Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T18:58:58.947Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Modeling language change: An evaluation of Trudgill's theory of the emergence of New Zealand English

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2009

Gareth J. Baxter
Affiliation:
Universidade de Aveiro
Richard A. Blythe
Affiliation:
University of Edinburgh
William Croft
Affiliation:
University of New Mexico
Alan J. McKane
Affiliation:
University of Manchester

Abstract

Trudgill (2004) proposed that the emergence of New Zealand English, and of isolated new dialects generally, is purely deterministic. It can be explained solely in terms of the frequency of occurrence of particular variants and the frequency of interactions between different speakers in the society. Trudgill's theory is closely related to usage-based models of language, in which frequency plays a role in the representation of linguistic knowledge and in language change. Trudgill's theory also corresponds to a neutral evolution model of language change. We use a mathematical model based on Croft's usage-based evolutionary framework for language change (Baxter, Blythe, Croft, & McKane, 2006), and investigate whether Trudgill's theory is a plausible model of the emergence of new dialects. The results of our modeling indicate that determinism cannot be a sufficient mechanism for the emergence of a new dialect. Our approach illustrates the utility of mathematical modeling of theories and of empirical data for the study of language change.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, John R., & Schooler, Lael J. (1991). Reflections of the environment in memory. Psychological Science 2:396408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batty, Michael, Desyllas, Jake, & Duxbury, Elspeth. (2003). Safety in numbers? Modelling crowds and designing control for the Notting Hill Carnival. Urban Studies 40:15731590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baxter, Gareth J., Blythe, Richard A., Croft, William, & McKane, Alan J. (2006). Utterance selection model of language change. Physical Review E 73:046118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baxter, Gareth J., Blythe, Richard A., & McKane, Alan J. (2007). Exact solution of the multi-allelic diffusion model. Mathematical Biosciences 209:124170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baxter, Gareth J., Blythe, Richard A., & McKane, Alan J. (2008). Fixation and consensus times in a network: A unified approach. Physical Review Letters, 101:258701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belich, James. (1996). Making peoples: A history of New Zealanders. Honolulu: University of Hawai'i Press.Google Scholar
Blythe, Richard A. & McKane, Alan J. (2007). Stochastic models of evolution in genetics, ecology and linguistics. Journal of Statistical Mechanics 07-P07018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. (1985). Morphology: A study into the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. (2001). Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., & Slobin, Dan I. (1982). Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense. Language 58:265289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, William. (2000). Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. Harlow, Essex: Longman.Google Scholar
Croft, William. (2006). The relevance of an evolutionary model to historical linguistics. In Thomsen, O. Nedergård (ed.), Competing models of linguistic change: Evolution and beyond. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 91132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, William. (forthcoming). The origins of grammaticalization in the verbalization of experience. Linguistics.Google Scholar
Crow, James F., & Kimura, Motoo. (1970). An introduction to population genetics theory. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Dalziel, Raewyn. (1997). Railways and relief centres (1870–1890). In Sinclair, K. (ed.), The Oxford illustrated history of New Zealand. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 99124.Google Scholar
Dawkins, Richard. (1976). The selfish gene. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ewens, Warren J. (2004). Mathematical population genetics, vol. 1: Theoretical introduction, 2nd ed.New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giles, Howard. (1973). Accent mobility: A model and some data. Anthropological Linguistics 15:87105.Google Scholar
Gordon, Elizabeth, Campbell, Lyle, Hay, Jennifer, Maclagan, Margaret, Sudbury, Andrea, & Trudgill, Peter. (2004). New Zealand English: Its origins and evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, Jennie. (1992). Settler society. In Rice, G. W. (ed.), The Oxford history of New Zealand, 2nd ed.Oxford: Oxford University Press. 112140.Google Scholar
Graham, Jennie. (1997). The pioneers (1840–1870). In Sinclair, K. (ed.), The Oxford illustrated history of New Zealand. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 4974.Google Scholar
Hamer, David. (1997). Centralization and nationalism (1891–1912). In Sinclair, K. (ed.), The Oxford illustrated history of New Zealand. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 125152.Google Scholar
Harrington, Jonathan, Palethorpe, Sallyanne, & Watson, Catherine I. (2000). Does the Queen speak the Queen's English? Nature 408:927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helbing, Dirk. (2001). Traffic and related self-driven many-particle systems. Reviews of Modern Physics 73:10671141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helbing, Dirk, Farkas, Illés, & Vicsek, Tamás. (2000). Simulating dynamical features of escape panic. Nature 407:487490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helbing, Dirk, Molnar, Péter, Farkas, Illés. J., & Vicsek, Tamás. (2001). Self-organizing pedestrian movement. Environment and Planning B: Planning and Design 28:361383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, David L. (1988). Science as a process: An evolutionary account of the social and conceptual development of science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hull, David L. (2001). Science and selection: Essays on biological evolution and the philosophy of science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kerner, Boris S. (2002). Empirical macroscopic features of spatial-temporal traffic patterns at highway bottlenecks. Physical Review E 65:046138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerswill, Paul. (1996). Children, adolescents, and language change. Language Variation and Change 8:177202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerswill, Paul. (2007). Review of Trudgill 2004. Language 83:657661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerswill, Paul, & Williams, Ann. (2000). Creating a New Town koine: Children and language change in Milton Keynes. Language in Society 29:65115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerswill, Paul, & Williams, Ann. (2005). New towns and koineization: Linguistic and social correlates. Linguistics 43:10231048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirchner, Ansgar, & Schadschneider, Andreas. (2002). Simulation of evacuation processes using a bionics-inspired cellular automaton model for pedestrian dynamics. Physica A 312:260276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. (1972). Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
LePage, Robert B., & Tabouret-Keller, Andrée. (1985). Acts of identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, Erez, Michel, Jean-Baptiste, Jackson, Joe, Tang, Tina, & Nowak, Martin A. (2007). Quantifying the evolutionary dynamics of language. Nature 449:713716.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maerivoet, Sven, & De Moor, Bart. (2005). Cellular automata models of road traffic. Physics Reports 419:164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKinnon, Malcolm. (ed.). (1997). New Zealand historical atlas. Auckland: David Bateman.Google Scholar
Milroy, Lesley. (1987). Language and social networks, 2nd ed.Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Mufwene, Salikoko. (2001). The ecology of language evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohala, John. (1989). Sound change is drawn from a pool of synchronic variation. In Breivik, L. E. & Jahr, E. H. (eds.), Language change: Contributions to the study of its causes. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 173198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olssen, Erik. (1992). Towards a new society. In Rice, G. W. (ed.), The Oxford history of New Zealand, 2nd ed.Oxford: Oxford University Press. 254284.Google Scholar
Owens, John Morky Roberts. (1992). New Zealand before annexation. In Rice, G. W. (ed.), The Oxford history of New Zealand, 2nd ed.Oxford: Oxford University Press. 2853.Google Scholar
Phillips, Betty S. (1984). Word frequency and the actuation of sound change. Language 60:320342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2001). Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast. In Bybee, J. L. & Hopper, P. (eds.), Frequency and emergence in grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 137157.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2003). Probabilistic phonology: Discrimination and robustness. In Boc, R., Hay, J., & Jannedy, S. (eds.), Probabilistic linguistics. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. 177228.Google Scholar
Rice, Geoffrey W. (ed.) (1992). The Oxford history of New Zealand, 2nd ed.Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rovee-Collier, Carolyn. (1995). Time windows in cognitive development. Developmental Psychology 31:147169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankoff, Gillian, & Blondeau, Hélène. (2007). Language change across the lifespan: /r/ in Montreal French. Language 83:566588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schreckenberg, Michael, & Sharma, Som D. (eds.). (2001). Pedestrian and evacuation dynamics. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Simpson, Tony. (1997). The immigrants: The great migration from Britain to New Zealand, 1830–1890. Auckland: Godwit Publishing.Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. (1986). Dialects in contact. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. (2004). New-dialect formation: The inevitability of colonial Englishes. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. (2008). Colonial dialect contact in the history of European languages: On the irrelevance of identity to new-dialect formation. Language in Society 37:241280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudgill, Peter, Gordon, Elizabeth, Lewis, Gillian, & Maclagan, Margaret. (2000). Determinism in new-dialect formation and the genesis of New Zealand English. Journal of Linguistics 36:299318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supplementary material: PDF

Baxter supplementary material

Appendix

Download Baxter supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 573.7 KB