Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T23:58:06.591Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Producing and perceiving the Canadian Vowel Shift: Evidence from a Montreal community

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 March 2017

Thomas Kettig
Affiliation:
University of Hawai‘i at Mānoa
Bodo Winter
Affiliation:
University of Birmingham

Abstract

This paper investigates interspeaker variation in the mid and low short vowels of Jewish Montreal English, analyzing the Canadian Shift in both production and perception. In production, we find that young women are leading in the retraction of /æ/ and the lowering and retraction of /ε/. We furthermore find that across speakers, the retraction of /æ/ is correlated with the lowering and retraction of /ε/, providing quantitative evidence that the movements of these two vowels are linked. The trajectory implied by our production data differs from what was reported in Montreal approximately one generation earlier. In contrast to reliable age differences in production, a vowel categorization task shows widespread intergenerational agreement in perception, highlighting a mismatch: in this speech community, there is evidently more systematic variation in production than in perception. We suggest that this is because all individuals are exposed to both innovative and conservative variants and must perceptually accommodate accordingly.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adank, Patti, Smits, Roel, & van Hout, Roeland. (2004). A comparison of vowel normalization procedures for language variation research. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 116(5):30993107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baayen, R. Harald, Davidson, Donald J., & Bates, Douglas M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language 59(4):390412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, Dale J., Levy, Roger, Scheepers, Christoph, & Tily, Harry J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language 68(3):255278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, Douglas, Mächler, Martin, Bolker, Ben, & Walker, Steve. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1):148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bigham, Douglas S. (2010). Correlation of the low-back vowel merger and TRAP-retraction. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 15(2):431.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul, & Weenink, David. (2013). Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.3.37. Available at: http://www.praat.org/. Accessed February 8, 2016.Google Scholar
Boberg, Charles. (2004). Ethnic patterns in the phonetics of Montreal English. Journal of Sociolinguistics 8(4):538568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boberg, Charles. (2005). The Canadian Shift in Montreal. Language Variation and Change 17(2):133154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boberg, Charles. (2010). The English Language in Canada. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, Sandra, Elms, Ford, & Youssef, Amani. (1995). The third dialect of English: Some Canadian evidence. Language Variation and Change 7(2):209228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Boer, Bart. (2001). The origins of vowel systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Decker, Paul. (2010). Sounds shifty: Gender and age differences in perceptual categorization during a phonetic change in progress. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 15(2):article 7. Available at: http://repository.upenn.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=1129&context=pwpl. Accessed February 8, 2016.Google Scholar
De Decker, Paul, & Mackenzie, Sara. (2000). Slept through the ice: A further look at lax vowel lowering in Canadian English. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 18. Available at: http://www.ensani.ir/storage/Files/20101125165203-167.pdf. Accessed February 8, 2016.Google Scholar
Diehl, Randy L., McCusker, Susan Buchwald, & Chapman, Laura S. (1980). Perceiving vowels in isolation and in consonantal context. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 69(1):239248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drager, Katie. (2010). Sociophonetic variation in speech perception. Language and Linguistics Compass 4(7):473480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drager, Katie, Kirtley, M. Joelle, Grama, James, & Simpson, Sean. (2013). Language variation and change in Hawai‘i English: KIT, DRESS, and TRAP. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 19(2):4150.Google Scholar
Durian, David. (2012). A new perspective on vowel variation across the 19th and 20th centuries in Columbus, OH. Ph.D. dissertation, The Ohio State University.Google Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. (1989). The whole woman: Sex and gender differences in variation. Language Variation and Change 1(3):245267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. (2008). Where do ethnolects stop? International Journal of Bilingualism 12(1–2):2542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudero, Paola, & Polka, Linda. (2003). A cross-language study of vowel categorization and vowel acoustics. In Sole, M.-J., Recansens, D., & Romero, J. (eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Barcelona: Causal Productions. 861864.Google Scholar
