeks (Schemer Reference Schemer, Schweiger and Fahr2013, 161).

From a memory studies perspective, this sounds sobering. Vastly different temporal horizons are at stake in the study of collective memory. Memory studies is concerned with the question of how medial (pre)formations can exert effects over years, decades, centuries, even millennia – and that means, not only across the life spans of individual subjects, but also across multiple generational thresholds: the longue durée of collective memory.Footnote 10

The narrative patterns of the Odyssey or of Exodus, the iconic formula of Pietà, as well as antisemitic stereotypes have quite obviously had ‘chronic accessibility’ over long periods of time. But what connections must be made to turn the psychological, sociological, and communications studies concepts discussed here into useful tools for the study of the long-term dynamics of collective memory?

Two aspects need to be taken into account. First, examples ranging from Homeric myths and their narrative templates to Christian iconography all the way to tenacious stereotypes and conspiracy theories show that long-term memorata are always built-up plurimedially (see Erll Reference Erll, Erll and Nünning2010). They are transmedial phenomena, remediated again and again across the spectrum of available media. This is, second, a social process through and through: Interaction, collaboration, dialogue, negotiation, agonism – the entire spectrum of the dynamics of social memory-making needs to be taken into account here. Psychological research on implicit memory, on the contrary, tends to focus solely on individual memory performance and not on the question of how memorata can be shared and thus become part of collective memory. More generally, in mainstream psychology, social interaction tends to be seen as ‘memory contamination’, rather than (as in the field of memory studies) as a means of ‘memory production’.Footnote 11

Implicit collective memory is produced and passed on in complex social and plurimedial constellations – often across the longue durée. This insight opens up an entirely new range of questions: How do such constellations come into existence? How is it that certain framings become strong primes, preforming action again and again in social groups? What media technologies and genres tend to have enough authority and power to disseminate frames and make them appear ‘applicable’? What forms of institutionalisation, canonisation, dissemination, educational politics, or marketing make the ‘chronic accessibility’ of certain primes possible? Such questions necessitate the combination of psychological memory studies with the conceptual toolbox, archives, and methods of media culture studies and mnemohistory. An example of such a combination will be presented in the following section.

Perspectives from mnemohistory: Remediating and premediating ‘Mutiny’

European post-colonial memory cultures remain a strong residue of implicit collective memory. European societies are replete with mostly non-conscious and unacknowledged afterlives of its Empires – all across Portugal, Spain, Italy, France, Belgium, the Netherlands, the UK, Denmark, and Germany. This is why research fields such as post-colonial studies and new imperial history have a lot to offer the study of implicit collective memory (Bijl Reference Bijl2016; Craps Reference Craps2013; Rothermund Reference Rothermund2015; Schwarz Reference Schwarz2011). Research on ‘post-colonial melancholia’ (Gilroy Reference Gilroy2004), ‘colonial durabilities’ (Stoler Reference Stoler2016), or ‘embers of empire’ (Ward and Rasch Reference Ward and Rasch2019) address the ways in which implicit legacies of colonialism (affective, archival, discursive) continue to shape mentalities and guide political action long after decolonisation.

Part of these largely invisible and unspoken legacies are ways of framing certain events of colonial history. In Germany, for example, for over a century the Herero and Nama Genocide of 1904–1908 (in what is now Namibia) had been framed as merely a ‘small war’ in a ‘short German colonial history’ (de Wolff Reference de Wolff2021). The case I discuss in this section concerns British imperial and post-imperial memory, where an ‘insurgent frame’ has lingered for at least two centuries. This frame has many mnemohistorical sources, but one particular important genealogy points back to the Indian rebellion of 1857/8.

The press coverage of the 1857/8 rebellion in northern India is a case in point for the potential tenacity of initial press framings, and for the quite seamless migration of frames from colonial to post-colonial times. The rebellion brought together Indian soldiers, farmers and princes of different ethnicities and religions against the British, and it was so successful that it almost cost them their Raj. The British called the rebellion a ‘Mutiny’ (it had indeed sprung from colonial soldiers’ uprising) and thus inserted a powerful framing into British imperial mnemohistory that still reverberates today. The term ‘mutiny’ implies an unlawful uprising as well as an event that is restricted to the military. Both interpretations of the events of 1857/8 are debatable.

Anglo-Indian and British press texts that immediately covered the ‘mutiny’ for the imperial metropolis worked with selections, highlighting, word choices, and narrative structures that could still be felt decades, even a century later in the ways in which the years 1857/8 were presented in British historiography and across broader media culture. The first reports printed in The London Times about the rebellion in Northern India were real or feigned eyewitnesses accounts. Their framing is unequivocally one-sided: The rebellion was cast as a perfidious mutiny of ungrateful, religiously fanatical, and cruel subjects against the just colonial rule of the unsuspecting and benevolent British. Atrocity stories became the most powerful genre in the ‘Mutiny’ coverage. The rape and killing of British women and children were specifically highlighted and embroidered with gory details. In 1857, Karl Marx, who offered an alternative framing of the events as the ‘first Indian war of independence’, had already exposed one of the ‘eye-witnesses’ featured in The Times as a liar (Sharpe Reference Sharpe1993, 66). However, the early British press framings and their narrative plots migrated untarnished, sometimes word-for-word, first into imperial historiography, then into English novels and theatre of the nineteenth century, early cinema, as well as – after Indian independence 1947 – into post-imperial historiography, fiction, and television.

Those ‘Mutiny’-frames erupted again with a vengeance in 2005 in a debate around a Bollywood movie of the rebellion (Mangal Pandey: The Rising, 2005, dir. Ketan Mehta). In unison with a diverse range of newspapers, and backed by the comments of eminent British historians, the Daily Mail (19 August 2005) criticised the movie as ‘fanatically anti-British’. ‘Fanatical’ – for one and a half century, this had been a standard attribute to frame resistance against British colonial rule. Under the workings of implicit collective memory, a filmic twentieth-century rendition of the ‘Mutiny’ seems to have turned into yet another mutiny.Footnote 12

What I can only sketch here (the whole story is set out in Erll Reference Erll2007) is the dynamics of remediation in memory culture – the transcription of memorata into ever-changing new media, a process, in which traces (here: framings) of older mediations travel along, often unheeded, across potentially very long stretches of time. Remediations are a vital agent in the dynamic plurimedial constellations which emerge around remembered events and keep them on the agenda of memory culture.

With time, remediations lead to mnemonic premediation: Much-repeated frames, narrative schemata, or visual patterns become ‘household items’ of a media culture. They can become detached (unlocked, unbound) from the stories they were originally used to convey, and reattached to new experience, which they then medially preform (i.e., shape even before the events take place).Footnote 13 Think of Bruner's cookie cutters.

In the case of the ‘Mutiny’, such mnemonically preformed events can include debates about the relative merits of a Bollywood drama. But more deadly, among the events that were quite possibly premediated by ‘Mutiny’-memory is the Amritsar massacre of 1919, a decisive turning point in the Indian independence movement of the early twentieth century. Facing a crowd of largely peaceful protesters, Colonel Dyer of the British Indian Army had his soldiers open fire and kill hundreds of Indians, who could not escape from the enclosed compound of the Jallianwala Bagh, where they had gathered. In a perceptive book about the ‘shadows of the Mutiny’ in the British Empire, The Other Side of the Medal (Reference Thompson1925, 53–54), historian Edward John Thompson surmised that the massacre was a knee-jerk reaction, the result of ‘inherited thought concerning the Mutiny’, and evidence of ‘the workings of imperfectly informed minds obsessed with’ the stories about Indian atrocities of 70 years earlier.Footnote 14 Seen from hindsight, the framings of mid-nineteenth century media culture were thus still palpable in the early twentieth century, and they are still active today, 150 years later. Surely, they were themselves the result of centuries-old dynamics of premediation. Already in the eighteenth century, Indian peasant uprisings had been framed by British colonisers as outpourings of religious fanaticism (see Guha Reference Guha1983).

‘Implicitness’ on a collective level does not mean ignorance of all. Perhaps some imperial historians used the gory and one-sided early press framings of the ‘Mutiny’ quite deliberately. Perhaps some journalists today critically revisit the old press archives. And perhaps some are astutely aware of the plurimedial mnemohistory of the rebellion and understand the logic of the ‘Mutiny cookie cutter’. But what is crucial is that the majority of people in British post-imperial memory culture are not aware of the chains (or better, cascades) of imperial remediations. They suffer from what might be termed collective source amnesia.

Thinking and talking about colonialism, actors in post-colonial memory cultures (not just in Britain, but also, say, in Germany or Russia) are often ‘thought and spoken’ by implicit mnemohistories. A word choice like ‘fanatic’ for the description of colonial subjects taking collective action may thus feel natural to some people – and in the logic of implicit memory therefore also ‘true’. These are ‘mere exposure effects’ and ‘validity effects’ on a collective level – effects which are prepared in the longue durée, by the self-reinforcing dynamics of much (re)mediated frames.

For mnemohistorical research, framing and priming are thus very relevant concepts. But they have to be ‘translated’ according to different dynamics on different scales of the complex ecologies of collective memory: for example, into ‘remediation’ (as a form of plurimedial repetitive framing in the longue durée) and into ‘mnemonic premediation’ (as a form of media priming, which is based on longue durée-frames). Mnemonic premediation has effects, on the one hand, on individual minds (fast accessibility and seeming ‘fitness’ of a frame for many people in memory culture). On the other hand, mnemonic premediation does become visible (and thus researchable) in mediations, when certain discernible framings, schemata, or narrative patterns migrate to new topics.

Perspectives for memory studies: Beyond the street lamp

What are the consequences of the hidden power of implicit collective memory for interdisciplinary memory research? Implicit collective memory remains largely an invisible agent. In everyday life, most people will neither notice its power to shape perception and action nor realise that there are sometimes enormous differences between implicit repertoires of different social groups. Think of Russian and ‘Western’ World War II narratives (Abel et al Reference Abel, Umanath, Fairfield, Takahashi, Roediger and Wertsch2019). Research into implicit collective memory means making visible what remains invisible to most people – and thus moving out of the (academically quite safe) light cone of a street lamp that illuminates only explicit (and mostly commemorative) memory.

Implicit collective memory is, contrary to commemoration, not primarily backward-looking, but fundamentally a preforming, a forward-facing dynamics. In cognitive psychology, priming is defined as the influence of past experience on subsequent action. A mnemohistorical perspective shows that new experience and action is often mnemonically premediated, i.e., implicitly preformed by the mediations of (sometimes longue durée) collective memories, in which subjects participate. What is ultimately at stake with the term of implicit collective memory is the social, medial, and for most people non-conscious aspects of a cultural remembering-imagining system (Conway et al Reference Conway, Loveday and Cole2016). Implicit collective memory is a form of ‘collective future thinking without thinking’.Footnote 15

What significance does existent research on memory cultures (sensu A Assmann Reference Assmann2011 and J Assmann Reference Assmann2011) have for the study of implicit collective memory? It is indispensable, because the non-conscious is mostly a long-term effect of active memory culture and its key processes: remediation, canonisation, institutionalisation, commemoration, and the wide dissemination of certain memorata. It is not surprising that some of the most influential long-term frames spring from canonised texts such as the Bible, from frequently remediated national myths, much-used textbooks, or from globally disseminated Hollywood movies. The resources of implicit collective memory emerge from explicit memory culture.

The term ‘implicit collective memory’ is proposed here as an umbrella term for a wide array of phenomena, which all share a certain onto-epistemological mode (they derive from collective memory and most people remain unaware of them) and a function (they preform thought and action). The term aims to bring together research from different quarters and traditions in order to study a transdisciplinary phenomenon. Among these traditions are not only those discussed in this article: psychological, sociological, and communication studies approaches to priming and framing; anthropological approaches to national narratives; mnemohistorical research on remediation and premediation. Implicit collective memory is moreover discernible as one of the key concerns of social movement studies (Lorenzo Zamponi's ‘repertoires’, Reference Zamponi2018), research on conspiracy myths (van Prooijen and Douglas Reference Van Prooijen and Douglas2017), recent discussions in the philosophy of history about the ‘presence’ of the past (Bevernage and Lorenz, Reference Bevernage and Lorenz2013), as well as post-colonial and decolonial debates about the continuities of imperial practices and forms of thinking (Stoler Reference Stoler2016). Last not least, the implicit poses a key challenge to theories of new media. It has been addressed as ‘the digital unconscious’ (Monk Reference Monk and Wood1998) and is part of what Andrew Hoskins (Hoskins and Halstead Reference Hoskins and Halstead2021) calls a ‘new grey in digital memory’.

The preoccupation with implicit forms of collective memory can boast a long tradition – perhaps an even longer one than the study of commemorative memory. A key figure is surely by Warburg (Reference Warburg2000 [1924]), whose understanding of the afterlives of antiquity gestures far beyond a deliberate recourse to a ‘classical tradition’. Warburg describes the workings of visual ‘pathos formula’ as a non-conscious activation of older forms and affects in new artworks. As we have seen, Halbwachs was also interested in invisible ‘social currents’. And Frederic Bartlettt's (Reference Bartlett1932) foundational research in experimental psychology is fundamental, too, for an understanding of non-conscious narrative schemata that seem specific to particular (memory) cultures.Footnote 16

For many humanities scholars, the ‘collective non-conscious’ may be suggestive of the psychoanalytic ‘unconscious’ and the ways it has been used for the description of social processes. Well-known examples include CG Jung's (Reference Jung2014) quite problematic ‘collective unconscious’, a concept developed in the early 1900s, as well as Sigmund Freud's mass psychology (Reference Freud2004 [1921], Reference Freud2010 [1939]). In memory studies, psychoanalytical thought has long played a key role in attempts to describe the dynamics of difficult non-conscious collective memory – all the way from Theodor W. Adorno's deliberations on the afterlife of fascism (Adorno Reference Adorno1977 [1959]) to Marianne Hirsch's (Reference Hirsch2012) ‘postmemory’ as a form of non-intentional transgenerational transmission of traumatic memory (for psychoanalytic approaches to historical consciousness, see Straub and Rüsen (Reference Straub and Rüsen2011 [1998]).

Why ‘implicit collective memory’, and not ‘the collective unconscious’? My use of a technical term coming from cognitive psychology is meant to act as a reminder that there is no easy equation between the Freudian unconscious, on the one hand, and the social effects of past experience and media reception that most people remain unaware of, on the other. It moreover directs attention to a possible dialogue between memory studies and recent cognitive science approaches to the ‘new unconscious’.Footnote 17 Moreover, applied in a conversation with post-human studies, the term opens up the possibility of studying the bio-technological co-production of forms of non-conscious memory.Footnote 18

But the essential rationale for the term ‘implicit collective memory’ is pragmatic: As Daniel Schacter reminds us, the ‘nonconscious world of implicit memory revealed by cognitive neuroscience differs markedly from the Freudian unconscious’, because it is ‘far more mundane’ than the Freudian drama about the forces of repression. Implicit memories ‘arise as a natural consequence of such everyday activities as perceiving, understanding, and acting’ (Schacter Reference Schacter1996, 190–191). What is at stake is an understanding of the quantitatively most frequent memory processes. Implicit memory – in its cognitive as well as in its social or medial instantiations – is not a pathology, but a basic and ubiquitous dynamics that first of all enables memory ecologies to function. What is therefore needed is a better understanding of the hidden power of everyday, automatic forms of collective remembering across its various dimensions. What is not needed is a playoff between, say, psychoanalysis and the cognitive sciences. Instead, only a joining of forces from different quarters will enable us to make sense of the invisible phenomena of collective remembering – of those ‘unusual suspects’, which are not found under the Freudian, or any other, single street lamp.

Upon closer inspection, implicit collective memory has in fact already emerged as a key concern of present-day interdisciplinary memory studies – but in a characteristically unexamined way. Jeffrey Olick's (Reference Olick2016, 60) studies on the path-dependence of social memory points in this direction, as does Robyn Fivush and Azriel Grysman's (Reference Fivush and Grysman2022) distinction between explicit and implicit gendered narratives, or Barbie Zelizer's (Reference Zelizer2022) discussion of framing as part of ‘journalism's backstage’. Implicit collective memory is, in the words of Eviatar Zerubavel (Reference Zerubavel2008, Reference Zerubavel2015), the ‘elephant in the room’ of memory studies, so far remaining ‘hidden in plain sight’.

The greatest challenge of research on implicit collective memory is of a methodological kind: How can we make hidden phenomena graspable, and thus researchable? Perhaps, to begin with, by creating connections between the rich methodological repertoires that already exist in memory studies across its diverse disciplines and that range from experimental and quantitative methods all the way to archival, discourse-analytic and narratological approaches and finally to the possibilities that digital humanities now open up. The conundrums of implicit memory phenomena provide one more reason to turn the multidisciplinary field of memory studies into a site of intensified interdisciplinary collaboration.

Data availability statement

As this is a theoretical article, there are no data involved, except the quotations, which are referenced.

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Henry L. Roediger for his critical reading and many helpful suggestions.

Funding support

This work received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Footnotes

1 See also Schacter (Reference Schacter1996). For a use of memory systems to describe mental representations of collective memory, see Manier and Hirst (Reference Manier, Hirst, Erll and Nünning2010). I have applied these categories to further distinguish between different social functions of collective memory. According to this model (Erll Reference Erll2011, 108), there are two explicit forms of collective memory: first, ‘collective-episodic (or: autobiographical) memory’, which means that memorata are related to time and collective identity (often, but not exclusively, in the mode of commemorative memory), and, second, ‘collective-semantic memory’, which refers to the production of cultural knowledge and accentuates its temporal dimension (For sociology of knowledge approaches to memory, see the entries in Olick et al Reference Olick, Vinitzky-Seroussi and Levy2010; Savelsberg Reference Savelsberg2021).

2 A caveat may be in place here: The more spectacular research on ‘social’ or ‘behavioural’ priming, as it was conducted in the 1980s and 1990s (e.g., Bargh's et al Reference Bargh, Chen and Burrows1996 ‘Florida experiment’ where students walked slower after being primed on words connected to old age), has now come under severe criticism as many of its findings could not be replicated (see Chivers Reference Chivers2019). On the one hand, there is now a call in psychology for greater methodological rigour when studying what is seen as universal mental patterns. On the other hand, there is an emerging insight that certain priming effects occur only in subsets of people (Chivers Reference Chivers2019, 202). This latter point resonates well with memory studies’ emphasis on the social situatedness and particular ecologies of each act of memory.

3 ‘The experience of familiarity has a simple but powerful quality of “pastness” that seems to indicate that it is a direct reflection of prior experience’ (Whittlesea, Jacoby and Girard Reference Whittlesea, Jacoby and Girard1990, 716).

4 Reformulated in the terms of current posthumanism: ‘Memory’ emerges as dynamic co-construction (or ‘sympoiesis’, Haraway Reference Haraway2019) and as an effect of distributed agency (Bennett Reference Bennett2010).

5 On Joyce's Ulysses, see Erll (Reference Erll2019); on the agency of the aesthetic in general, see Rigney (Reference Rigney2021).

6 On the role of narrative schemata for memory, see Bartlett (Reference Bartlett1932; Wagoner Reference Wagoner2017) and Bruner (Reference Bruner2003); on ‘metaphors we live by’, see Lakoff and Johnson (Reference Lakoff and Johnson1980).

7 For foundational studies on the role of narrative for history and memory, see White (Reference White1973) and Ricœur (Reference Ricœur1984). On the role of national basic narratives for European memories of the Second World War, see Welzer (Reference Welzer2007).

8 For recent discussions among cultural sociologists about how to conceptualise ‘implicit culture’, see Olick and Simko (Reference Olick and Simko2021) and Lizardo (Reference Lizardo2022). Important building blocks for the sociological study of implicit memory are Zerubavel's (Reference Zerubavel2008, Reference Zerubavel2015) works.

9 An interesting exception is perceptual priming (see Mitchell Reference Mitchell2006).

10 The term longué durée was introduced by Annales-historian Fernand Braudel in the 1950s. He studied long-term changes, across centuries and millennia, of social structures that people to not become aware of. Today, the term is taken up again by critical historians (Guldi and Armitage Reference Guldi and Armitage2014) in order to address the long and slow processes underlying climate change or social inequalities.

11 But for an overview of existent cognitive psychology research on collective memory, see Hirst, Yamashiro and Coman (Reference Hirst, Yamashiro and Coman2018). For the social contagion of memory paradigm, see Meade and Roediger (Reference Meade and Roediger2002).

12 Sociological approaches (e.g., Alexander Reference Alexander2012) might see here a typical case of the production of ‘cultural trauma’. But apart from the fact that the term ‘trauma’ sounds cynical in contexts where imperialists fashion themselves as innocent victims, the theory of cultural trauma is concerned with ‘publicly available narratives of collective suffering’ (Alexander Reference Alexander2012, 29). It thus uses a different lens for potentially identical archives: It is concerned with the visible social construction of an explicit and painful memory, while the approach advocated here focuses on the implicit afterlives and potentially perilous agency of such constructions. (On new approaches to cultural trauma, see the articles in Hirst Reference Hirst2020).

13 My use of the term premediation is different from Grusin's (Reference Grusin2010) important theory, which is about the explicit representation of future disasters. See Erll (Reference Erll and Wagoner2017).

14 Such mechanisms are also suggested in E.M. Forster's famous Raj-novel A Passage to India (1924; for a discussion, see Brantlinger Reference Brantlinger1988). Interestingly, these books addressing phenomena of implicit collective memory of colonialism appeared around 1925, the annus mirabilis of collective memory research, when Maurice Halbwachs’ Les cadres sociaux de la mémoire was published.

15 On collective future thought, see Szpunar and Szpunar (Reference Szpunar and Szpunar2016).

16 For individual implicit memory, Schacter (Reference Schacter1987, 502) shows that philosophers, psychologists, neurologists, and psychiatrists from the seventeenth century onwards became interested in how ‘memory for recent experiences was expressed in the absence of conscious recollection’. He discusses, among others, René Descartes, Gottfried Wilhelm Leibnitz, Erasmus Darwin, Maine de Biran, Johann Friedrich Herbart, William Carpenter, Ewald Hering, Sergei Korsakoff, Pierre Janet, Sigmund Freud, Henri Bergson, Hermann Ebbinghaus, and William McDougall.

17 Where cognitive sciences have given up the focus on individual memory and the metaphor of computation and opened up towards ecological approaches, they turn into an interesting interlocutor for memory studies. See, for example, The New Unconscious (Hassin, Uleman and Bargh Reference Hassin, Uleman and Bargh2005).

18 See, for example, Katherine Hayles’ cognitive non-conscious (Reference Hayles2017). Identifying non-conscious processes in human cognition, but also in technological systems and in the realm of plants and animals, Hayles theorises a ‘planetary cognitive ecology’.

