Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T12:12:41.913Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Shame for money: Shame enhances the incentive value of economic resources – RETRACTED

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Chia-Chi Wang
Affiliation:
National Sun Yat-sen University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan
Ying-Yao Cheng
Affiliation:
National Sun Yat-sen University, Kaohsiung, Taiwan
Wen-Bin Chiou*
Affiliation:
Institute of Education, National Sun Yat-sen University, 70 Lien-Hai Rd., Kaohsiung, 80424, Taiwan, ROC
Chun-Chia Kung
Affiliation:
National Cheng Kung University, Tainan, Taiwan
*
Corresponding author: Email: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Shame leads to devaluation of the social self, and thus to a desire to improve self-esteem. Money, which is related to the notion of one’s ability, may help people demonstrate competence and gain self-esteem and respect from others. Based on the perspectives of feelings-as-information and threatened ego, we tested the hypothesis that a sense of shame heightens the desire for money, prompting self-interested behaviors as reflected by monetary donations and social value orientation. The results showed that subjects in the shame condition donated less money (Experiment 1) and exhibited more self-interested choices in the modified decomposed game (Experiment 2). The desire for money as reflected in overestimated coin sizes mediated the effect of shame on self-interested behavior. Our findings suggest that shame elicits the desire to acquire money to amend the threatened social self and improve self-esteem; however, it may induce a self-interested inclination that could harm social relationships.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2012] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

As a particular self-conscious emotion, shame involves a global negative evaluation of self (Kemeny, Grueneward, & Dickson, Reference Kemeny, Gruenewald and Dickerson2004; Lewis, Reference Lewis1971; Mascolo & Fischer Reference Mascolo, Fischer, Tangney and Fischer1995; Tangney & Dearing, Reference Tangney and Dearing2002). Feelings of shame are often accompanied by a sense of shrinking or being small and by a sense of exposure in front of a real or imagined audience (Tangney, Niedenthal, Covert, & Barlow, Reference Tangney, Niedenthal, Covert and Barlow1998). In current research, shame refers to a particular self-conscious emotion that is elicited when one’s social self is threatened (Lewis, Reference Lewis1971; Kemeny et al. Reference Kemeny, Gruenewald and Dickerson2004; Silfver, Helkama, Lönnqvist, & Verkasalo, Reference Silfver, Helkama, Lönnqvist and Verkasalo2008).

In line with the view of shame as a negative emotion, studies have so far mainly focused on tendencies to withdraw or to hide (Lewis, Reference Lewis1971; Lindsay-Hartz, De Rivera, & Mascolo, Reference Lindsay-Hartz, De Rivera, Mascolo, Tangney and Fischer1995; Tangney & Fischer, Reference Tangney and Fischer1995). For instance, Wicker, Payne, and Morgan (Reference Wicker, Payne and Morgan1983) demonstrated that people reported a higher tendency to hide after describing a shame experience than after describing a guilt experience. Tangney, Miller, Flicker, and Barlow (Reference Tangney, Miller, Flicker and Barlow1996) replicated this finding in a comparison of shame, guilt, and embarrassment. However, the feelings-as-information framework proposes that people with a negative affect tend to proactively adopt strategies to amend their negative mood (Schwarz, Reference Schwarz, Higgins and Sorrentino1990). Frijda, Kuipers, and Ter Schure (Reference Frijda, Kuipers and Ter Schure1989) found that shame was characterized by the tendency to disappear from view but also by the desire to undo the shame situation. These action tendencies are an important experiential component of emotions because they reflect the priority of mood-repair behavior that is motivated by the emotion (Frijda, Reference Frijda1986). The central focus of experiences of shame is a threatened or damaged self (Lewis, Reference Lewis1971). Thus, a central motivation of shame will be to cope with this threat. Consistent with the threatened egotism theory (Baumeister, Smart, & Boden, Reference Baumeister, Smart and Boden1996), several psychologists have argued that people who feel shame are likely to seek positive feedback, or self-enhancement, to improve their self-esteem (Frijda, Reference Frijda1986; Gilligan, Reference Gilligan2003).

Evolutionary psychology may also provide insight into money as an ego-protective function in relation to shame. Shame is a human survival mechanism (Gilbert, Reference Gilbert1997). The evolutionary roots of shame may be grounded in the negotiation and evaluation of status in social dominance hierarchies (Barkow, Reference Barkow1989). Social status determined whether a person could secure the resources necessary to survive (Kemeny et al., Reference Kemeny, Gruenewald and Dickerson2004); thus, shame associated with a low social status would lead to the desire to increase money acquisition to ensure survival. Moreover, money may help people express their competence and abilities (Furnham, Reference Furnham1984) and gain self-esteem and respect from others (Tang, Reference Tang1992). Money and self-esteem partially compensate for one another when an option contains an abundance of one type of utility and lacks the other (Zhang, Reference Zhang2009). Additionally, money produces a state of self-sufficiency (Vohs, Mead, & Goode, Reference Vohs, Mead and Goode2006), which may buttress self-esteem. Ferguson & Bargh (Reference Ferguson and Bargh2004) showed that the desirability of an object is increased when it becomes a necessary goal (e.g., the desirability of water is increased for a thirsty person). Because money may not only serve as an entity for survival but also compensate for self-esteem (e.g., Mandara, Johnston, Murray, & Varner, Reference Mandara, Johnston, Murray and Varner2008; Rubenstein, Reference Rubenstein1981; Tang, 2009; Zhang, Reference Zhang2009), it should possess a self-protective function with regard to shame. In the present paper, we tested the prediction that the desire for money would be heightened in individuals experiencing shame in order to overcome the threatened social self.

We conducted two experiments using a diverse set of behavioral measures to determine whether shame increased the desire for money and promoted self-interested tendencies in terms of economic resources. We examined the effect of shame on the inclination to donate money (Experiment 1) and to produce self-interested choices in a decomposed game that assessed social value orientation (Experiment 2). Furthermore, we tested whether the desire for money mediated the relationship between shame and the tendency to be self-interested.

2 Experiment 1

Experiment 1 tested the hypothesis that feeling shame heightens the desire for money and thereby weakens donation behavior. We included a guilt condition to rule out the alternative explanation that negative feelings alone may increase the desire for money. Tangney (Reference Tangney1995) proposed that people’s interpersonal focus was different when feeling shame than when feeling guilt. People with feelings of shame are likely to seek self-enhancement to save their self-esteem, whereas people with feelings of guilt are likely to engage in sharing, cooperation, and sacrifice (Frijda, Reference Frijda1986; Gilligan, Reference Gilligan2003). Moreover, guilt may motivate prosocial behavior in related and unrelated situations (Ketelaar & Au, Reference Ketelaar and Au2003). Thus, we predicted that subjects with induced shame would show a greater desire for money than would subjects in the neutral and guilt groups, and that this tendency would reflect the amount of money donated.

2.1 Method

The subjects were 75 undergraduate students (mean age = 21.3 years; SD = 2.5; 41 females and 34 males) recruited from a general education course who participated in this experiment for course credit. Upon arrival at the laboratory, subjects were instructed to help us with several unrelated tasks. Each subject was paid NT$100 (approximately US$3) in NT$10 coins for participating in the study. This ensured that subjects had money to donate.

Subjects were randomly assigned to one of three study conditions (shame, guilt, or neutral) using a block-randomized method. The emotional-event recollection technique developed by Leith and Baumesiter (Reference Leith and Baumeister1996) was used to induce shame. The recollection technique has been widely used in behavioral priming studies to prime particular concepts or affects (e.g., de Hooge, Breugelmans, & Zeelenberg, Reference de Hooge, Breugelmans and Zeelenberg2008; Shariff & Norenzayan, Reference Shariff and Norenzayan2007; Vohs et al., Reference Vohs, Mead and Goode2006; Williams & Bargh, Reference Williams and Bargh2008; Zhong & DeVoe, Reference Zhong and DeVoe2010). This experiment was disguised as a self-reflection study. Subjects received a booklet describing self-reflection as “the ability to re-experience past events with significant meaning.” To increase engagement in the task, subjects were further told: “People with better self-reflection ability have been found to be better parents, lovers, couples, and managers. Furthermore, they tend to learn lessons from experience, which enables them to avoid making the same mistakes.” For the conditions of guilt or shame, subjects were instructed to recall and write down salient and impressive events that had made them feel a strong sense of guilt or shame. The instructions were semi-structured using a directed recollection procedure commonly used to investigate autobiographical memories (e.g., Bruhn, Reference Bruhn1990; Keltner, Ellsworth, & Edwards, Reference Keltner, Ellsworth and Edwards1993; Yang, Yang, & Chiou, Reference Yang, Yang and Chiou2010; see Appendix A). In the neutral-affect condition, subjects were asked to describe a normal weekday. Instructions were in Mandarin, in both experiments.

Next, subjects were asked to help us with a coin-size-estimation task, which was introduced as a pilot test for future studies on human perception. In a classic study, Bruner and Goodman (Reference Bruner and Goodman1947) found that the value attributed to money can interfere with normal perceptual processing. Their results showed that people with a high level of desire for money overestimate the size of coins relative to people with a lower desire for money (also see Briers, Pandelaere, Dewitte, & Warlop, Reference Briers, Mario Pandelaere, Dewitte and Warlop2006, for a related finding). Thus, we hypothesized that the subjects in the shame condition would estimate the size of the NT coins as larger than would subjects in the neutral affect or guilt condition. Each subject was asked to identify which of seven coin sizes was the actual size of four coins (NT $1, NT $5, NT $10, and NT $50). The size of the test coins ranged from 92.5 to 107.5% of the actual size, with the fourth option being the true coin size. The average estimated coin size was rated on a scale from 1–7.Footnote 1

After the coin size-estimation task, subjects were asked to reread their situation description and indicate how much regret, guilt, embarrassment, fear, shame, anger, anxiety, or pleasure they felt in the induced-affect situation or in the normal weekday. All items were rated on 11-point scales ranging from 0 (not at all) to 10 (very strongly). This section was intended to examine the effectiveness of our affect manipulation. Following the manipulation check, the subjects completed a filler questionnaire until the experimenter told them the experiment was finished and gave them a false debriefing. This step was performed so the subjects would not connect the donation opportunity to the experiment. As the experimenter exited the room, he mentioned that the laboratory was taking donations for the University Student Fund and that there was a box by the door if the subjects wished to donate. The amount of money donated was the dependent measure.

2.2 Results and discussion

The manipulation check indicated that the experiment had successfully manipulated affect. Subjects in the shame condition felt significantly more shame (M = 8.04, SD = 0.89) than did subjects in the neutral condition (M = 1.44, SD = 0.51; t(72) = 33.34, p < 0.001) and guilt condition (M = 1.56, SD = 0.65; t(72) = 32.73, p < 0.001). Moreover, subjects with induced shame felt more shame than other emotions, with all ts(24) > 23.84 and all ps < 0.001. Subjects in the guilt condition felt significantly more guilt (M = 8.28, SD = 0.89) than subjects in the neutral condition (M = 1.64, SD = 0.57; t(72) = 28.22, p < 0.001) or shame condition (M = 1.72, SD = 0.98; t(72) = 27.88, p < 0.001). Subjects in the guilt condition felt more guilt than other emotions, with all ts(24) > 22.85 and all ps < 0.001. No significant differences were found for the other assessed emotions (i.e., regret, embarrassment, fear, anger, and pleasure), with all Fs < 2.2 and all ps > 0.05.

No gender differences were found in the coin size-estimate task (female: M = 3.49, SD = 1.52; male: M = 3.82, SD = 1.42; t(73) = –0.98, p = 0.33); thus, the data from males and females were combined for subsequent analyses (Table 1). The results of the coin size-estimate task showed that the average estimate of the shame group (M = 4.72, SD = 1.28) was significantly larger than that of the subjects in the neutral condition (M = 3.52, SD = 1.33; t(72) = 3.46, p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.41), suggesting that the desire for money was heightened in the shame group. Moreover, subjects in the shame condition showed a greater estimated coin size than did those in the guilt condition (M = 2.68, SD = 1.07; t(72) = 5.87, p < 0.001; Cohen’s d = 0.69). The estimated coin size was smaller in the guilt condition compared to the neutral condition (t(72) = –0.75, p = .018, Cohen’s d = 0.28). These findings indicate that negative emotions are not associated with the desire for money.

Table 1: Mean estimates and confidence intervals according to condition in Experiment 1.

Note: Confidence intervals were set at 95%. Larger estimates of the coin size indicated greater desire for money.

A similar pattern was found in terms of monetary donations (Table 1). The donations ranged from NT$0 to NT$100 (M = 40.27, SD = 20.07). Subjects in the shame condition donated less money to the student fund (M = 26.40, SD =12.21) than did subjects in the neutral condition (M = 42.00, SD = 18.93; t(72) = –3.12; p = 0.002; Cohen’s d = 0.38) and those in the guilt condition (M = 52.40, SD =19.43; t(72) = –5.35; p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.63). More importantly, subjects in the guilt condition donated more money than did those in the neutral condition (t(72) = 5.35, p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.63). This result is consistent with previous findings indicating that guilt induces a prosocial inclination (Ketelaar & Au, 2003) suggesting that the effect of shame on self-interested tendencies is unique. No gender difference was found in the amount donated (females: M = 44.15, SD = 21.09; males: M = 35.59, SD =17.96; t(73) = 1.87; p > 0.05).

We followed the procedures recommended by Baron and Kenny (Reference Baron and Kenny1986) to determine whether the desire for money measured by the coin size estimations mediated the effect of shame on monetary donations. Using the amount of money donated as the dependent variable, we created a dummy variable for two study conditions (1 = the shame condition, 0 = the neutral condition). As expected, when we controlled for the estimated coin size (M = 3.64, SD = 1.48), the effect of shame on the amount donated was no longer significant (from β = –0.45, t = –3.46; p = 0.001 to β = –0.20, t = –1.71; p > 0.05). The estimated coin size was a significant predictor of the amount donated (β = –0.66, t = –6.14; p < 0.001). The inclusion of perceived invulnerability significantly increased the variance explained (by 27%, from R 2 = 0.20 to R 2 = 0.47; F(1, 47) = 24.36; p < 0.001); Sobel Z = 2.87; p = 0.004), indicating the desire for money mediated the effect of feeling shame on monetary donations (Figure 1).

Figure 1: Mediation analysis of Experiment 1. Numbers outside parentheses are unstandarized regression coefficients; numbers inside parentheses are the standard errors of regression coefficients. The desire for money was measured by a coin size-estimation task in which larger estimates indicated greater desire for money (1–7). An asterisk indicates a p-value of less than .001.

These findings suggest that people who feel ashamed have an increased desire for money, and thus behave in a self-interested manner in terms of their donation behavior. The second experiment used an instrument that assessed social value orientation to measure self-interested behavior. Rather than relying on self-report measures, we used a decomposed game (Van Lange, Otten, De Bruin, & Joireman, Reference Van Lange, Otten, De Bruin and Joireman1997), which has been extensively tested in psychology experiments, to obtain an accurate indicator of self-interested tendencies rather than artifacts of impression management. Furthermore, we manipulated anxiety, another prevalent negative emotion, as a contrast condition to show that shame, but not all negative emotions, increase the desire for money. Anxiety is defined as a high degree of uncertainty over an outcome and low control over a situation (e.g., Frijda et al., Reference Frijda, Kuipers and Ter Schure1989; Izard, Reference Izard1977; Roseman, Reference Roseman and Shaver1984). From the feelings-as-information perspective (Schwarz, Reference Schwarz, Higgins and Sorrentino1990), anxious individuals are likely to interpret their feelings as signaling high uncertainty and lack of control. As a result, anxiety may elicit an implicit goal of uncertainty reduction and risk avoidance. For example, Raghunathan and Pham (Reference Raghunathan and Pham1999) conducted a series of experiments showing that anxious individuals were risk averse (also see Raghunathan, Pham, & Corfman, Reference Raghunathan, Pham and Corfman2006, Chiou, Chang, & Chen, Reference Chiou, Chang and Chen2009, for related findings). In Experiment 2, we hypothesized that shame would enhance the desire for money, leading to more self-interested choices, but that anxious subjects would not show this tendency because their primed goal was risk avoidance.

3 Experiment 2

We used coin size estimations as a measure of desire for money and used subjects’ responses in the decomposed game to assess social value orientation as an indicator of self-interested motivation. We predicted that feeling shame would enhance the desire for money and cause subjects to make self-interested choices.

3.1 Method

We recruited 72 people from the community (aged 18–49 years, mean age = 30.5, SD = 7.2; 33 females and 39 males) through flyers distributed in the 11 district offices in Kaohsiung, Taiwan. Upon arrival at the laboratory, the subjects were given a short introduction and then signed consent forms. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of the three study conditions (shame, neutral, anxiety) using a randomized-block design. The affect manipulation method was similar to that used in Experiment 1.

Following the manipulation, each subject was asked to complete the coin size-estimation task and then rate their emotions, which were similar to those in Experiment 1. Later, subjects were asked to help us test an economic game that they were told was to be used in future studies. We used a nine-item decomposed game to measure social value orientation, an efficient and easy-to-administer instrument that was adopted from prior studies (e.g., Van Lange & Kuhlman, Reference Van Lange and Kuhlman1994; Van Lange et al., Reference Van Lange, Otten, De Bruin and Joireman1997). We modified the decomposed game to assess social value orientation in the monetary context by changing point value to monetary value (see Appendix B). The supposedly unrelated task involved an ostensible interpersonal interaction with a stranger in a different room. We emphasized that subjects would not see or talk to their counterparts during or after the experiment. The interaction was incorporated into an anonymous dictator game that included two roles: proposer and recipient (Hoffman, McCabe, Shachat, & Smith, Reference Hoffman, McCabe, Shachat and Smith1994). The proposer makes a choice from a combination of outcomes for oneself and for the other person. The second player (the recipient) can either accept or reject this offer, but their choices did not affect proposers’ outcomes. Subjects were told they had been randomly assigned to a role; however, all played the proposer.

An example of a monetary decomposed game is the choice among three options: Option A, NT $50 for self and NT $10 points for other; Option B, NT $56 for self and NT $30 for other; and Option C, NT $49 for self and NT $49 points for other. In this example, Option A represents the competitive choice, because it provides a larger difference between one’s own and the other’s monetary outcomes (50 - 10 = 40) than does either Option B (56 - 30 = 26) or Option C (49 - 49 = 0). Option B represents the individualistic choice, because one’s own outcomes are larger (56) than are those in Option A (50) or Option C (49). Finally, Option C represents the prosocial choice, because it provides a larger joint outcome (49 + 49 = 98) than does either Option A (50 + 10 = 60) or Option B (56 + 30 = 86); also, Option C represents a smaller discrepancy between one’s own and other’s outcomes (49 - 49 = 0) than does either Option A (50 - 10 = 40) or Option B (56 - 30 = 26).