Fox, John, & Weisberg, Sanford. (2011). An {R} companion to applied regression. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
Fox, Robert A. (1989). Dynamic information in the identification and discrimination of vowels. Phonetica 46(1–3):97116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fridland, Valerie, & Kendall, Tyler. (2012). Exploring the relationship between production and perception in the mid front vowels of U.S. English. Lingua 122(7):779793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, Matthew Hunt, Roeder, Rebecca, & Childs, Rebecca. (2016). Social-moderation of a structural sound change? The Canadian Shift in four communities. Paper presented to the 2016 Meeting of the American Dialect Society, Washington, DC, January 7–10.Google Scholar
Gorman, Kyle, Howell, Jonathan, & Wagner, Michael. (2011). Prosodylab-Aligner: A tool for forced alignment of laboratory speech. Canadian Acoustics 39(3):192193.Google Scholar
Harrington, Jonathan, & Cassidy, Stephen. (1994). Dynamic and target theories of vowel classification: Evidence from monophthongs and diphthongs in Australian English. Language and Speech 37(4):357373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer, Nolan, Aaron, & Drager, Katie. (2006). From fush to feesh: Exemplar priming in speech perception. Linguistic Review 23(3):351379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, Michol. (2010). The role of social factors in the Canadian Vowel Shift: Evidence from Toronto. American Speech 85(2):121140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hothorn, Torsten, Hornik, Kurt, & Zeileis, Achim. (2006). Unbiased recursive partitioning: A conditional inference framework. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics 15(3):651674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janson, Tore. (1983). Sound change in perception and production. Language 59(1):1834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janson, Tore. (1986). Sound change in perception: An experiment. In Ohala, J.J. & Jaeger, J.J. (eds.), Experimental phonology. Orlando: Academic Press. 253260.Google Scholar
Johnson, Keith, Strand, Elizabeth A., & D'Imperio, Mariapaola. (1999). Auditory-visual integration of talker gender in vowel perception. Journal of Phonetics 27(4):359384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, Jeffery A., & Munhall, Kevin G. (2000). Perceptual calibration of F0 production: Evidence from feedback perturbation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 109:12461251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendall, Tyler, & Fridland, Valerie. (2010). Mapping production and perception in regional vowel shifts. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 16(2):103112.Google Scholar
Kendall, Tyler, & Fridland, Valerie. (2012). Variation in perception and production of mid front vowels in the U.S. Southern Vowel Shift. Journal of Phonetics 40(20):289306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, Robert, & Grama, James. (2012). Chain shifting and centralization in California vowels: An acoustic analysis. American Speech 87(1):3956.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kettig, Thomas. (forthcoming). Now say ‘ah’: Internal factors of shifting and the English low vowel space. In Louviot, E. & Delesse, C. (eds.), Studies in language variation and change 2: Shifting, switching and alternating patterns in the history of English. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Labov, William. (1990). The interaction of sex and social class in the course of linguistic change. Language Variation and Change 2(2):205254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. (1991). The three dialects of English. In Eckert, P. (ed.), New ways of analyzing sound change. New York: Academic Press. 144.Google Scholar
Labov, William, Ash, Sharon, & Boberg, Charles. (2006). Atlas of North American English: Phonology and phonetics. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langstrof, Christian. (2009). On the role of duration in the New Zealand English front vowel shift. Language Variation and Change 21:437453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lennes, Mietta. (2003). Collect_formant_data_from_files.praat [Praat script]. Available at: http://www.helsinki.fi/~lennes/praat-scripts/public/collect_formant_data_from_files.praat. Accessed February 8, 2016.Google Scholar
Liljencrants, Johan, & Lindblom, Björn. (1972). Numerical simulations of vowel quality systems: The role of perceptual contrast. Language 48(4):839862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lobanov, Boris M. (1971). Classification of Russian vowels spoken by different listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 49(2B):606608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macchi, Marian J. (1980). Identification of vowels spoken in isolation versus vowels spoken in consonantal context. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 68(6):16361642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinet, André. (1955). Economie des changements phonétiques: Traité de phonologie diachronique. Berne: Francke.Google Scholar
