Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T20:00:43.206Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Transferable communicative routines: strategies and group identity in two speech events

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2008

Karen Ann Watson
Affiliation:
East-West Center

Abstract

ABSTRACT

Two speech events, narration and joking conversation, are analyzed from a sample of speech data recorded from Hawaiian children 5–7 years old, in a peer group setting. An underlying routine, which is transferable from one genre of speech event to another, is identified in both narration and joking. This routine is iterative, and allows for both stories and joking to be produced jointly in a contrapuntal style. Some social rules governing the use of the routine are discussed. (Linguistic routines, narration, joking, conversation, Hawaiian talk story.)

Type
Articles: Conversational devices and structures
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1975

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bernstein, L. (1969). Humor as an indication of social relationships among twenty Hawaiian children. Senior Honors Thesis, Department of Anthropology. Honolulu: University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Boggs, S. T. (1974). Speech events involving part-Hawaiian children five years old and younger. Unpublished mss.Google Scholar
Boggs, S. T. (forthcoming). From the mouths of babes: reflections of social structure in the verbal interaction of part-Hawaiian children.Google Scholar
Gumperz, I. I. & Herasimchuk, E. (1972). The conversational analysis of social meaning: a study of classroom interaction, in Shuy, R. W. (ed.), Sociolinguistics: current trends and prospects. (Report on the 23rd Annual Round Table. Georgetown University Monograph Series on Languages and Linguistics No. 25) Washington, D.C.: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Hymes, D. (1971). On linguistic theory, communicative competence, and the education of disadvantaged children. In Wax, M. L., Diamond, S., and Gearing, F. O. (eds.), Anthropological perspectives on education. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Labov, W. et al. (1968). A study of the non-standard English of Negro and Puerto Rican speakers in New York City. Vol. i. (Cooperative Research Project No. 3288.) Washington, D.C.: Office of Education.Google Scholar
Reisman, K. (1970). Contrapuntal conversations in an Antiguan village. Penn-Texas Working Papers in Sociolinguistics, No. Austin: University of Texas.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1968). Sequencing in conversational openings. AmA 70 (6): 1075–95.Google Scholar
Speier, M. (1972). Some conversational problems for interactional analysis. in Sudnow, D. (ed.), Studies in social interaction. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Speier, M. (1972). Some in conversational openings. In Sudnow, D. (ed.), Studies in social interaction. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Watson, K. A. (1972). The rhetoric of narrative structure: a sociolinguistic analysis of stories told by part-Hawaiian children. (Unpublished dissertation. University of Hawaii.)Google Scholar
Watson, K. A. (1973). A rhetorical and sociolinguistic model for the analysis of narrative. AmA 75 (1): 243–64.Google Scholar
Watson, K. A., & Boggs, S. T. (1974). From verbal play to talk story: the role of routines in speech events among Hawaiian children. (Paper to be presented at the 73rd Annual Meeting of the American Anthropological Association, Mexico City.)Google Scholar