In a typical decomposed game, subjects are classified as competitors, individualists, or prosocials when they make six or more consistent choices (Knight & Dubro, Reference Knight and Dubro1984; Van Lange et al., Reference Van Lange, Otten, De Bruin and Joireman1997). Competitors tend to maximize their own outcomes relative to others’ outcomes, seeking relative advantage over others; individualists tend to maximize their own outcomes with little or no regard for others’ outcomes; and prosocials tend to maximize outcomes for both themselves and others and to minimize differences between outcomes for themselves and others (Van Lange et al., Reference Van Lange, Otten, De Bruin and Joireman1997). However, our experiment aimed to investigate the relationship between experiencing shame and a tendency toward self-interest. Previous studies have demonstrated that individualists and competitors exhibit tendencies toward maximizing their own and relative gain (e.g., McClintock & Liebrand, Reference McClintock and Liebrand1988; Sattler & Kerr, Reference Sattler and Kerr1991; Van Lange & Kuhlman, Reference Van Lange and Kuhlman1994). Thus, we used the number of individualistic and competitive choices as our dependent measure for the inclination toward self-interest.

The experiment ended after subjects made their choices. Then they answered a few demographic questions and were paid the amount of the accepted offer. Each subject was debriefed and asked if they suspected the true nature of the experiment. No subject reported being aware that the affect manipulation and our dependent measures were related.

3.2 Results and discussion

As to manipulation check, subjects in the shame condition reported significantly more shame (M = 7.97, SD = 0.91) than did subjects in the neutral condition (M = 1.92, SD = 0.78; t(69) = 26.91, p < 0.001) and anxiety condition (M = 1.71, SD = 0.62; t(69) = 27.83, p < 0.001). In addition, subjects with induced shame reported more shame than other emotions, with all ts(23) > 18.53 and all ps < 0.001. Subjects in the anxiety condition reported significantly more anxiety (M = 7.54, SD = 1.02) than did subjects in the neutral condition (M = 1.79, SD = 0.72; t(69) = 22.03, p < 0.001) or shame condition (M = 1.75, SD = 0.94; t(69) = 22.19, p < 0.001). Subjects in the anxiety condition reported more anxiety than other emotions, with all ts(23) > 17.02 and all ps < 0.001. No differences in the other assessed emotions (i.e., regret, embarrassment, fear, anger, and pleasure) between the study conditions were observed, with all Fs < 2.05 and all ps > 0.05.

No gender differences were found for the number of self-interested choices (females: M = 5.36, SD = 1.58; males: M = 5.65, SD = 1.12; t(70) = –0.91, p = 0.37) or the estimated coin size (female: M = 3.73, SD = 1.35; male: M = 3.92, SD = 1.61; t(70) = –0.55, p = 0.58). Furthermore, age did not affect either the self-interested choice (M = 5.52, SD = 1.35; r = –.13, p = 0.27) or the average estimated coin size (M = 3.83, SD = 1.49; r = –.06, p = 0.60); thus, the data from males and females of all ages were combined for subsequent analyses.

The shame group (M = 4.71, SD = 1.30) made larger estimates of coin size than did the neutral condition group (M = 3.58, SD = 1.38; t(69) = 2.85; p = 0.006, Cohen’s d = 0.34). Thus, this finding agreed with that of Experiment 1 in showing that feeling shame increased the desire for money (Table 2). Moreover, the shame group revealed a greater estimated coin size than did the anxiety group (M = 3.21, SD = 1.41; t(69) = 3.80; p < 0.001, Cohen’s d = 0.46). However, the average estimated coin size did not differ between the guilt condition and the neutral condition, (t(69) = –0.95, p = 0.36). These findings lend support to the argument that negative emotions in general are not linked to the desire for money.

Table 2: Mean estimates and confidence intervals according to conditions in Experiments 2.

Note: Confidence intervals were set at 95%. Larger estimates of the coin size indicated greater desire for money. The sum of individualistic and competitive choices in the monetary decomposed game represented self-interested choices.

The differences in self-interested choices among the three affect conditions supported our prediction that self-interested behaviors would be associated with shame (F(2, 69) = 4.26, p = 0.018). The shame group (M = 6.15, SD = 1.33) made more self-interested choices than did the neutral group (M = 5.25, SD = 1.39; t(69) = 2.41; p =0.019, Cohen’s d = 0.29) and the anxiety group (M = 5.17, SD = 1.13; t(69) = 2.63; p = 0.01, Cohen’s d = 0.32; Table 2). No difference in self-interested choices was found between the neutral group and the anxiety group (t(69) = 0.22; p = 0.82). As shown in Figure 2, a mediation analysis using the responses in the two study conditions (1 = the shame condition, 0 = the neutral condition) supported the prediction that the desire for money would mediate the effect of shame on self-interested choices. Feeling shame predicted the estimated coin size (β = 0.39, t = 2.91; p = 0.006), the estimated coin size predicted the number of self-interested choices in the monetary decomposed game (β = 0.68, t = 6.21; p < 0.001), and the relationship between experiencing shame and the number of self-interested choices (β = 0.32, t = 2.28; p = 0.027) was no longer significant when we controlled for the estimated coin size (β = 0.06, t = 0.52; p = 0.60), Sobel Z = 2.62; p < 0.01.

Figure 2: Mediation analysis of Experiment 2. Numbers outside parentheses are unstandarized regression coefficients; numbers inside parentheses are the standard errors of regression coefficients. The desire for money was measured by a coin size-estimation task in which larger estimates indicated greater desire for money (1–7). An asterisk indicates a p-value of less than .001.

In short, the results of Experiment 2 showed that people who felt ashamed placed a greater value on money than did people who did not feel shame. The mediation analysis suggested that shame increases the desire for money, and thereby elicits a self-interested inclination in terms of economic resources.

4 General discussion

The results of Experiments 1 and 2 indicated that shame heightens the value of money. The shamed subjects donated less money to the student fund (Experiment 1) and exhibited more self-interested choices in the monetary decomposed game (Experiment 2). The results of our two experiments provide convergent evidence showing that the desire for money mediated the effect of experiencing shame on these self-interested behaviors.

An interpretation of these results using a combination of the feelings-as-information (Morris & Reilly, Reference Morris and Reilly1987; Schwarz, Reference Schwarz, Higgins and Sorrentino1990) and threatened egotism (Baumeister et al., Reference Baumeister, Smart and Boden1996) theories suggests that a motivational shift occurs in people experiencing shame, such that they seek self-enhancing items to overcome self devaluation. Self-completion theory (SCT; Gollwitzer & Kirchhof, Reference Gollwitzer, Kirchhof, Heckhausen and Dweck1998; Wicklund & Gollwitzer, Reference Wicklund and Gollwitzer1982) may provide a framework for understanding the dynamics of this tendency. SCT posits that individuals’ personal (or group) identities act as defining goals that motivate them to acquire identity-relevant symbols (Barry & Tyler, Reference Barry and Tyler2009; Ledgerwood, Liviatan, & Carnevale, Reference Ledgerwood, Liviatan and Carnevale2007). Individuals tend to feel a sense of incompleteness when experiencing shame. According to the SCT, the resulting tension may lead to compensatory action aimed at acquiring alternative symbols of the cherished identity (Gollwitzer, Wicklund, & Hilton, Reference Gollwitzer, Wicklund and Hilton1982). Money holds intrinsic value from an evolutionary psychology point of view (Lea & Webley, Reference Lea and Webley2006), and people who feel shame may consider the acquisition of economic resources a means to overcome a threatened social self or to bolster self-esteem.

Although caution should be exercised when generalizing the findings from laboratory experiments, this research provides an insightful and viable explanation for the link between negative mood and the incentive value of money. A large body of previous work indicates that shame is associated with a desire to hide or escape from others (e.g., Lindsay-Hartz et al. Reference Lindsay-Hartz, De Rivera, Mascolo, Tangney and Fischer1995; Tangney, Reference Tangney1995) and with socially insensitive narcissism (e.g., Covert, Tangney, Maddux, & Heleno, Reference Covert, Tangney, Maddux and Heleno2003; Gilbert, Reference Gilbert1997). In addition to this passive manner, the current research provides the first reported evidence to show that people experiencing shame tend to engage in self-enhancement to amend the threatened social self by maximizing their own and relative economic resources. Moreover, money makes people feel self-sufficient, which drives them to behave accordingly (Vohs et al., Reference Vohs, Mead and Goode2006) and, as such, may be able to compensate for a threatened ego. Our findings are consistent with this hypothesis of the psychological value of money and suggest that money may serve a self-protective function for people who feel ashamed.

A previous study showed that endogenous shame motivated prosocial behavior for proself, but exogenous shame did not (de Hooge et al., Reference de Hooge, Breugelmans and Zeelenberg2008). The influence of emotion is denoted as endogenous when it concerns behaviors in situations that are related to the emotion-causing event. These influences are relevant for and part of current goal pursuit. When the influence of shame is exogenous, that is, not relevant to the current decision situation, the situation in which the self was threatened is different from the decision situation at hand. Our dependent measures were not relevant to induced shame. No effects of shame on prosocial behavior in situations unrelated to the induction procedure were found (de Hooge, Zeelenberg, & Breugelmans, Reference de Hooge, Zeelenberg and Breugelmans2007). Thus, the shame is no longer part of the current goal pursuit and no effect of endogenous shame on prosocial behavior should be expected.

In conclusion, our findings suggest that shame heightens the desire for money as a way to enhance self worth. However, poverty carries a stigma that may cause shame (Reyles, Reference Reyles2007); thus, shame may strengthen the cycle of poverty and greed for money. Our findings suggest that shame makes people less likely to give away money and more likely to behave in a self-interested manner. According to evolutionary psychology theory, selfish individuals would be excluded from future cooperative ventures (e.g., Gintis, Bowles, Boyd, & Fehr, Reference Gintis, Bowles, Boyd and Fehr2003) and, because exclusion from the social community is a primary source of shame (Karlsson & Sjöberg, Reference Karlsson and Sjöberg2009), self-interest induced by shame may trigger a cycle of self-centeredness and social exclusion. Thus, the acquisition of money to buffer the threatened social self may be adaptive in the short term, but it is maladaptive for social relationships in the long run.

Appendix A: Instructions for the emotion manipulation

The current study is an investigation on self reflection. Self-reflection refers to the ability to re-experience past events with significant meaning. Extant research indicates that people with better self-reflection ability have been found to be better parents, lovers, and managers. Furthermore, they tend to learn from experience which enables them to avoid making the same mistakes.

Now, please recall and write down a personal experience in which you felt very ashamed (guilty or anxious):

  1. 1. What was the emotional event?

  2. 2. Why did that happen?

  3. 3. How did you feel then?

  4. 4. What was the consequence of that event?

Please describe the event as vividly as possible.

Appendix B: The modified decomposed game

In this task, your role is the “Proposer,” and you have been randomly paired with another person, whom we will refer to simply as the “Recipient.” This other person is someone you do not know and that you will not knowingly meet in the future. Since your role is the “Proposer,” your job is to make choices by circling either the letter A, B, or C. Your choices will be passed to the “Recipient” who can either accept or reject them, but their choices will not affect your outcomes.

Here’s an example of how this task works:

In this example, if you chose A you would receive NT $50 and the other would receive NT $10; if you chose B, you would receive NT $50 and the other NT $50; and if you chose C, you would receive NT $55 and the other NT $30. So, you see that your choice influences both the money you receive and the money the “Recipient” receives. Before you begin making choices, please keep in mind that there are no right or wrong answers—choose the option that you, for whatever reason, prefer most.

For each of the nine choice situations, circle A, B, or C, depending on which column you prefer most:

Footnotes

This research is financially supported by the National Science Council of Taiwan (Project No.: NSC 100–2628-S-110–005-MY3). Dr. Wen-Bin Chiou would also like to acknowledge the support received from “Aim for the Top University Plan” of National Sun Yen-sen University and Ministry of Education, Taiwan.

This article was retracted by the corresponding author, Wen-Bin Chiou, on August 9, 2013, after problems were discovered with the data.

1 In both experiments, the rounded mean of the four coin-size estimates was used in the results reported, but the results were substantively the same with the exact mean.

References

Barkow, J. H. (1989). The elastic between genes and culture. Evolution and Human Behavior, 10, 111129.Google Scholar
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 11731182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barry, H., & Tyler, T. R. (2009). The other side of injustice: When unfair procedures increase group-serving behavior. Psychological Science, 20, 10261032.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., Smart, L., & Boden, J. M. (1996). Relation of threatened egotism to violence and aggression: The dark side of high self-esteem. Psychological Review, 103, 533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briers, B., Mario Pandelaere, M., Dewitte, S., & Warlop, L. (2006). Hungry for money: The desire for caloric resources increases the desire for financial resources and vice versa. Psychological Science, 17, 939943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruhn, A. R. (1990). Cognitive-perceptual theory and the projective use of autobiographical memory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 58, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and need as organizing factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 42, 3344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiou, W., Chang, M., & Chen, C. (2009). The moderating role of personal relevance on differential priming of anxiety and sadness on perceived travel risk: A replication. Psychological Reports, 104, 500508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Covert, M. V., Tangney, J. P., Maddux, J. E., & Heleno, N. M. (2003). Shame-proneness, guilt-proneness, and interpersonal problem solving: A social cognitive analysis. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Hooge, I. E., Breugelmans, S. M., & Zeelenberg, M. (2008). Not so ugly after all: When shame acts as a commitment device. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 933943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Hooge, I. E., Zeelenberg, M., & Breugelmans, S. M. (2007). Moral sentiments and cooperation: Differential influences of shame and guilt. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 10251042.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, M. J., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). Liking is for doing: The effects of goal pursuit on automatic evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 557572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & Ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furnham, A. (1984). Many sides of the coin: The psychology of money usage. Personality and Individual Difference, 5, 501509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, P. (1997). The evolution of social attractiveness and its role in shame, humiliation, guilt and therapy. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 70, 113147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilligan, J. (2003). Shame, guilt, and violence. Social Research, 70, 11491180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., & Fehr, E. (2003). Explaining altruistic behavior in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 153172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kirchhof, O. (1998). The willful pursuit of identity. In Heckhausen, J. & Dweck, C. S. (Eds.), Life-span perspectives on motivation and control (pp. 389423). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M., Wicklund, R. A., & Hilton, J. L. (1982). Admission of failure and symbolic self-completion: Extending Lewinian theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 358371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K., Shachat, K., & Smith, V. (1994). Preferences, property rights and anonymity in bargaining games. Games and Economic Behavior, 7, 346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlsson, G., & Sjöberg, L. G. (2009). The experiences of guilt and shame: A phenomenological-psychological study. Human Studies, 32, 335355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P. C., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pessimism: Effect of sadness and anger on social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 740752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemeny, M. E., Gruenewald, T. L., & Dickerson, S. S. (2004). Shame as the emotional response to threat to the social self: Implications for behavior, physiology, and health. .Psychological Inquiry, 15, 153160.Google Scholar
Ketelaar, T., & Au, W. T. (2003). The effects of guilty feelings on the behavior of uncooperative individuals in repeated social bargaining games: An affect-as-information interpretation of the role of emotion in social interaction. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 429453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knight, G. P., & Dubro, A. F. (1984). An individualized regression and clustering assessment of the social values of adults and children. Journal of Research in Personality, 18, 372382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lea, S. E. G., & Webley, P. (2006). Money as tool, money as drug: The biological psychology of a strong incentive. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 161209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ledgerwood, A., Liviatan, I., & Carnevale, P. J. (2007). Groupidentity completion and the symbolic value of property. Psychological Science, 18, 873878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1996). Why do bad moods increase self-defeating behavior? Emotion, risking-taking, and self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 12501267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: International Universities Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Lindsay-Hartz, J., De Rivera, J., & Mascolo, M. F. (1995). Differentiating guilt and shame and their effects on motivation. In Tangney, J. P. & Fischer, K. W. (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 274300). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Mandara, J., Johnston, J. S., Murray, C. B., & Varner, F. (2008). Marriage, money, and African American mothers’ self-esteem. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70, 11881199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mascolo, M. E., & Fischer, K. W. (1995). Developmental transformation in appraisals for pride, shame, and guilt. In Tangney, J. E. & Fischer, K. W. (Eds.), Self-conscious emotions: Shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride (pp. 64113). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
McClintock, C. G., & Liebrand, W. B. G. (1988). The role of interdependence structure, individual value orientation, and other’s strategy in social decision making: A transformational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 396409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, W. N., & Reilly, N. P. (1987). Toward the self-regulation of mood: Theory and practice. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 215249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raghunathan, R., & Pham, M. T. (1999). All negative moods are not equal: Motivational influences of anxiety and sadness on decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 79, 5677.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raghunathan, R., Pham, M. T., & Corfman, K. P. (2006). Informational properties of anxiety and sadness, and displaced coping. Journal of Consumer Research, 32, 596601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyles, D. Z. (2007). The ability to go about without shame: A proposal for internationally comparable indicators of shame and humiliation. Oxford Development Studies, 35, 405426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotion. In Shaver, P. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Emotions, relationships, and health (Vol. 5, pp. 1134). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Rubenstein, C. (1981). Money and self-esteem, relationships, secrecy, envy, satisfaction. Psychology Today, 15, 2944.Google Scholar
Sattler, D. N., & Kerr, N. L. (1991). Might versus morality explored: Motivational and cognitive bases for social motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 756765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Higgins, T.E. & Sorrentino, R.M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (pp. 527561). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming god concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science, 18, 803809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silfver, M., Helkama, K., Lönnqvist, J., & Verkasalo, M. (2008). The relation between value priorities and proneness to guilt, shame, and empathy. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 6980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tang, T. L. (1992). The meaning of money revisited. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13, 197202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tangney, J. P. (1995). Recent advances in the empirical study of shame and guilt. American Behavioral Scientist, 38, 11321145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., & Fischer, K. W. (1995). Self-conscious emotions: The psychology of shame, guilt, embarrassment, and pride. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 12561269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tangney, J. P., Niedenthal, P. M., Covert, M. V., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). Are shame and guilt related to distinct self-discrepancies? A test of Higgins’s (1987) hypotheses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 256268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Lange, P. A. M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (1994). Social value orientations and impressions of partner’s honesty and intelligence: A test of the might versus morality effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 126141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Lange, P. A. M., Otten, W., De Bruin, E. M. N., & Joireman, J. A. (1997). Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: Theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 733746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological consequences of money. Science, 314, 11541156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wicker, F. W., Payne, G. C., & Morgan, R. D. (1983). Participant descriptions of guilt and shame. Motivation and Emotion, 7, 2539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-completion. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Williams, L. E., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science, 322, 606607.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, M., Yang, C., & Chiou, W. (2010). When guilt leads to other orientation and shame leads to egocentric self-focus: Effects of differential priming of negative affects on perspective taking. Social Behavior and Personality, 38, 605614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, L. (2009). An exchange theory of money and self-esteem in decision making. Review of General Psychology, 13, 6676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhong, C. B., & DeVoe, S. E. (2010). You are how you eat: Fast food and impatience. Psychological Science, 21, 619622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1: Mean estimates and confidence intervals according to condition in Experiment 1.

Figure 1

Figure 1: Mediation analysis of Experiment 1. Numbers outside parentheses are unstandarized regression coefficients; numbers inside parentheses are the standard errors of regression coefficients. The desire for money was measured by a coin size-estimation task in which larger estimates indicated greater desire for money (1–7). An asterisk indicates a p-value of less than .001.

Figure 2

Table 2: Mean estimates and confidence intervals according to conditions in Experiments 2.

Figure 3

Figure 2: Mediation analysis of Experiment 2. Numbers outside parentheses are unstandarized regression coefficients; numbers inside parentheses are the standard errors of regression coefficients. The desire for money was measured by a coin size-estimation task in which larger estimates indicated greater desire for money (1–7). An asterisk indicates a p-value of less than .001.