Niedzielski, Nancy. (1999). The effect of social information on the perception of sociolinguistic variables. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 18(1):6285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, Kuniko. (2011). Specificity and abstractness of VOT imitation. Journal of Phonetics 39(2):132142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team. (2015). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at: http://www.R-project.org/. Accessed February 6, 2016.Google Scholar
Rakerd, Brad. (1984). Vowels in consonantal context are perceived more linguistically than are isolated vowels: Evidence from an individual differences scaling study. Perception and Psychophysics 35(2):123136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakerd, Brad, & Plichta, Bartłomiej. (2010). More on Michigan listeners’ perceptions of /ɑ/-fronting. American Speech 85(4):431449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roeder, Rebecca. (2010). Northern Cities Mexican American English: Vowel production and perception. American Speech 85(2):163184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roeder, Rebecca, & Jarmasz, Lidia-Gabriela. (2009). The lax vowel subsystem in Canadian English revisited. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 31:112.Google Scholar
Roeder, Rebecca, & Jarmasz, Lidia-Gabriela. (2010). The Canadian Shift in Toronto. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 55(3):387404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roeder, Rebecca, & Gardner, Matthew. (2013) The phonology of the Canadian Shift revisited: Thunder Bay & Cape Breton. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 19(2):161170.Google Scholar
Sadlier-Brown, Emily, & Tamminga, Meredith. (2008). The Canadian Shift: Coast to coast. In Jones, S. (ed.), Proceedings of the 2008 Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association. Available at: http://homes.chass.utoronto.ca/~cla-acl/actes2008/CLA2008_Sadlier-Brown_Tamminga.pdf. Accessed February 8, 2016.Google Scholar
Schielzeth, Holger. (2010). Simple means to improve the interpretability of regression coefficients. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 1:103113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Statistics Canada. (2011). Linguistic characteristics of Canadians: Language, 2011 Census of Population. Government of Canada. Available at: http://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2011/as-sa/98-314-x/98-314-x2011001-eng.pdf. Accessed February 6, 2016.Google Scholar
Strand, Elizabeth A. (1999). Uncovering the role of gender stereotypes in speech perception. Journal of Language and Social Psychology 18(1):86100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strange, Winifred, Edman, Thomas R., & Jenkins, James J. (1979). Acoustic and phonological factors in vowel identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology 5(4):643656.Google ScholarPubMed
Strange, Winifred, Jenkins, James J., & Johnson, Thomas L. (1983). Dynamic specification of coarticulated vowels. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 74(3):695705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strange, Winifred, Verbrugge, Robert R., Shankweiler, Donald P., & Edman, Thomas R. (1976). Consonant environment specifies vowel identity. Journal of the Linguistic Society of America 60(1):213224.Google ScholarPubMed
Thomas, Erik R. (2002) Sociophonetic applications of speech perception experiments. American Speech 77(2):115147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, Erik R., & Kendall, Tyler. (2007). NORM: The vowel normalization and plotting suite. Available at: http://ncslaap.lib.ncsu.edu/tools/norm/. Accessed February 6, 2016.Google Scholar
Vaux, Bert, & Samuels, Bridget. (2015) Explaining vowel systems: Dispersion theory vs natural selection. Linguistic Review 32(3):573599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, Hongyan, & van Heuven, Vincent J. (2006). Acoustical analysis of English vowels produced by Chinese, Dutch and American speakers. In van de Weijer, J.M. & Los, B. (eds.), Linguistics in the Netherlands. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 237248.Google Scholar
Wedel, Andrew B. (2006). Exemplar models, evolution and language change. Linguistic Review 23:247274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, John. (1982). Accents of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, Hadley, & Francois, Romain. (2015). dplyr: A grammar of data manipulation. R package version 0.4.2. See https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=dplyr. Accessed February 19, 2016.Google Scholar
Willis, Clodius. (1972). Perception of vowel phonemes in Fort Erie, Ontario, Canada, and Buffalo, New York: An application of synthetic vowel categorization tests to dialectology. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 15(2):246255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winter, Bodo, & Wedel, Andrew B. (2016). The co-evolution of speech and the lexicon: The interaction of functional pressures, redundancy and category variation. Topics in Cognitive Science 8:503513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed