Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T05:42:43.774Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The nature of sociolinguistic perception

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 April 2009

Kathryn Campbell-Kibler
Affiliation:
The Ohio State University

Abstract

This study investigates how linguistic variation carries social meaning, examining the impact of the English variable (ING) on perceptions of eight speakers from the U.S. West Coast and South. Thirty-two excerpts of spontaneous speech were digitally manipulated to vary only in tokens of (ING) and used to collect listener perceptions in group interviews (N = 55) and an experiment (N = 124). Interview data and experimental results show that (ING) impacts social perception variably, inhabiting an indexical field of related meanings (Eckert, Penelope. [2008]. Variation and the indexical field. Journal of Sociolinguistics 12(4):453–476). One of these meanings, intelligence/education, is explored in detail to understand how a given meaning is realized or not in a specific context. Speakers were heard as less educated/intelligent when they used -in, but this effect is driven by reactions to speakers heard as aregional and not as working-class. Some implications on our future understanding of the processing of socially laden variation are discussed.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baayen, Harald. (2008). Analyzing linguistic data: A practical introduction to statistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boersma, Paul, & Weenink, David. (2007). Praat: doing phonetics by computer (version 4.6.29) http://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/.Google Scholar
Bradac, James J., Cargile, Aaron Castelan, & Hallett, Jennifer S. (2001). Language attitudes: Retrospect, conspect and prospect. In Robinson, W. P. & Giles, H. (eds.), The new handbook of language and social psychology. New York: John Wiley & Sons. 137155.Google Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn. (2005). Listener perceptions of sociolinguistic variables: The case of (ING). Ph.D. thesis, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn. (2007). Accent, (ING), and the social logic of listener perceptions. American Speech 82(1):3264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn. (2008). I'll be the judge of that: Diversity in social perceptions of (ING). Language in Society 37(5):637659.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chun, Elaine. (2006). Talking preppy: Indeterminacies of style, structure and social meaning. Paper presented at New Ways of Analyzing Variation 35, Columbus Ohio.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. (2000). Linguistic variation as social practice: The lingustic construction of identity in Belten High, Language in Society. Vol. 27. New York: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. (2005). Variation, convention, and social meaning. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Linguistic Society of America. Oakland, California.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. (2008). Variation and the indexical field. Journal of Sociolinguistics 12(4):453476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridland, Valerie, Bartlett, Kathryn, & Kreuz, Roger. (2004). Do you hear what I hear? Experimental measurement of the perceptual salience of acoustically manipulated vowel variants by Southern speakers in Memphis, TN. Language Variation and Change 16:116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, Howard, & Billings, Andrew C. (2004). Assessing language attitudes: Speaker evaluation studies. In Davies, A. & Elder, C. (eds.), The handbook of applied linguistics. Malden, MA: Blackwell. 187209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, Howard, Coupland, Nikolas, Henwood, Karen, Harriman, Jim, & Coupland, Justine. (1990). The social meaning of RP: An intergenerational perspective. In  Ramgaran, S. (ed.), Studies in the pronunciation of English: A commemorative volume in honor of A. C. Gimson. New York: Routledge. 191211.Google Scholar
Guaïtella, Isabelle. (1999). Rhythm in speech: What rhythmic organizations reveal about cognitive processes in spontaneous speech production versus reading aloud. Journal of Pragmatics 31:509523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Half Moon Bay Style Collective. (2006). Elements of style. Poster presented by Kathryn Campbell-Kibler, Penelope Eckert, Norma Mendoza-Denton, & Emma Moore at New Ways of Analyzing Variation 35. Columbus, Ohio.Google Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, Nolan, Aaron, & Drager, Katie. (2006). From fush to feesh: Exemplar priming in speech perception. The Linguistic Review 23(3):351379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazen, Kirk. (2005). The in/ing variable. In Brown, K. (ed.), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics. Vol. 5. 2nd ed.St. Louis, MO: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Hirose, Keikichi, & Kawanami, Hiromichi. (2002). Temporal rate change of dialogue speech in prosodic units as compared to read speech. Speech Communication 36:97111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hornik, Kurt. (2007). R FAQ. Available at: http://CRAN.R-project.org/doc/FAQ/R-FAQ.html. Accessed: 1/11/09.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, Ray. (2002). Foundations of grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Laan, Gitta P. M. (1997). The contribution of intonation, segmental durations, and spectral features to the perception of a spontaneous and a read speaking style. Speech Communication 22:4365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. (1963). The social motivation of a sound change. Word 19:273309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. (1966). The social stratification of English in New York City. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (1972). Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Labov, William, Ash, Sharon, Baranowski, Maciej, Nagy, Naomi, Ravindranath, Maya, & Weldon, Tracy. (2006). Listeners' sensitivity to the frequency of sociolinguistic variables. Penn Working Papers in Linguistics: Selected papers from NWAV 34. Vol. 12.2. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania, Penn Linguistics Club. 105129.Google Scholar
Levinson, Stephen C. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lippi-Green, Rosina. (1997). English with an accent: Language, ideology, and discrimination in the United States. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
McGurk, Harry, & Macdonald, John. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature 264:746748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehta, Gita, & Cutler, Anne. (1988). Detection of target phonemes in spontaneous and read speech. Language and Speech 31(2):135157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendoza-Denton, Norma. (2003). Language and identity. In Chambers, J. K.Trudgill, P. & Schilling-Estes, N. (eds.), The handbook of language variation and change. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell. 475499.Google Scholar
Niedzielski, Nancy A. (1999). The effect of social information on the perception of sociolinguistic variables. In Milroy, L. & Preston, D. R. (eds.), Special issue: Attitudes, perception, and linguistic features. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 18(1):6285.Google Scholar
Ochs, Elinor. (1992). Indexing gender. In Duranti, A. & Goodwin, C. (eds.), Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 335358.Google Scholar
Plichta, Bartek, & Preston, Dennis R. (2005). The /ay/s have it: The perception of /ay/ as a North-South stereotype in US English. In Kristiansen, T.Coupland, N. & Garrett, P. (ed.), Theme issue: Subjective processes in language variation and change. Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 37:243285.Google Scholar
Rogers, Henry, & Smyth, Ron (2003). Phonetic differences between gay- and straight-sounding male speakers of North American English. In Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Solé, M. J.Recasens, D. & Romero, J., J. (ed.), Barcelona: Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona. 18551858.Google Scholar
Strand, Elizabeth A. (1999). Uncovering the roles of gender stereotypes in speech perception. In Milroy, L. & Preston, D. R. (eds.), Special issue: Attitudes, perception, and linguistic features. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 18(1):8699.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. (1974). The social differentiation of English in Norwich. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wald, Benji, & Shopen, Timothy. (1985). A researcher's guide to the sociolinguistic variable (ING). In Clark, V.Escholtz, P. & Rosa, A. (eds.), Language: Introductory readings. New York: St. Martin's Press. 515542.Google Scholar
Williams, Frederick, Hewett, Nancy, Hopper, Robert, Miller, Leslie M., Naremore, Rita C., & Whitehead, Jack L. (1976). Explorations of the linguistic attitudes of teachers. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Google Scholar
Wyer, Robert S. Jr. (2004). Social comprehension and judgment: The role of situation models, narratives and implicit theories. Philadelphia: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Zahn, Christopher J., & Hopper, Robert. (1985). Measuring language attitudes: The speech evaluation instrument. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 4(2):113123.Google Scholar
Zhang, Qing. (2005). A Chinese yuppie in Beijing: Phonological variation and the construction of a new professional identity. Language in Society 34:431466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar