Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T21:34:42.875Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of a narrative context on the verbal style of middle-class and lower-class children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2008

Jan Jewson
Affiliation:
Graduate Center, City University of New York
Jacqueline Sachs
Affiliation:
The University of Connecticut
Ronald P. Rohner
Affiliation:
The University of Connecticut

Abstract

The verbal style of middle-class and lower-class ten-year-old children was assessed in a descriptive communication task using three different types of stimulus items: standard visual, tactual, and items embedded in a narrative. In a partial replication of Heider's (1971) work it was found that lower-class children used a global descriptive style to communicate information about standard visual (abstract) stimuli, whereas middle-class children used an analytic style. Group differences also were observed with tactually input stimuli, although the lower-class responses shifted toward an analytic style. However, both middle-class and lower-class children used an analytic style to describe stimulus items when they were embedded in a narrative context. In addition, order effects were observed, suggesting that lower-class children use an analytic style with standard visual stimuli if they perceive this style as situationally appropriate. Social class differences in verbal style are not only a function of type of stimulus item, but are also a function of strategies applied in the communicative situation.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1981

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baldwin, T. L., McFarlane, P. T., & Garvey, C. (1971). Children's communication accuracy related to race and socioeconomic status. Child Development 42:345–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, B. (1961). Social class and linguistic development. In Halsey, A. H., Floud, J. & Anderson, C. (eds.), Education, Economy, and Society. Glencoe, Ill.: Free Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, B. (1972). A sociolinguistic approach to socialization: with some reference to educability. In Gumperz, J. & Hymes, D. (eds.), Directions in Sociolinguistics. New York: Holt, Rinehart, &Winston.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. & Wales, R. (1970). The study of language acquisition. In Lyons, J. (ed), New Horizons in Linguistics. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Cazden, C. (1972). Child Language and Education. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Deutsch, M. (1965). The role of social class in language development and cognition. American Journal of Orthopsychology 35:7888.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, A. D. (1976). Speech codes and speech variants: social class and task differences in children's speech. Journal of Child Language 3:247–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1969). Sociolinguistics. In Berkowitz, L. (ed), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, V. 4. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1972). On sociolinguistic rules: alternation and co-occurrence. In Gumperz, J. & Hymes, D. (eds.), Directions in Sociolinguistics. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Glucksberg, S., Krauss, R., & Weisberg, R. (1966). Referential communication in nursery school children. Journal of Exceptional Child Psychology 3:333–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodnow, J. (1972). Rules and repertories, rituals and tricks of the trade: social and informational aspects to cognitive and representational development. In Farnham-Diggory, S. (ed), Information Processing in Children. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goodnow, J. (1975). The nature of intelligent behavior: questions raised by cross-cultural studies. In Resnick, L. (ed.), New Approaches to intelligence. New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gumperz, J. & Hymes, D. (1972). Directions in Sociolinguistics. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Heider, E. R. (1969). Style and effectiveness of children's verbal communication within and between social classes. Unpublished dissertation. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University.Google Scholar
Heider, E. R. (1971). Style and accuracy of verbal communication within and between social classes. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology 18:3347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hymes, D. (1974). Foundations in Sociolinguistics. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. & Glucksberg, S. (1969). The development of communication: competence as a function of age. Child Development 40:255–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. & Glucksberg, S. & Rotter, G. (1968). Communication abilities as a function of age and status. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly 14:161–73.Google Scholar
Labov, W. (1971). The logic of non-standard English. In Williams, F. (ed), Language and Poverty. Chicago: Markham.Google Scholar
Loban, W. (1963). The Language of Elementary School Children. Champaign, Ill.: National Council of Teachers of English.Google Scholar
Pozner, J., & Saltz, E. (1974). Social class, conditional communication, and egocentric speech. Developmental Psychology 10: 764–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quay, L. C., Mathews, M., & Schwarzmuller, B. (1977). Communication encoding and decoding in children from different socioeconomic and racial groups. Developmental Psychology 13:415–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, W. p. (1965). The elaborated code in working class language. Language and Speech 8:243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scribner, S. (1976). Situating the experiment in cross-cultural research. In Riegel, K. F. & Meacham, J. A. (eds.), The Developing Individual in a Changing World. Chicago: Aldine.Google Scholar
Templin, M. C. (1977). Certain Language Skills in Children: Their Development and Interrelationships. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Van der Broeck, J. (1977). Class differences in syntactic complexity in the Flemish town of Maaseik. Language in Society 6:149–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, F. & Naremore, R. C. (1969). On the functional analysis of social class differences in modes of speech. Speech Monograph 36:777–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar