1 Introduction
From the interwar period onwards, members of the British medical profession connected a gradual rise in road traffic accidents with an increase in the number of patients being admitted to hospital with blunt, non-penetrating injuries to the head. The more serious traumata would result in death, coma or permanent disability, but physicians were also called upon to treat symptoms that developed from less severe injuries – post-traumatic sequelae that included headaches, dizziness or mild forms of amnesia. It was these symptoms that doctors found the most difficult to assess, for concussive sequelae bore no constant relationship to the severity of the head trauma. Furthermore, medics complained that a variety of psychological issues could enter the clinical picture to complicate diagnosis – a neurosis might develop if the impact of head injury sounded worse than it actually was or an anxiety disorder might emerge if the patient became overly-concerned with his future earnings or role as breadwinner.Footnote 1 Indeed, it was even claimed that otherwise innocuous remarks – say, about how lucky the concussed patient was to have survived – might encourage the development of a complex.Footnote 2 Things were only made worse if the patient brought an insurance claim or legal action – physicians believed that this might cause the deliberate or unwitting stimulation or exaggeration of symptoms, for, as one neurologist later commented in 1946, ‘The cumbersome machinery [of compensation] itself involves endless delays during which the [patient’s symptoms] become transformed into a “condition neurosis” in the sustained effort required in a fight for compensation’.Footnote 3
These myriad concerns were regularly articulated in debates over head trauma and the so-called ‘post-concussional syndrome’ by the three physicians who are the focus of this article. For neurosurgeon Hugh Cairns, writing with neurologist Russell Brain in 1928, the archetypal patient (an injured workman, in this case) was impressed upon by a variety of individuals following head trauma, with detrimental psychiatric consequences. This included the doctor who informed the patient of his ‘narrow escape from a fractured skull’; the friends who identified the cause of his accident as another worker’s carelessness; and the employer who was unable to offer any light work while the patient regained his full strength. Anxiety neurosis was the outcome.Footnote 4 Writing in 1941, neuropsychiatrist Charles Symonds expressed similar concerns about the patient’s recovery from mild to moderate head trauma. Echoing wartime concerns over military capacity, Symonds commented on the circumstances that may stymie a patient’s recovery, including ‘separation from friends, loss of promotion, [or] anxiety as to future capacity for service or civilian employment’. Indeed, as Symonds also acknowledged, insurance-claims and the medico-legal process could create all manner of difficulties for the diagnostician.Footnote 5 For neurologist Ritchie Russell, the potential for cupidity was especially problematic, as it introduced a degree of unconscious motivation into the clinical picture.Footnote 6 In fact, Russell found the issue of compensation so troublesome that he excluded patients with pending claims from his research on head trauma.Footnote 7 It was better to focus on patients who had no pending claim, he asserted, for here the physician could limit the range of psychological factors at work in the sequelae of head injury.Footnote 8
Cairns, Symonds and Russell were the most active British contributors to debates over the diagnosis and treatment of head trauma in the decades prior to the Second World War. Indeed, by 1940, the three of them would become involved in a pioneering neurosurgical hospital, the Oxford Military Hospital for Head Injuries.Footnote 9 With recourse to published and archival sources, this article focuses on how Cairns, Symonds and Russell attempted to make more manageable the assessment of blunt head trauma in the context of the diagnostic problems identified above. In particular, I focus on how they made time central to neurological diagnosis by the beginning of the Second World War.Footnote 10 My interest is in why the measurement of post-traumatic amnesia (or PTA) became so popular within British neurology in this period. PTA referred to a stage in the patient’s recovery in which he was conscious and aware of his surroundings but still confused as to where he was; the point at which this confusion ended, when studied in conjunction with the length of time that had elapsed from the accident, served as a proxy for assessing the severity of concussion (where the longer the duration of PTA, the greater the injury).Footnote 11 By the 1940s, the measurement of PTA had crystallised as the most robust method for measuring the severity of head trauma, which in turn shaped the nature of the early research undertaken at the Oxford Military Hospital.Footnote 12 My argument is that the importance of PTA stemmed from a combination of epistemic, professional and material factors.
This article begins by outlining a shift in medical thinking that occurred in the 1920s and which was associated by contemporaries with the surgeon Wilfred Trotter. I argue that concussion – the name given to transient loss of consciousness that left no long-term physiological damage – was reconceptualised by Trotter, leading to two broader developments: greater speculation that concussive sequelae were not psychologically but organically caused, and a new-found belief that recovery from concussion followed a specific model. The first task of this article is to show how Trotter gained such influence and how his model of recovery imbued a temporal dimension to neurological assessment of concussion.
Building on this, this article then shows how this temporal dimension to neurological assessment intersected with PTA and with the professional ambitions of Cairns, Symonds and Russell, for, by the late 1930s, neurologists and neurological surgeons argued that they were better placed than other medics to assess head trauma.Footnote 13 Yet, the efforts by neurologists to examine concussed patients were often frustrated both by medical norms and by the difficulty of assessing the patient’s ‘subjective’ symptoms. Neurologists therefore turned to the use of PTA to overcome their difficulties and achieve an ‘objective’ assessment of blunt head trauma (it was, according to Cairns, the ‘best yardstick at present available’Footnote 14 ). The second job of this article is to show how PTA became adopted by neurologists and neurosurgeons in the interwar years so as to facilitate their study of concussion.
The third aspect of this article focuses on the Oxford Military Hospital for Head Injuries. I identify the research undertaken there by Cairns, Symonds and Russell as pivotal in entrenching PTA within neurological thinking. It achieved this through the accumulation, and indexing, of patient-records for more than 1000 of the patients that it treated. For the purposes of this article, I want to draw attention to the two ways in which these records bolstered the neurological focus on PTA: through their explicit use in research publications by Cairns, Symonds and Russell; and through their composition, which privileged quantifiable neurological data like PTA. The third task of this article is show how the regime of data-gathering at Oxford was both product and producer of the temporal dimension to head trauma.
Through its focus on the epistemic, professional and material factors that made PTA central to neurological assessment, this article seeks to better develop the historical study of head trauma, which has thus far garnered only modest interest from historians.Footnote 15 Those few studies to consider the subject have adopted a broad approach to stress continuity in the study of blunt head trauma, such that debates about concussion during the 1930s and 1940s echoed centuries-old tensions as to whether traumatic sequelae were organically or psychologically caused.Footnote 16 The content of this article does not demur from this reading, offering a snapshot of a medical debate that had temporarily swung in favour of the organic theory,Footnote 17 but the ambition of this article is to push beyond broad overviews of how medical thinking oscillated between organic and psychological theories of blunt head trauma over the past two centuries. Rather, by building upon recent studies of materiality and the neurosciences, I use this article to agitate for a richer, more fine-grained appreciation of how the development of medical theories of head trauma was imbricated within the particular circumstances of neurology and neurosurgery.Footnote 18
2 The Epistemic Context: Wilfred Trotter’s Conceptualisation of Head Trauma
Trotter is today best known for his 1919 monograph on fear and group psychology, Instincts of the Herd in Peace and War.Footnote 19 But his writings on concussive sequelae sparked far greater interest among his peers.Footnote 20 In a series of publications and lectures after the First World War, Trotter began to speculate that concussion – the name given to transient loss of consciousness that left no long-term physiological damage – may cause longer-lasting injury than previously thought.Footnote 21 Even minor degrees of head injury, he suggested, disrupted the circulatory system of the brain, contracting smaller blood vessels temporarily but in such a way as to produce physiological damage. As a consequence, Trotter argued, concussive sequelae like headaches were not always neurotic, as many within the medical profession believed, but could instead be related to unobservable organic damage to the brain.
Trotter’s first writings on the subject of concussive sequelae were initially published in 1914 as a lengthy chapter in a leading surgical textbook, Choyce’s System of Surgery.Footnote 22 Various lectures and talks to learned societies then followed, many of which were later reproduced in the British Medical Journal or The Lancet. Systems of Surgery was republished in 1923, and then again with a third edition in 1932.Footnote 23 Despite the interval between these chapters, Trotter’s theory of concussion remained largely unchanged, and his chapter in the 1932 edition of Systems of Surgery was well-cited in the decade following its publication. In all of his writings on head trauma in this period, Trotter adopted a deductive approach: he did not undertake any original anatomical research of the brain to derive his theory, but instead extrapolated both from published studies of severe head trauma and from physiological research into cerebral circulation. On this, of course, Trotter had form – his Instincts of the Herd in Peace and War was originally written in a spirit of academic enquiry, not as the result of anthropological field-work.Footnote 24
Trotter’s theory of concussion began with a rejection of neurosis as cause of post-concussional symptoms.Footnote 25 His argument was premised on a belief that a concussed patient needed to possess a near-complete memory of their original trauma in order to subsequently develop a state of fear or anxiety. This was often denied to the concussed, however, for they frequently had amnesia for the events leading up to their head injury. Without a reliable memory of their trauma, Trotter reasoned, the concussed patient could not develop neurotic symptoms.Footnote 26
This led Trotter to reject neurosis as cause of the post-concussional syndrome and to advance an organic theory instead. It pivoted on three points. Focusing on post-traumatic headache, which he regarded as the ‘most characteristic’ of the concussive sequelae (and able, as such, to stand for other symptoms like giddiness), Trotter hypothesised that intracranial pressure was its primary cause. Whilst conceding that much remained unknown about minor head injuries, intracranial pressure, he noted, if distributed inequitably, altered the tension between the dural septa, with headache the consequence.Footnote 27 Indeed, he identified disturbance of brain-circulation as central to the development of cerebral symptoms, as had already been shown by notable physiologists and surgeons like Horsley, Hill, Kocher and Cushing.Footnote 28 Because of this, Trotter pointed to the brain’s location within the skull as a source of distress, for when an organ needs to release a build-up of blood, he observed, it swells in such a way as to carry away any excess and thus to restore circulation to normal functioning. However, the brain – uniquely, Trotter argued – was often unable to do this as fully as it needed to, as the space between it and the skull was restricted and could not be easily enlarged. As a consequence, Trotter claimed, the brain could be unable to reabsorb excess fluid, in some cases for several years following contusion, and which could lead, in even minor injuries, to the development of headaches and associated symptoms.Footnote 29 This build-up of fluid, Trotter concluded, was reversible and would resolve itself naturally in most cases, but for this required the passage of time.
3 The Popularity of Trotter
Trotter’s peers took no issue with the model of recovery he proposed. What they did not accept was the circulatory (or vascular) theory he advanced as the cause of post-concussive sequelae. For example, neurosurgeon Geoffrey Jefferson faulted it on the grounds that Trotter could not explain what caused long periods of unconsciousness, beyond a general bruising of the brain (Jefferson’s view was that concussive sequelae were caused by brain-stem lesions).Footnote 30 Elsewhere, Symonds criticised Trotter’s vascular theory because, he argued, concussive sequelae were not comparable with other disorders caused by intracranial pressure – they were not, he noted, characterised by ‘irritability, restlessness, and confusional excitement’ as found in concussed patients. Symonds further observed that his histological studies had demonstrated no clear correlation between intracranial pressure and the severity of the symptoms.Footnote 31 Russell criticised Trotter on this point too, arguing that the vascular theory did not sufficiently explain discrepancies in the duration of unconsciousness between certain cases. According to this theory, mild injuries should recovery rapidly, yet Russell had encountered many cases where that was not the case. Like Symonds, he instead advocated for a theory of head trauma that emphasised neuronal loss as cause of concussive sequelae.Footnote 32
Despite these criticisms of Trotter’s vascular theory of concussion, he nevertheless held considerable respect among contemporary medics for proposing an organic basis to post-concussive symptoms. He was celebrated for ‘rescuing’ concussed patients who had previously been stigmatised as neurotic. Trotter’s model of recovery was also praised, even by those who demurred from his vascular theory of concussion, such as Symonds, Jefferson and Russell.Footnote 33 Indeed, Trotter received considerable praise from Symonds, who later positioned the surgeon as the starting-point in the gradual (and progressive) evolution in theories of concussion.Footnote 34 Yet there is an important point to be made about exactly when Trotter became popular, for his earliest writings in the 1920s were but modestly referenced by anyone other than Symonds. It was not until the mid-1930s, and after, that Trotter’s theory of concussion became implicated in broader medical debates about blunt head trauma, accidents and the role of compensation in prolonging traumatic sequelae. The reason for this is to be found in the epistemic context of the period. This framed concussion in a particular way, based on a particular set of priorities, and is best explored with reference to the complaints made of one of the labels applied to concussive sequelae, that of traumatic neurasthenia.
Neurasthenia was a disorder that pre-dated the twentieth century, where its defining symptom was fatigue.Footnote 35 By the interwar period, however, neurasthenia was reframed in psychological terms, and regarded essentially as an anxiety neurosis, a state of intense fear of an unspecified future danger and which could follow any physical injury.Footnote 36 Given the linking of concussion with road traffic accidents and systems of compensation, neurasthenia, when it was applied to head trauma, implied that the sufferer’s concussive sequelae were hysterical or outright fraudulent. Because of this, some doctors fretted that concussed patients with otherwise genuine physical injuries could have their symptoms dismissed as cupidity or be cast onto the ‘dumping ground’ of neurasthenia.Footnote 37 Hence the concern, iterated throughout the 1930s, that neurasthenia was a misleading, and potentially disastrous, medical diagnosis.Footnote 38 Indeed, writing in 1931, psychotherapist Millais Culpin cautioned other doctors against labelling atypical concussive sequelae as neurasthenic or related to financial remuneration, claiming that he had observed post-concussive anxiety in cases where compensation was not an issue or had already been settled.Footnote 39 He went so far as to suggest that compensation was only clung to by the concussive patient as a coping-mechanism in light of their fears for the future and of the disdain they were held in by many members of the medical profession.Footnote 40
In Culpin’s model of concussive sequelae, psychological factors were superimposed onto the organic trauma, magnifying or extending sequelae beyond what was regarded as their natural point of termination – in the same way that a neurosis could develop from an injury to any other part of the body because of fears of permanent disablement. This aetiological model was popular and could be easily applied to injuries with a clear physiological basis, including concussion once Trotter’s organic theory was accepted. Indeed, as psychiatrist Edward Mapother argued, to suggest that neurasthenia and the post-concussional syndrome were separate disorders would mean having to explain how two different pathologies produced identical symptoms (a task that Mapother regarded as ‘very doubtful’).Footnote 41
The attraction of using this psychogenic model of concussive sequelae was that it could bind together strands of medical thought that were elsewhere in tension. For example, it allowed doctors to side-step long-running debates about organic-vs-psychological causes of the post-concussional syndrome: a head injury was always both, they now argued.Footnote 42 More significantly, the psychogenic aetiology allowed doctors to duck contentious arguments about the actual damage concussion caused to cerebral function, for, although thinking began to consolidate around the organic theory of concussive sequelae by the 1940s, doctors were no closer in agreeing what the precise mechanism was by which head trauma disrupted cerebral function.Footnote 43 Trotter’s explanation focused on circulatory pressures within the brain, but it was argued that it left questions unanswered.Footnote 44 Symonds proposed an alternative theory of concussion based on neuronal damage, which was met with modest enthusiasm.Footnote 45 It need not have mattered, however. Trotter’s organic theory of post-concussional syndrome did not need to explain precisely how concussion affected brain function. A belief that something had occurred was sufficient.
Indeed, the real advantage of Trotter’s theory was that it allowed doctors to attribute all manner of concussive sequelae to psychological factors.Footnote 46 This is evident in the writings of Symonds, Trotter’s most vigorous champion during the interwar period.Footnote 47 For Symonds, the brilliance of Trotter’s organic theory of post-concussional syndrome was that it accommodated the place of fear in its genesis. Writing in the 1930s, and reflecting a broader medical interest in the subject, Symonds began to underline the role of fear and anxiety in the aetiology of post-concussive sequelae, highlighting how psychological factors could be overlaid onto even mild head trauma.Footnote 48 The attraction to Trotter’s organic theory of concussion was that it allowed for these issues to be amply accommodated within the medical diagnosis.
4 ‘Stage Theory’ and the Importance of PTA to Neurological Assessment of Blunt Head Trauma
The effect of Trotter’s reconceptualisation of concussion was to imbue a temporal dimension to recovery from blunt head trauma, as was evident in the growing use of so-called ‘stage theory’ from the 1930s. Building upon Trotter’s work, this held that, irrespective of the severity of most injuries (and barring death or deep coma), all concussed patients recovered in the same way, by passing through a series of stages that varied only by the rate of their progression – they moved, as Cairns wrote in 1942, from a state of unconsciousness, to one of confusion, to another characterised by intermittent headaches, before culminating in ‘restor[ation] to normal or near-normal’.Footnote 49 Because of the universality of the stages, a measurement of the speed at which the patient progressed through them emerged by the 1930s as a proxy for assessing the severity of the original head trauma; and knowledge of the original trauma prevented the neurologist from being misled or diverted by any psychological disorders or neuroses that the patient had developed post-accident.
The problem for the neurologist, however, was that he was rarely in a position to measure the patient’s progress through every stage. Writing in 1942, Symonds conceded that physicians in general faced tremendous challenges in treating concussed patients, as it proved difficult to assess the severity of an injury when ignorant of the circumstances surrounding the accident – challenges magnified when the patient was unconscious or unable to recollect anything of what had happened to them.Footnote 50 Neurologists had even harder luck: typically, they were the last to see concussed patients after surgeons and casualty-officers, and often had to wait several days – sometimes weeks during wartime – before the patient was transferred to them. By this point, the concussed patient had been assessed by a variety of medics who held various degrees of specialism, had documented head trauma with their own nomenclature, had their own set of priorities (the most immediate for surgeons being to resuscitate the comatose patient) and did not always collect information of later use to neurologists (ie. how the patient progressed through the early stages of recovery).Footnote 51 It was a record of the early stages that proved particularly frustrating for neurologists like Symonds and Russell, as they acknowledged that most neurological and psychometric tests were of little use too early in the patient’s recovery: more basic information was required as to the occurrence of symptoms, for instance, or the length of unconsciousness. Indeed, given the lack of consensus on precisely how concussion caused physiological damage, it meant that X-rays and other physical tests (eg. measurement of intracranial pressure or of cerebrospinal fluid) were not regarded as essential.Footnote 52
It is little surprise, therefore, that in the 1930s neurologists were calling for the acquisition of greater information on concussed patients, of the sort that the average surgeon would not normally be concerned with – eg. the time and place of the accident, or the mental state of the patient after admission (plus his response to stimuli and commands). As Symonds explained, the neurologist required a speedy means of charting the sequencing of the patient’s early recovery that simply contained ‘the main facts’, similar to a pulse or respiration chart.Footnote 53 Furthermore, neurologists argued that observational data was required on the patient on the days and weeks following their recovery to gauge progress, in combination with other tests such as X-rays.Footnote 54 There was thus a noticeable push by neurologists, beginning in the 1930s, to castigate general surgeons for not adequately assessing and documenting concussed patients.Footnote 55 There was an attempt, in so doing, for neurologists to enfold concussed patients into their own professional remit.Footnote 56 Surgical intervention was rarely needed, neurologists argued, and only then in the first twenty-four hours of recovery.Footnote 57 After this point, they averred, the patient was best served by a specialist rather than a general surgeon.Footnote 58 Indeed, as Cairns observed in 1940, documentation was central to the treatment of the concussed patient; neurologists were identified as the best people to compile it.Footnote 59
It was within this context that Symonds and Russell began to advocate for the importance of the measurement of PTA in the assessment of head trauma.Footnote 60 It was the latter who, in 1932, first wrote of its usefulness, borrowing explicitly from Trotter’s stage theory to identify PTA as an effective gauge of the severity of an injury to the head.Footnote 61 That the measurement of PTA did not require knowledge of the accident nor immediate access to the comatose patient made it particularly apposite for neurological assessment. Indeed, by the start of the Second World War, neurologists began to make a virtue out of their lack of immediate access to the patient – writing in 1940, Symonds noted that concussion could only be measured retrospectively, for a length of time was necessary to allow the concussive sequelae to develop.Footnote 62 Furthermore, in the writings of Symonds and Russell, PTA began to be associated with a more scientific approach to the assessment of blunt head trauma, as if it could act as an empirical buttress against the subjectiveness of the post-concussional syndrome.Footnote 63 After all, measuring the length of the patient’s unconsciousness, through minutes and hours, was easy to quantify and standardise, and required no more expensive technology than a wrist-watch.
This is not to suggest that PTA did not have its limitations. Indeed, there was some initial disagreement as to how PTA should be measured and used. As Russell conceded, it could be hard to determine when a patient had moved from a stage of confusion to one of lucidity, for some confused patients could nevertheless speak intelligibly.Footnote 64 Moreover, the use of PTA also brought with it a number of more specific warnings by Russell and Symonds: it could not address complications like haemorrhage or local injuries (ie. to the skull) and must not be used in isolation from other tests and assessments.Footnote 65
Despite these limitations, by the Second World War PTA had become central to the neurological assessment of blunt head trauma as advocated by Symonds and Russell. There is some indication in the contemporary medical literature as to why this was, and which explains why neurologists wanted to achieve the standardisation of the assessment of head injury, because neurologists wrote in the 1930s of the real difficulty they faced in their assessment of concussed patients. This was the patients themselves or, more accurately, their emotional baggage.Footnote 66 As I have already shown, many physicians acknowledged the role of psychological factors in the development of concussional sequelae. The challenge for neurologists came from seeking to differentiate between psychological overlay and organic damage. Moreover, with so many concussional symptoms labelled ‘subjective’ (eg. headaches, dizziness), the neurologist was reliant on the patient in assessing the extent of injury – an unreliable source if that patient also hinted at psychological issues.Footnote 67 The financial wish for compensation, in particular, was identified as especially problematic, introducing a degree of unconscious motivation into the range of factors that physicians had to take into account.Footnote 68 Moreover, neurologists expressed frustration with the subjectiveness of psychiatric nomenclature: the difficulties they faced in collecting information on patients was confounded by what was identified as a lack of clearly-defined terminology for describing the sequelae of concussion. The neurologist’s task of trying to make sense of it all was a source of irritation, though one which they were as much a cause of as any other medical specialism: following the writings of Trotter, many neurologists began to question what the terms ‘concussion’ and ‘post-concussional’ actually meant (it was noted that the former implied a transience that may be unjust, particularly in cases involving long-term concussive sequelae).Footnote 69 Hence the reason why many doctors proposed new definitions of concussion, or new terms altogether (eg. ‘cerebral contusion’, ‘traumatic psychasthenia’).Footnote 70 It was also for this reason, according to Symonds, that the Brain Injuries Committee of the Medical Research Council (MRC; of which he and Cairns were members) produced a list of medical terms at the start of the Second World War, to better standardise the language that neurologists and other physicians used in writing about head trauma.Footnote 71
5 The Data-Collection Regime of the Oxford Military Hospital for Head Injuries
It was within this context that the measurement of PTA became central to neurological assessment of head trauma. Yet, the professional ambitions of neurologists also led them to create a data-collection regime of the Oxford Military Hospital which privileged what were perceived as ‘objective’ and standardised elements of neurological assessment.Footnote 72 The hospital began admitting patients from February 1940 onwards, with Cairns, Symonds and Russell on the medical staff from the outset.Footnote 73 From early in the hospital’s development, many medics wrote widely of the opportunities that such a neurosurgical centre created.Footnote 74 Of particular importance in this period were the activities of the Brain Injuries Committee of the MRC, which first met in March of 1940 and which comprised eminent neurologists and neurosurgeons, including Cairns and Symonds.Footnote 75 It had the ability to review grant-applications and to make funding recommendations to the MRC. In anticipation of the Oxford Military Hospital opening, the committee’s members suggested a number of potential ‘lines of enquiry’ for neurologists based at or close to Oxford to pursue – for instance, an electroencephalogram (EEG) analysis of patients who developed traumatic epilepsy, or a study of the efficacy of various antiseptics on brain tissue.Footnote 76 Oxford’s potential was not lost on Symonds, who, along with other committee members, sought to underscore the boost to medical research that the Military Hospital could create if appropriate administrative steps were taken.
Symonds proposed that the Military Hospital should mandate physicians to take detailed notes on patients, and then have these notes ‘filed, indexed and kept at the hospital, as a basis for research along whatever lines appear most profitable, as the material accumulates’.Footnote 77 He successfully acquired funding from the MRC so as to employ one full-time secretary and purchase the necessary Dictaphones, typewriters and filing-cabinets to properly document and organise the patient-records.Footnote 78 Furthermore, following discussions that originally began in 1940, and which included the Ministry of Pensions, Cairns and Symonds planned long-term, follow-up studies with patients, with a view to ascertaining the presence or abeyance of symptoms at six months from discharge. This initially caused some anxiety for Symonds and Cairns, for how were they to create an index to be used at some stage in the future for research that could develop in any direction? As Cairns wrote to Geoffrey Jefferson in July 1940, ‘…it is quite a difficult business to formulate questions for a statistical enquiry that will be of value 10 years hence…’.Footnote 79 Robust data was therefore needed, and, in planning the data-collection regime of the hospital, Cairns and Symonds set out to future-proof the system. The hospital was thus not only to treat patients, but to accumulate their records for current and future researchers.
To achieve this, Symonds proposed an extensive system of record-keeping for physicians and medical officers at the Military Hospital, which was to be kept separate from army or pension records.Footnote 80 Under this plan, doctors were to record the patient’s history and the medical examination in the same way in every case, with Symonds supplying physicians with extensive criteria for documentation. Thus, the written record of the patient’s history was to include details of their memory (last recollection before the injury, first memory after), their present complaints, their family history (of epilepsy or migraines, and including ‘mental or nervous instability’ in close relatives) and the patient’s personal history (eg. school performance, employment history, hobbies, significant other(s) and any past medical illnesses).Footnote 81 The physical examination of the patient was to be similarly extensive, incorporating a neurological and general assessment of the patient, plus an evaluation of their mental state (their behaviour, mood, preoccupations and of their ‘intellectual functions’ like memory and calculation).Footnote 82
This accumulation and organisation of patient-records at the Oxford Military Hospital was to have deeper ramifications for the use of ‘objective’ criteria in neurological research. As I have explained, Symonds sought to acquire extensive information on every patient, incorporating a record of their history and their neurological examination, and he provided written instructions to medics to secure this.Footnote 83 Symonds’s wish to record this information in every patient admitted to the Military Hospital thus had the effect of standardising the process of record-keeping.Footnote 84 This process of standardisation did not stem from mere fussiness on the part of Symonds, however. Rather, it was an essential function of neurological research.Footnote 85 If a researcher wanted to quantify the relationship between certain criteria (say, between the site of a head injury and the severity of later symptoms), then they required complete data on the presence/absence of that criteria in a robust number of patients.Footnote 86 For research to be undertaken reliably, the data collected had to comparable and clearly defined, and this meant standardising the process of record-keeping.Footnote 87
In seeking to correlate patient-data, neurologists at Oxford therefore made use of a database (of sorts) – a paper database, that is, composed of punch-cards.Footnote 88 These were compiled from a process involving several stages. The first stage was for the secretary to transcribe the handwritten patient-records. The typewritten record was then indexed by Symonds, who undertook a review of each patient-record; the indexes that he compiled were then passed to Russell, who, after a further check, transferred the data to punch-cards (or Paramount cards, as they were sometimes referred to). As Russell later wrote: ‘In order to facilitate analysis the main facts of the records [were] transferred to Paramount Cards. These particulars had previously been indexed in each case by Air Commodore Symonds, and were transferred by myself after a further check to the cards. This ensured a relatively uniform assessment of the main facts.’Footnote 89
A punch-card was thus created for each of the first 1000 consecutive patients admitted to the Oxford Military Hospital, which was intended to give a researcher immediate access to the range of information contained in any one patient’s corresponding record.Footnote 90 Along the four edges of each card was an array of perforations, each one corresponding to an aspect of the patient’s symptoms or to their pre- and post-traumatic state. Thus, there were perforations corresponding to the site of the head injury (one perforation each for qualities like ‘Frontal’, ‘Temporal’, ‘Brain Stem’, etc.); perforations for aspects of the patient’s mental examination (one each for symptoms like ‘Elation’, ‘Apathy’, ‘Hysteria’, etc.); perforations relating to family history (one each for ‘Mental’, ‘Epilepsy’, ‘Migraine’); and perforations corresponding to the length of post-traumatic amnesia (one for ‘Nil’, ‘<1 Hr’, ‘1–24 Hr’, ‘Delayed’, etc.).Footnote 91
Altogether, each punch-card contained over 160 perforations; and if the patient possessed, say, a family history of migraine or suffered from a brain-stem injury, then the relevant perforation would be slightly larger (accordingly, the perforation for ‘Migraine’ and ‘Brain Stem’). Were a researcher to arrange these cards so that their perforations aligned, patients in possession of particular symptoms could be easily identified by looking for the wider perforation that corresponded to the symptom under review (the researcher could trace the widened perforations along a stack of cards with ease, as several widened perforations together leave a striation).Footnote 92 Thus, when arranged together, the punch-cards guided the researcher to the relevant patient-records, in effect mediating the researcher’s access to the original record: no longer did the researcher need to peruse all 1000 patient-records to identify, say, all those patients with contre-coup injuries; instead, the researcher simply examined the punch-cards to learn the names or patient-numbers of the relevant records.Footnote 93 With the names/numbers of patient-records in hand, the researcher could simply collect their record from the appropriate filing-cabinet. This system of punch-cards allowed researchers at Oxford both to undertake large statistical studies of patients who had multiple medical problems (post-concussive migraines and brain-stem injuries, for example) but also, it was hoped, to identify a ‘new clinical syndrome’: ‘A case, for example, may appear in which there is a striking association between delayed traumatic amnesia and slow pulse rate. The observer [researcher] may by means of the index [punch-cards] easily obtain and review the notes of all cases in which this particular combination has occurred.’Footnote 94 The punch-card was thus central to research conducted by the Oxford neurologists. It was, in effect, the gatekeeper for the patient-records.
6 Publications and PTA
The data-collection regime at the Oxford Military Hospital would benefit researchers for decades after the War, and in a way that stretches beyond the scope of this article.Footnote 95 Yet, it is worthwhile to briefly draw attention to the effect of the data-collection regime at Oxford on the place of PTA within neurological thinking in the early years of the Second World War. Indeed, as much as the data-collection undertaken at the hospital reflected the new place of PTA within neurological thinking, it also helped to reinforce it.
In publications during the 1940s, Cairns, Symonds and Russell continued to underline the usefulness of PTA for neurological assessment. Just as with Russell’s earlier publications on the usefulness of PTA, the Oxford neurologists sought to classify patients based on the severity of their head trauma. Both sets of publications – Russell’s in the early 1930s, and those by Cairns, Symonds and Russell in the 1940s – used PTA identically. What changed was the context in which both sets of publications were produced, for the research from the 1940s was primarily concerned with the usefulness of PTA in assessing prognosis (or the earliest opportunity at which the patient could be returned to front-line duty).Footnote 96 It is in these publications from the 1940s that we can observe the impact of the military context on neurological research – the needs of war heightened the assessment of prognosis and rehabilitation of the concussed, and PTA was to be used to judge this. Indeed, the need for rehabilitation became more prominent in medical thinking as the war progressed, and particularly following the publication of the Beveridge Report in 1942 and the Inter-Departmental Committee on Rehabilitation in 1943. Indeed, from 1940 onwards, Symonds and Cairns pressed the Ministry of Pensions for the construction of neurosurgical and rehabilitation centres for use post-warFootnote 97 This wartime context is evident in other ways, for one of the puzzles regarding the Oxford research on PTA is why it was ever published in the first place. Russell’s work from the early 1930s had already demonstrated – as both Symonds and Cairns conceded – that PTA was a valuable means of gauging the severity of an injury. No was any criticism levelled against Russell by either Symonds or Cairns in the 1940s, nor did Russell row back on his earlier argument.
The military context explains why neurologists nevertheless sought to demonstrate the value of PTA all over again. As Cairns wrote, blunt head trauma had become more prevalent during the war, and was subject, he argued, to treatment by greater numbers of physicians, none of whom had undergone the same degree of training or had kept abreast of recent medical developments.Footnote 98 There was a need, therefore, for a greater education of medical audiences, to rectify what Symonds and Russell identified as ‘uncertainty among surgeons and physicians regarding the prognosis in these cases’.Footnote 99 It was for this reason, I suggest, that Russell republished his earlier work on PTA in the British Medical Journal in 1942, which he prefaced as being of ‘practical use to help those who treat these cases [of head injury] and to those who as yet have made no special study of the subject’.Footnote 100 There may also have been further requests for knowledge directed at the Oxford neurologists too, as, according to Symonds and Russell, the encouragement of others within the medical establishment lay behind their decision to publish some preliminary comments on the treatment of concussion in 1943.Footnote 101
Yet, it is also worth considering the punch-cards used by Cairns, Symonds and Russell and their impact vis-a-vis neurological assessment. The system of indexing utilised at Oxford imposed a structure to patient-records, circumscribing how they were subsequently accessed by other researchers.Footnote 102 For a symptom to be later studied, it had to be recorded on the punch-card. As patient-records at Oxford were compiled in anticipation of their eventual indexing, a particular shape was imposed upon them. For example, symptoms had to be succinctly expressed so as to fit on a punch-card. They also had to be standardisable, able to be deduced from the patient and recorded in every case (the same template was used for every card). Finally, symptoms had to be clearly proven – a patient either developed meningitis or not; they either had a penetrating head injury or they did not; they either possessed a pre-existing history of epilepsy or they did not. There was no place for nuance or ambiguity on the punch-card; and, in such a system, only two qualities were privileged – incidences and quantification. PTA, which was measured through quantification (eg. hours and days), was therefore afforded a high degree of stability. PTA thus became central to neurological assessment not only because of an epistemic shift and the professional ambitions of Cairns, Symonds and Russell, but also because it was easily represented and understood by the regime of data-collection introduced at the Oxford Military Hospital.
7 Conclusion
The centrality of PTA to the assessment of blunt head trauma is thus explained with reference to the epistemic, professional and material factors influencing British neurology in the decades prior to the Second World War. This article focused on Wilfred Trotter’s reconceptualisation of concussion in the 1920s, showing how his organic theory of head trauma gained considerable influence. Notwithstanding misgivings over his vascular hypothesis, Trotter was able to reconfigure how the medical profession viewed concussive sequelae, encouraging medics to understand head trauma as an organic process and not simply as a psychological one. Trotter’s theory also imbued a temporal dimension to medical thinking, encouraging the wider use of ‘stage theory’ to interpret recovery from concussion.
As this article demonstrated, the temporal dimension of neurological diagnosis then led to the greater use of PTA by neurologists and neurosurgeons like Cairns, Symonds and Russell. They complained in the 1930s about how frequently specialists were frustrated in their efforts to gain immediate access to concussed patients, and, in any event, found the patient difficult to diagnose owing to the psychological issues that followed the trauma. Within this context, PTA emerged as a useful tool for neurologists to use, requiring no immediate access to the patient and perceived as an objective buttress against the patient’s subjective symptoms. Finally, this article showed how the material objects used to research blunt head trauma by Cairns, Symonds and Russell both reflected the new place of PTA in medical diagnosis and also helped to consolidate it. I drew attention to the data-collection regime at the Oxford Military Hospital for Head Injuries where Cairns, Symonds and Russell were based during the Second World War, documenting how this privileged quantifiable neurological data. The result was the further entrenchment of PTA within neurological diagnosis.
The implications of this article on the extant historical literature are two-fold. On one level, it complements the growing research on the relationship between scientific knowledge and the professional and material context from which that knowledge is shaped. At the same time, this article offers a more pointed intervention in historical studies of head trauma. Granted, this article’s focus offers further evidence for what historians have identified as the centuries-old debate as to whether traumatic sequelae were organically or psychologically caused – a debate that was never fully resolved by the end of the twentieth century.Footnote 103 Indeed, when read against the debates that would characterise postwar Britain neurology, it could be argued that the acceptance of Trotter’s organic theory of head trauma was only temporary.Footnote 104 There is something to be said, perhaps, for studying the long-term development of medical thinking on blunt head injuries, of setting the scene or offering broad overviews. The content of this article could be accommodated within these broader studies.
Yet, through its narrower focus on Cairns, Symonds and Russell, this article has shown how shifts in medical theories of head trauma were imbricated with a wider range of issues affecting neurologists and neurosurgeons (in this case, professional and material factors). My point is that developments in medical thinking occurred not merely at the level of the epistemic, either through force of logic or the acceptance of one school-of-thought over another; rather, ideas gained influence because they reflected (and were reflective of) a larger range of issues. Broad histories of head trauma that focus on epistemic change alone risk overlooking the more specific interests that prompt (and then secure) developments in medical understanding. This article recognises that there may be a place for big narratives, but it also demonstrates the value of attuning to the more specific contexts in which developments in medical thinking occur.