References

Abel, M, Umanath, S, Fairfield, B, Takahashi, M, Roediger, HL and Wertsch, JV (2019) Collective memories across 11 nations for World War II: Similarities and differences regarding the most important events. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 8(2), 178188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2019.02.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adorno, TW (1977 [1959]) Was bedeutet: Aufarbeitung der Vergangenheit? In Gesammelte Schriften, vol 10.2. Frankfurt a.M.: Suhrkamp, 555572.Google Scholar
Alexander, JC (2012) Trauma: A Social Theory. Cambridge: Polity.Google Scholar
Assmann, J (2011 [1992]) Cultural Memory and Early Civilization. Writing, Remembrance, and Political Imagination. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assmann, A (2011 [1999]) Cultural Memory and Western Civilization. Functions, Media, Archives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baden, C and Lecheler, S (2012) Fleeting, fading, or far-reaching? A knowledge-based model of the persistence of framing effects. Communication Theory 22, 359382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, JA (1994) The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, intention, efficiency, and control in social cognition. In Wyer, RS and Srull, TK (eds), Handbook of Social Cognition, vol 1, Basic Processes. New York: Erlbaum, 140.Google Scholar
Bargh, JA, Chen, M and Burrows, L (1996) Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype priming on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71, 230244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartlett, FC (1932) Remembering. A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Beck, U (2006) Cosmopolitan Vision. Cambridge: Polity Press.Google Scholar
Beiner, G (2018) Forgetful Remembrance: Social Forgetting and Vernacular Historiography of a Rebellion in Ulster. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beiner, G (ed) (2022) Pandemic Re-Awakenings. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bennett, J (2010) Vibrant Matter: A Political Ecology of Things. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Bevernage, B and Lorenz, C (eds) (2013) Breaking Up Time: Negotiating the Borders between Present, Past, and Future. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bijl, P (2016) Emerging Memory: Photographs of Colonial Atrocity in Dutch Cultural Remembrance. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Bornstein, RF and Craver-Lemley, C (2017) Mere exposure effect. In Pohl, RF (ed), Cognitive Illusions: Intriguing Phenomena in Thinking, Judgment and Memory. London and New York: Routledge, 256275.Google Scholar
Brantlinger, P (1988) Rule of Darkness. British Literature and Imperialism, 1870-1914. Ithaca and London: Cornell UP.Google Scholar
Bruner, J (2003) Making Stories. Law, Literature, Life. Boston: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Butler, J (2009) Frames of War: When Is Life Grievable? London: Verso.Google Scholar
Carlston, D (2010) Models of implicit and explicit mental representation. In Gawronski, B and Payne, BK (eds), Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications. New York: Guilford Press, 3861.Google Scholar
Chivers, T (2019) A theory in crisis. What's next for psychology's embattled field of social priming. Nature 576, 200202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connerton, P (1989) How Societies Remember. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conway, MA, Loveday, C and Cole, SN (2016) The remembering-imagining system. Memory Studies 9(3), 256265. doi:10.1177/1750698016645231CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craps, S (2013) Postcolonial Witnessing: Trauma Out of Bounds. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Wolff, K (2021) Post-/koloniale Erinnerungsdiskurse in der Medienkultur: Der Genozid an den Ovaherero und Nama in der deutschsprachigen Presse von 2001 bis 2016. Bielefeld: Transcript.Google Scholar
Edy, JA (2006) Troubled Pasts: News and the Collective Memory of Social Unrest. Philadelphia: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Entman, RM (1993) Framing: Toward clarification of a fractured paradigm. Journal of Communication 43(4), 5158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erll, A (2007) Prämediation – Remediation. Repräsentationen des indischen Aufstands in imperialen und post-kolonialen Medienkulturen (von 1857 bis zur Gegenwart). Trier: WVT.Google Scholar
Erll, A (2010) Literature, film and the mediality of cultural memory. In Erll, A and Nünning, A (eds), A Companion to Cultural Memory Studies. Berlin and New York: De Gruyter, 389398.Google Scholar
Erll, A (2011) Memory in Culture. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erll, A (2012) War, film and collective memory: Plurimedial constellations. Journal of Scandinavian Cinema 2(3), 231235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erll, A (2017) Media and the dynamics of memory: From cultural paradigms to transcultural premediation. In Wagoner, B (ed), The Oxford Handbook of Culture and Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 305324.Google Scholar
Erll, A (2019) Homer, Turko, Little Harry: Cultural memory and the ethics of premediation in James Joyce's Ulysses. Partial Answers 17(2), 227253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erll, A and Rigney, A (eds) (2009) Mediation, Remediation, and the Dynamics of Cultural Memory. Berlin and New York: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R and Grysman, A (2022) Narrative and gender as mutually constituted meaning-making systems. Memory, Mind & Media 1, E2. doi:10.1017/mem.2021.4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freud, S (2004 [1921]) Mass Psychology and Other Writings. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Freud, S (2010 [1939]) Moses and Monotheism. Mansfield Centre, CT: Martino Publishing.Google Scholar
Gilroy, P (2004) After Empire: Multiculture or Postcolonial Melancholia. Abingdon: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E (1986 [1974]) Frame Analysis. Boston: Northeastern University Press.Google Scholar
Graf, P and Schacter, DL (1985) Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 11(3), 501518. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.501Google ScholarPubMed
Grusin, R (2010) Premediation: Affect and Mediality after 9/11. New York, NY: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guha, R (1983) Elementary Aspects of Peasant Insurgency in Colonial India. Delhi: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Guldi, J and Armitage, D (2014) The History Manifesto. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halbwachs, M (1925) Les cadres sociaux de la mémoire. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Halbwachs, M (1997 [1950]) La mémoire Collective, Namer G (ed). Paris: Albin Michel.Google Scholar
Haraway, DJ (2019) Staying with the Trouble: Making Kin in the Chthulucene. Durham: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Hassin, R, Uleman, JS and Bargh, JA (eds) (2005) The New Unconscious. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hayles, K (2017) Unthought: The Power of the Cognitive Nonconscious. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirsch, M (2012) The Generation of Postmemory. Writing and Visual Culture After the Holocaust. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Hirst, W (ed) (2020) Cultural Trauma. Social Research: An International Quarterly 87(3).Google Scholar
Hirst, W, Yamashiro, JK and Coman, A (2018) Collective memory from a psychological perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22(5), 438449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoskins, A (2016) Memory ecologies. Memory Studies 9(3), 348357. doi:10.1177/1750698016645274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoskins, A (ed) (2018) Digital Memory Studies: Media Pasts in Transition. New York and London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hoskins, A and Halstead, H (2021) The new grey of memory: Andrew Hoskins in conversation with Huw Halstead. Memory Studies 14(3), 675685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jung, CG (2014) The Archetypes and the Collective Unconscious. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D (2011) Thinking, Fast and Slow. Toronto: Anchor Canada.Google Scholar
Klemperer, V (2013 [1947]) Language of the Third Reich. LTI: Lingua Tertii Imperii. New York: Bloomsbury Academic.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G and Johnson, M (1980) Metaphors We Live By. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lecheler, S and de Vreese, CH (2019) News Framing Effects. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lizardo, O (2022) What is implicit culture? Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 126. https://doi.org/10.1111/jtsb.12333Google Scholar
Manier, D and Hirst, W (2010) A cognitive taxonomy of collective memories. In Erll, A and Nünning, A (eds), A Companion to Cultural Memory Studies. Berlin and New York: De Gruyter, 253262. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110207262Google Scholar
Meade, ML and Roediger, HL (2002) Explorations in the social contagion of memory. Memory & Cognition 30, 9951009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, DB (2006) Nonconscious priming after 17 years: Invulnerable implicit memory? Psychological Science 17(11), 925929. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01805.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monk, J (1998) The digital unconscious. In Wood, J (ed), Virtual/Embodied Presence/Practice/Technology. London: Routledge, 3044.Google Scholar
Olick, JK (2016) The Sins of the Fathers: Germany, Memory, Method. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olick, JK and Simko, C (2021) What we talk about when we talk about culture: A multi-facet approach. American Journal of Cultural Sociology 9, 431459.Google Scholar
Olick, J, Vinitzky-Seroussi, V and Levy, D (eds) (2010) The Collective Memory Reader. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Otele, O, Gandolfo, L and Galai, Y (eds) (2021) Post-Conflict Memorialization: Missing Memorials, Absent Bodies. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Price, V and Tewksbury, D (1997) News values and public opinion: A theoretical account of media priming and framing. In Barnett, GA and Boster, FJ (eds), Progress in the Communication Sciences, vol 13. New York: Ablex, 173212.Google Scholar
Proctor, R and Schiebinger, LL (eds) (2008) Agnotology: The Making and Unmaking of Ignorance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Reading, A (2016) Gender and Memory in the Globital Age. London: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renner, CH (2017) Validity effect. In Pohl, RF (ed), Cognitive Illusions: Intriguing Phenomena in Thinking, Judgment and Memory. London and New York: Routledge, 201213.Google Scholar
Ricœur, P (1984) Time and Narrative, vol 1. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rigney, A (2021) Remaking memory and the agency of the aesthetic. Memory Studies 14(1), 1023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rigney, A (2022) Decommissioning monuments, mobilizing materialities. In Gutman, Y and Wüstenberg, J (eds), The Routledge Handbook of Memory Activism. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Roediger, HL (1990) Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American Psychologist 45, 10431056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roskos-Ewoldsen, DR, Roskos-Ewoldsen, B and Dillman Carpentier, FR (2009) Media priming: An updated synthesis. In Bryant, J and Oliver, MB (eds), Media Effects: Advances in Theory and Research. New York, NY: Routledge, 7493.Google Scholar
Rothberg, M (2019) The Implicated Subject: Beyond Victims and Perpetrators. Stanford: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Rothermund, D (ed) (2015) Memories of Post-Imperial Nations: The Aftermath of Decolonization, 1945-2013. Delhi: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savelsberg, JJ (2021) Knowing About Genocide: Armenian Suffering and Epistemic Struggles. Oakland: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, DL (1987) Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 13(3), 501518. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.501Google Scholar
Schacter, DL (1996) Searching for Memory. The Brain, the Mind, and the Past. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Schacter, DL (2002) The Seven Sins of Memory: How the Mind Forgets and Remembers. Boston: Mass: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Schemer, C (2013) Priming, framing, stereotype. In Schweiger, W and Fahr, A (eds), Handbuch Medienwirkungsforschung. Wiesbaden: Springer VS, 153169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheufele, DA (1999) Framing as a theory of media effects. Journal of Communication 49(1), 103–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schudson, M (1997) Lives, law, and language: Commemorative versus non-commemorative forms of effective public memory. The Communication Review 2(1), 317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schudson, M (2014) Journalism as a vehicle of non-commemorative cultural memory. In Zelizer, B and Tenenboim-Weinblatt, K (eds), Journalism and Memory. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan, 8596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B (2009) Collective forgetting and the symbolic power of oneness: The strange apotheosis of Rosa Parks. Social Psychology Quarterly 72(2), 123–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, B (2011) Memories of Empire. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sharpe, J (1993) Allegories of Empire. The Figure of Woman in the Colonial Text. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Sonnett, J (2019) Priming and framing: Dimensions of communication and cognition. In Brekhus, WH and Ignatow, G (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Sociology. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, 226240.Google Scholar
Squire, LR (1987) Memory and Brain. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stoler, AL (2016) Duress: Colonial Durabilities in Our Times. Durham and London: Duke University Press.Google Scholar
Straub, J and Rüsen, J (eds) (2011 [1998]) Dark Traces of the Past: Psychoanalysis and Historical Thinking. New York: Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
Sutton, J, Harris, CB, Keil, PG and Barnier, AJ (2010) The psychology of memory, extended cognition, and socially distributed remembering. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9, 521560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szpunar, PM and Szpunar, KK (2016) Collective future thought: Concept, function, and implications for collective memory studies. Memory Studies 9(4), 376389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tewksbury, D and Scheufele, DA (2009) News framing theory and research. In Bryant, J and Oliver, MB (eds), Media Effects: Advances in Theory and Research. 3rd edn. New York, NY: Routledge, 1733.Google Scholar
Thompson, E (1925) The Other Side of the Medal. London: Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Tulving, E (1983) Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Van Dijck, J (2007) Mediated Memories in the Digital Age. Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Prooijen, JW and Douglas, KM (2017) Conspiracy theories as part of history: The role of societal crisis situations. Memory Studies 10(3), 323333. doi:10.1177/1750698017701615CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vinitzky-Seroussi, V (2011) ‘Round up the unusual suspects’: Banal commemoration and the role of the media. In Neiger, M, Zandberg, E and Meyers, O (eds), On Media Memory. Collective Memory in a New Media Age. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan, 4861.Google Scholar
Wagoner, B (2017) The Constructive Mind: Bartlett's Psychology in Reconstruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warburg, A (2000 [1924]) Der Bilderatlas Mnemosyne. Warnke M and Brink C (eds). Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.Google Scholar
Ward, S and Rasch, A (eds) (2019) Embers of Empire in Brexit Britain. London: Bloomsbury Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welzer, H (ed) (2007) Der Krieg der Erinnerung: Holocaust, Kollaboration und Widerstand im europäischen Gedächtnis. Frankfurt am Main: Fischer.Google Scholar
Wertsch, JV (2021) How Nations Remember: A Narrative Approach. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, H (1973) Metahistory: The Historical Imagination in Nineteenth-Century Europe. Baltimore, MA: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Whittlesea, BWA, Jacoby, LL and Girard, K (1990) Illusions of immediate memory: Evidence of an attributional basis for feelings of familiarity and perceptual quality. Journal of Memory and Language 29(6), 716732. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90045-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zamponi, L (2018) Social Movements, Memory and Media. Narrative in Action in the Italian and Spanish Student Movements. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelizer, B (2022) What journalism tells us about memory, mind and media. Memory, Mind & Media 1, E6. doi:10.1017/mem.2021.9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zerubavel, E (2008) The Elephant in the Room: Silence and Denial in Everyday Life. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zerubavel, E (2015) Hidden in Plain Sight: The Social Structure of Irrelevance. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar