Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T03:24:08.096Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The impact of actively open-minded thinking on social media communication

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Jordan Carpenter
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania
Daniel Preotiuc-Pietro
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania Computer and Information Science, University of Pennsylvania
Jenna Clark
Affiliation:
Center for Advanced Hindsight, Duke University
Lucie Flekova
Affiliation:
Department of Computer Science, University College London
Laura Smith
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania
Margaret L. Kern
Affiliation:
Melbourne Graduate School of Education, The University of Melbourne, Australia
Anneke Buffone
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania
Lyle Ungar
Affiliation:
Computer and Information Science, University of Pennsylvania
Martin Seligman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Online, social media communication is often ambiguous, and it can encourage speed and inattentiveness. We investigated whether Actively Open Minded Thinking (AOT), a dispositional willingness to seek out new or potentially threatening information, may help users avoid these pitfalls. In Study 1, we determined that correctly assessing social media authors’ traits was positively predicted by raters’ AOT. In Study 2, we used data-driven methods to devise a three-dimensional picture of online behaviors of people high or low in AOT, finding that AOT is associated with thoughtful, nuanced, idiosyncratic actions and with resisting the typically fast pace of online interactions. AOT may be an important factor in accurate, socially responsible online behavior.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2018] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

In 1993, a famous New Yorker cartoon by Peter Steiger coined the famous axiom representing the difficulty of understanding other people online: “On the internet, nobody knows you’re a dog.” Although technological advancements have greatly changed online interactions in the subsequent years, the internet continues to have a number of limitations as a platform for interaction. Compared to face-to-face interaction, successful online communication requires additional attention and motivation. Social cues can be ambiguous or absent, and the variety and fleeting nature of the information can be overwhelming. In addition, users have a large degree of control over the information and people they encounter, meaning there can be minimal social consequences to misunderstandings online.

Because of this combination of an increased ability to filter content along with an overwhelming amount of ambiguous information, many researchers have expressed concern over the possibility that internet users selectively filter out or ignore material and perspectives they dislike, disagree with, or do not quickly understand (e.g., Reference Bakshy, Messing and AdamicBakshy, Messing & Adamic, 2015; Reference Barberá, Jost, Nagler, Tucker and BonneauBarbera, et al., 2015; Bennett & Iyengar, 2008). In other words, social media use may be particularly prone to promoting closed-mindedness.

One characteristic that might address this problem is Actively Open-Minded Thinking (AOT; Reference BaronBaron, 1993, in press), the dispositional willingness to seek out and thoughtfully engage with new and even threatening information. In this paper, we demonstrate explicit benefits of trait AOT in the accurate perception of other people online; then, using data-driven methods such as Natural Language Processing, we present a picture of how AOT manifests itself in online behaviors.

1.1 Challenges in computer-mediated communication (CMC)

Computer-mediated communication (CMC) using text is an inherently limited channel for interaction when compared to face-to-face interactions: it cannot convey important cues such as facial expression, tone of voice, and body language (Reference Sproull and KieslerSproull & Kiesler, 1986). Some researchers believe that the absence of these cues presents difficulties that cannot be easily overcome, leading to miscommunication (Reference Kruger, Epley, Parker and NgKruger, Epley, Parker & Ng, 2005) and misperceptions of others (Epley & Kruger, 2004; Reference Okdie, Guadagno, Bernieri, Geers and Mclarney-VesotskiOkdie, Guadagno, Bernieri, Geers, Mclarney-Vesotski, 2011). Some evidence suggests that such misperceptions even extend to assessments of a person’s membership in basic demographic categories such as age and gender, which are typically automatic and easy in face-to-face communication (Reference Quinn, Yahr, Kuhn, Slater and PascalisQuinn et al., 2002; Reference Tranel, Damasio and DamasioTranel, Damasio & Damasio, 1988), but are more difficult when there is a dearth of visual or audible cues, such as on social media (Flekova at al., 2016).

However, other research indicates that users can learn to attend to alternative cue systems (e.g., Reference WaltherWalther, 1992, 1993) to form correct impressions of others online (e.g., Reference Carpenter, Preotiuc-Pietro, Flekova, Giorgi, Hagan, Kern, Buffone, Ungar and SeligmanCarpenter et al., 2016; Reference Darbyshire, Kirk, Wall and KayeDarbyshire et al., 2016; Reference Tskhay and RuleTskhay & Rule, 2014). Overall, it is not clear how well these alternative cue systems work — that is, whether the impressions made through CMC are necessarily less accurate than those made face-to-face, or if people can accurately judge others based solely on text-based, online communication.

Furthermore, text-based social communicaton necessarily occurs in a digital medium, which generally facilitates a faster and shallower orientation towards information. Users have poorer retention of information they expect to be able to encounter online (Reference Sparrow, Liu and WegnerSparrow, Liu & Wegner, 2011), they focus on “ground-level information” at the expense of “big-picture construal” (Reference Kaufman and FlanaganKaufmann & Flanagan, 2016), and they compensate for large amounts of information by strategically skimming content (Reference Duggan and PayneDuggan & Payne, 2011). Heavy social media users are less likely to enjoy effortful thought and also are likely to engage in media multitasking (Reference Zhong, Hardin and SunZhong, Hardin & Sun, 2011), which is associated with less attentiveness and poorer performance on cognitive tasks (Reference Vega, McCracken, Nass and LabsVega, McCracken, Nass & Labs, 2008).

Finally, social media platforms typically allow users a degree of control over the people and information they encounter, which can result in ideological segregation (e.g., Reference Bakshy, Messing and AdamicBakshy, Messing & Adamic, 2015; Reference Dehghani, Johnson, Hoover, Sagi, Garten, Parmar, Vaisey, Iliev and GrahamDehghani, et al., 2016; Reference Vaccari, Valeriani, Barberá, Jost, Nagler and TuckerVaccari, et al., 2016). Although the magnitude of this “echo chamber effect” is debated (e.g., Reference Barberá, Jost, Nagler, Tucker and BonneauBarbera, et al., 2015; Reference Flaxman, Goel and RaoFlaxman, Goel & Rao, 2016), the consequences may be severe: online extremism has been linked to devaluing science (Reference Helmuth, Gouhier, Scyphers and MocarskiHelmuth, Gouhier, Scyphers & Mocarski, 2016), the spread of misleading political information (Reference Shin, Jian, Driscoll and BarShin, Jian, Driscoll & Bar, 2016), and ultimately might lead to less competent citizenship (Reference Flynn, Nyhan and ReiflerFlynn, Nyhan & Reifler, 2017).

In sum, not only are some basic types of social cognition more difficult in text, but the digital, online medium can facilitate a closed-minded mindset (Reference KruglanskiKruglanski, 2004) characterized by inattentiveness and avoidance of challenging information. However, individual differences may affect the extent to which users draw on useful cues within the CMC context despite these limitations.

1.2 Actively open-minded thinking

One potentially promising antidote to this closed-mindedness is users’ willingness to attend to new information and adopt a thoughtful, methodical mindset. This cognitive style is known as Actively Open-Minded Thinking (AOT). People high in AOT on the trait level (measured by the AOT scale used by Reference Haran, Ritov and MellersHaran, Ritov & Mellers, 2013, and other similar scales) are more accurate at a variety of judgments, such as estimating amounts (Reference Haran, Ritov and MellersHaran, Ritov & Mellers, 2013), distinguishing between good and bad arguments (Reference Stanovich and WestStanovich & West, 1997), and forecasting world events (Reference Mellers, Stone, Atanasov, Rohrbaugh, Metz, Ungar, Bisop, Horowitz, Merkle and TetlockMellers et al., 2015).Footnote 1 In short, this cognitive style has real-world benefits: by ignoring biases and being open to sources of information, people are more likely to make accurate inferences.

AOT is one of many constructs related to trait-level, epistemic orientations. Other commonly used scales are Need for Cognition (Reference Cacioppo and PettyCacioppo & Petty, 1982), which measures a motivation to cognitively elaborate on information; Need for Closure (Reference Webster and KruglanskiWebster & Kruglanski, 1994), which measures intolerance for ambiguity and uncertainty; and Personal Fear of Invalidity (Reference Thompson, Naccarato, Parker, Moskowitz and MoskowitzThompson, Naccarato, Parker & Moskowitz, 2001), which measures the aversion to reaching erroneous conclusions. AOT is correlated with these measures (e.g., Reference Haran, Ritov and MellersHaran, Ritov & Mellers, 2013), but it is distinct in that it specifically describes a preference towards taking in more diverse amounts of information, especially information that may conflict with previous intuitions. This tendency is in direct contrast to the hasty, inattentive mindset typically facilitated by digital communication (e.g., Reference Sparrow, Liu and WegnerSparrow, Liu & Wegner, 2011; Reference Zhong, Hardin and SunZhong, Hardin & Sun, 2011). Therefore, AOT may play a beneficial role in CMC by increasing users’ tendency to attend to the distinctively scarce social cues online, via either more fair-minded or more extensive search.

If AOT is indeed beneficial for understanding others online, the next step in understanding its influence on social media behavior is to take advantage of the enormous amounts of information produced on social media to reveal the patterns of behavior associated with high and low levels of AOT. Data-driven, unrestricted techniques can allow us to visualize how AOT’s thoughtful and tolerant orientation has distinct effects on the ways people communicate online in the real world; in particular, the behavior of people high in AOT may be distinguished by their preference to avoid impulsive conclusions.

1.3 Current goals

The present study had two aims. First, we used quasi-experimental methods to investigate whether having high AOT would facilitate drawing accurate conclusions about others based only on their social media activity. Second, we applied data-driven methods to explore and visualize the ways that AOT manifests itself across people’s general social media behavior: its effect on linguistic expression, visual self-representation, and interpersonal behaviors. We therefore focused on both sides of social media participation: responding to content and creating content. In other words, we sought to answer: (1) is AOT beneficial for CMC? and (2) what general, real-world social media behavior is associated with AOT?

2 Study 1

The goal of Study 1 was to investigate whether AOT is associated with drawing objectively accurate conclusions about other people online. Specifically, we predicted that high-AOT individuals would be better at guessing targets’ basic demographic traits relying solely on the targets’ social media posts. Because these basic categorizations are often automatic and easy in face to face interactions (e.g., Reference Tranel, Damasio and DamasioTranel, Damasio & Damasio, 1988), it is especially useful to examine if AOT is associated with greater accuracy in a social media context, where sparser cues make people more likely to fail.

2.1 General methods and materials

Study 1 consists of four sub-studies, all of which followed the same procedure and had the same hypothesis. Participants were shown a series of tweets by target authors and asked to guess each author’s status on a selected characteristic (1a: gender; 1b: age; 1c: education level; 1d: political orientation). Each set of authors’s tweets was rated on only a single characteristic, and participants signed up to rate one of the four characteristics, and after signing up, rated tweets on that characteristic only. For all studies, we hypothesized that participants’ levels of trait AOT would be positively associated with more accurate guesses.

Participants were recruited via Amazon Mechanical Turk and underwent a brief training explaining the task and the trait they were to identify (1a: gender; 1b: age; 1c: education level; 1d: political orientation). Participants then completed a short demographic survey and the 9-item Actively Open-Minded Thinking questionnaire (Reference Haran, Ritov and MellersHaran et al., 2013; see Supplement A for items). Reliability for AOT, which was measured across studies on a 1–7 scale, was acceptable in each sample (Study 1a α = .77; Study 1b α = .75; Study 1c α = .77; Study 1d α = .83).

Participants then completed the rating task, in which they were shown a set of 20 randomly chosen tweets out of a battery of 100 tweets posted by a single author in the past year (user mentions and URLs, which might disclose personal information, were replaced with placeholders). Based only on these tweets, participants attempted to guess the author’s demographics. To discourage blind guessing, participants were not allowed to submit an answer until a minimum of 10 seconds had passed.

Participants were paid $0.02 for each task and were allowed to perform the task as many times as they wished, but never for the same author. They were presented with an initial bonus after filling in the training and surveys ($0.25) and another bonus after completing 10 ratings ($0.25).Footnote 2 Figure 1 shows a screenshot of an example task from Study 2d.

Figure 1: Sample task for Study 1.

To control for the fact that outcomes regarding the same authors would likely be intercorrelated, we used hierarchical linear regression (Reference Dai, Li and RockeDai, Li & Rocke, 2006) to predict binary outcomes (correct/incorrect), from raters’ trait AOT. Because each author was rated by more than one participant, we used a model with both rater and author as crossed random effects.

2.2 Study 1a: Gender

Tweets from 2,607 authors who could be identified as male or female were collected. An objective gender label (male/female) was determined by linking their Twitter profile to self-reported information available on Twitter or similar apps (Reference Burger, Henderson, Kim and ZarrellaBurger et al., 2011).Footnote 3

Participants (n = 1,078) were asked to guess each label using a forced, binary choice and thus had a 51.9% chance of being correct if always guessing female (there were slightly more female authors in our dataset). Participants completed the task an average of 21 times.

Results.

Participants with higher trait AOT were more likely to assign the correct gender to authors. The odds ratio estimate was 1.064 (95% confidence interval [CI] = 1.014, 1.117) indicating that for each 1-unit increase in raters’ AOT (on the 1–7 scale), each guess was 1.064 times more likely to be correct; a guess by a rater whose AOT was 7 was almost 50% more likely to be correct than a rater whose AOT was 1. Overall, participants’ guesses were correct 75.70% of the time.

2.3 Study 1b: Age

Tweets from 826 authors were collected; authors reported their own ages in an online survey. Because age is a scalar variable, participants’ accuracy was not measured on a binary correct/incorrect scale. Instead, they were asked to guess each author’s age in years, and their accuracy was assessed as the absolute value of the difference between the author’s actual age and the participant’s guess. Participants (n = 691) completed the task an average of 11 times.

Results.

Participants with higher trait AOT were overall more accurate at guessing authors’ ages, b = −0.237, p = .002, indicating that for each unit increase in AOT, each guess was approximately a quarter of a year closer to the author’s actual age. Overall, participants’ guesses diverged from authors’ actual ages by an average of 7.25 years.

2.4 Study 1c: Education level

Education information was available for 900 Twitter authors, based on self-reported occupations in the user description field on Twitter (Reference Preoţiuc-Pietro, Lampos and AletrasPreotiuc-Pietro, Lampos & Aletras, 2015). We mapped an estimated education level (no bachelor’s degree, bachelor’s degree or equivalent, advanced degree), based on the education level required for occupations listed in the UK Social Occupation Classification (SOC, 2000). The three groups were evenly split. Participants (n = 482) were asked to assign one of the three labels. Participants completed the task an average of 49 times and had a 33.33% chance of being correct.

Results.

Trait AOT was positively associated with correct categorizations. The odds ratio estimate was 1.141 (95% CI = 1.065, 1.223), indicating that for each one-unit increase in rater AOT, the likelihood of a guess being correct was 1.141 times more likely to be correct. In general, guesses were correct 54% of the time.

2.5 Study 1d: Political orientation

Political orientation (Republican or Democrat) could be determined for 2,500 Twitter authors, based on their patterns of following political leaders on Twitter in August of 2015. We selected four politicians associated with the American Democratic party (@SenSanders, @JoeBiden, @CoryBooker, @JohnKerry) and four politicians associated with the American Republican party (@marcorubio, @tedcruz, @RandPaul, @RealBenCarson). Authors labelled “Democrats” followed all four of the Democrat politicians and none of the Republicans, while authors labelled “Republicans” followed all four of the Republican politicians and none of the Democrats. Participants (n = 943) were asked to guess each author’s political orientation; they had a 50% chance of being correct. Participants performed the task an average of 23 times.

Results.

Raters’ AOT was strongly related to the accuracy of their guesses. The estimated odds ratio was 1.240 (95% CI = 1.154, 1.333): for each one-unit increase in AOT, raters’ guesses were 1.240 times more likely to be correct. Overall, guesses were correct 81.69% of the time.

2.6 Discussion

Across four sub-studies, correctly assessing social media authors’ traits was significantly associated with raters’ AOT. In other words, people with higher AOT were more skilled at drawing correct conclusions about people solely based on social media behavior.

These results suggest that being motivated to think deeply and search for new information can help overcome the ambiguous aspects of much online communication. Although the tasks in Study 1 were not very difficult -- participants generally performed better than chance across the board -- participants higher in AOT were better at using the cues in online text to draw accurate conclusions about authors.

In other words, being low in actively open-minded thinking was associated with lower accuracy about social categorizations online, despite the relative ease of these categorizations overall; even very basic kinds of social cognition were hindered by the combination of low AOT and the online social media setting. Therefore, the decision-making and reasoning benefits to AOT extend to social perception in a social media setting.

3 Study 2

Study 1 established AOT’s relationship with how people perceive and respond to social media information. Study 2 explored how AOT is related to social media behaviors directly; that is, how it is reflected in people’s actions online. To create a broad picture of social media behaviors, we considered three dimensions: platform usage, language use, and profile image choice. While the exploratory nature of Study 2 kept us from making specific hypotheses about our results, we had particular interest in AOT’s relationship to closed-mindedness and thoughtlessness.

3.1 Participants

Participants (n = 1,464)Footnote 4 were recruited via online platforms (Amazon Mechanical Turk and Qualtrics). Because of our focus on uncontrolled, field data, we expected the effect sizes of our results to be somewhat small. Using the standards of a small effect size (ρ = .10), a two-tailed test, power of .80, and α = .05, G*Power determined a minimum sample size of 779 participants (Reference Faul, Erdfelder, Buchner and LangFaul, Buchner & Lang, 2009). Because data-driven language analysis requires especially large samples (e.g., Reference Schwartz and UngarSchwartz & Ungar, 2015), we included all participants we could access.

Mean age was 31.1 years old (SD = 11.03, range = 13, 72), and 67.9% (994) of participants were female. For our analysis, we downloaded up to the most recent 3,200 public tweets using the Twitter API per the API restrictions. Participants had posted an average of 1,109 tweets in total.

3.2 Methods and materials

Participants were asked to share their Twitter handles and to complete a basic demographic survey including age, gender, and a 9-item version of the Actively Open-minded Thinking scale (Reference Haran, Ritov and MellersHaran et al., 2013; Cronbach’s α = .71). We then collected three types of information about each user: platform related behaviors, language use, and profile image.

Platform related behaviors.

One approach to gaining insight about online behavior involves querying the general ways that people tend to use the social media platform itself, such as the number of posts they make in a day, the average length of their posts, and the frequency of retweeting (i.e., passing along someone else’s tweet to a new audience). These kinds of behaviors have been shown to be associated both with demographic characteristics (e.g., Reference Preoţiuc-Pietro, Volkova, Lampos, Bachrach and AletrasPreoţiuc-Pietro, Volkova, Lampos, Bachrach, & Aletras, 2015) and personality traits (e.g., Reference Farnadi, Zoghbi, Moens and De CockFarnadi, Zoghbi, Moens & de Cock, 2013; Reference Quercia, Kosinski, Stillwell and CrowcroftQuercia, Kosinski, Stillwell, & Crowcroft, 2011).

Table 1 indicates the behaviors measured for each user, grouped by type.Footnote 5 Most variables had extremely skewed distributions. For instance, the average tweets per day was close to three, but the most prolific user posted over 300 times per day. We thus log-transformed or logit-transformed each variable (proportion variables were logit-transformed; count variables were log transformed; cases of 0 or 1 for logit-transformed variables were deleted (Reference AitchisonAitchison, 1986)). Analyses were a series of univariate, linear regressions predicting each behavior from AOT.

Table 1: Twitter behavioral measures and descriptive information, grouped by type.

Language use.

A second type of social media behavior is language use: the words and topics that characterize different users. The large-scale text data found in social media make it possible for data-driven analyses to automatically identify words that tend to be used by people high or low in certain traits (Kern et al., 2016, Park et al., 2015).

Language analysis in psychology has most commonly been performed using the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC), a set of theory-driven dictionaries which categorize words in psychologically meaningful ways (Reference Pennebaker, Francis and BoothPennebaker, Francis & Booth, 2001; Reference Tausczik and PennebakerTausczik & Pennebaker, 2010). For example, the ‘Positive Emotion’ LIWC dictionary contains words such as ‘happy’, ‘good’, ‘love’ and ‘lol’. By counting how often words in the dictionary are used by a certain group, one can determine which group expresses more positive emotions in their writing. LIWC has been used to reveal group differences in spontaneous language: for instance, men are more likely than women to use articles such as the or an, while women are more likely to use social words and first-person pronouns (Newman, Groom, Handelman & Pennebaker, 2008).

However, methods based on Natural Language Processing can be used to discover a wider breadth of words and topics associated with a specific group or trait, especially in the context of the diversity in language use that exists in social media. Using Natural Language Processing and statistical analysis, one can identify all words or phrases that are associated with a given trait (Park et al., 2015). To aid interpretation, we followed the procedure introduced in Reference Preoţiuc-Pietro, Volkova, Lampos, Bachrach and AletrasPreoţiuc-Pietro, Lampos, and Aletras (2015) which automatically groups words that are semantically or syntactically similar into clusters. Specifically, we used the GloVe algorithm (Reference Pennington, Socher and ManningPennington, Socher & Manning, 2014), which generates 1,000 discrete sets of words called topics, with each word belonging to a single topic. We then used these 1,000 topics to quantify the language use of users on Twitter by aggregating all the words in a user’s tweets and representing the user as a distribution of the fraction of words which belong to each topic. All analyses were performed on the topics, and not individual words.

To find the most discriminative features of AOT, we then correlated each individual topic with the AOT score of the authors. Because our method involves calculating thousands of independent, univariate regression equations, all language analysis was corrected using the Simes p-correction (Reference SimesSimes, 1986). Our sample size is appropriate for our analyses and fits accepted standards of big-data language correlations (e.g., Reference Eichstaedt, Kern, Tobolsky, Yaden, Schwartz, Park, Smith, Buffone, Iwry, Seligman and UngarEichstaedt et al., 2017).

Profile image.

Twitter allows users to represent themselves with profile pictures; these pictures are a form of self-presentation online and are related to individual differences of users (e.g., Reference Liu, Preotiuc-Pietro, Samani, Moghaddam and UngarLiu, Preotiuc-Pietro, Samani, Moghaddam & Ungar, 2016). Profile images were automatically downloaded on the same day as the tweets using the public Twitter API. Out of 1,474 users, 104 users had the default Twitter profile image and 16 users’ pictures could not be opened, leaving 1,354 profile images for analysis.

A number of high-level features of the pictures were automatically extracted. These features describe basic aspects of the pictures’ composition and content. The analyzed features were:

  • Brightness: the amount of light in the picture, ranging from 0 (totally black) to 255 (totally white) (M = 116.01, SD = 41.02)

  • Contrast: the relative variation of luminance, ranging from 0 (entirely dark or light) to 57,303 (M = 10032.70, SD = 7034.18)

  • Saturation: the level of vividness and chromatic purity in the picture, ranging from 0 (very sharp distinctions between objects) to 1 (no distinction between objects) (M = 0.30, SD = 0.17)

  • Colorfulness: the amount of shades of red, green, and blue compared to shades of grey, ranging from 0 (entirely grayscale) to 33,394 (complete color) (M = 10401.13, SD = 4298.50).

  • Faces: the presence or absence of at least one photographed human face.Footnote 6 75.3% of analyzed profile images contained at least one human face.

3.3 Results

Participants’ AOT had a significant, positive correlation with age, r(1464) = .100, p < .001, 95% CI = .05, .15. Men’s AOT scores (M = 4.91) were slightly higher than women’s (M = 4.64), t(1474) = 6.15, p < .001, r = .158. Because of these relationships, age and gender were entered as covariates for all results presented in Study 2.

Platform related behaviors.

In general, higher AOT was associated with less frequent tweeting but longer tweets. Users high in AOT had fewer followers and followed fewer people themselves, but their tweets were liked more often. They also were less likely to have geo-location enabled, suggesting that they tweet on desktops or laptops rather than handheld devices. We uncovered no relationship between AOT and users’ hashtags and retweeting behaviors. Full results for platform behaviors are presented in Tables 2a-2d.

Table 2: Aot’s relationships, above and beyond age and gender.

Note:

* indicates p < .05

** indicates p < .01

Language use.

Interpretable and coherent patterns in language were apparent for both high and low levels of AOT. The twelve topics most strongly correlated with low AOT are presented in Figure 2, and the twelve topics most strongly correlated with high AOT are presented in Figure 3. The topics are visually presented such that the most frequently used words in our dataset and thus the ones most likely to drive the association are larger. The number of topics for each direction was chosen to present an illustrative range of responses. For ease of interpretation, we have arranged the topics post-hoc into categories; these categories and their labels are open to interpretation, but they highlight clearly distinct linguistic patterns of users high or low in AOT.

Figure 2: The 12 topics most strongly negatively correlated with AOT. All topics significant at Simes-corrected p < .01. Size of word within topic indicates frequency within data.

Figure 3: The 12 topics most strongly positively associated with AOT. All topics significant at Simes-corrected p < .01. Size of word within topic indicates frequency within data.

Users low in AOT used topics consisting of informal words that are often used in conversational settings (“Casual Speech” category in Figure 2) or that display a tendency of positively referencing valued personal relationships (such as expressions of gratitude, birthday wishes, positive personal traits, or family and friends). Overall, low AOT involved a focus on personal or social interests, expressed through casual language use.

In contrast, high AOT was associated with elevated diction and broad ideas. Individuals high in AOT used relatively sophisticated words, particularly modifiers such as adverbs, suggesting a tolerance for shades of grey and nuance. They also described wide-focus issues and ideas such as religion, political ideologies, education, nature and imagination. Users high in AOT also expressed their views on social issues such as injustice and economic inequality, and their consequences and outcomes. One topic specifically consisted of ways to quote or allude to other people’s statements or points of view (e.g., “referred,” “admitted,” “stated”).

Profile image.

Users high in AOT were less likely to have human faces in their profile images. Because a portrait of oneself is the most common and expected kind of profile picture, AOT predicts a more unorthodox manner of visually presenting oneself. AOT was not significantly related to any other descriptive features of profile image. (See Table 3 for full results.)

Table 3: AOT’s relationship with profile picture features, above and beyond age and gender.

** indicates p < .01.

3.4 Discussion

Overall, our data-driven methods revealed a distinct picture of high AOT users as thoughtful in their expression, intellectually curious, oriented towards the big picture rather than personal issues, and somewhat unorthodox in their manner of self-presentation. In their language, users high in AOT demonstrated a focus on broad, abstract ideas rather than the immediate situation. AOT was positively associated with a willingness to talk about unpleasant things, particularly perceived systematic injustices like “Islamophobia” or “patriarchy”, and it was negatively associated with happy, optimistic communication and references to friends and family. This may be another instance in which people high in AOT are willing to expose themselves to information others would rather ignore. It may also reflect a left wing political orientation, which is consistent with previous research connecting political liberalism with openness (Reference JostJost, 2017).

Notably, participants high in AOT were also characterized by a focus on big picture ideas and concepts, precisely the level of construal that people are prone to avoid on digital media compared to analog (Reference Kaufman and FlanaganKaufmann & Flanagan, 2016). Low-AOT users, on the other hand, were more likely to discuss their immediate relationships. Because AOT’s definition includes an orientation towards an expanded breadth of information, it may help people to avoid “losing the forest for the trees” online. Finally, high-AOT users were more likely to use words referring to others’ speech or quotes. This finding directly demonstrates an interest in others’ perspectives.

Although the focus of this paper is on AOT’s effects, it is plausible that these specific effects may be more directly related with other individual differences correlated with AOT, particularly education level (Reference Carpenter, Preotiuc-Pietro, Flekova, Giorgi, Hagan, Kern, Buffone, Ungar and SeligmanCarpenter, et al., 2016).

Users high in AOT were less likely to visually present themselves in the conventional manner: they were less likely to follow the typical behavior of having a human face in their profile image, instead using other kinds of pictures, such as cartoon characters, pets, or landscapes. Also, they posted less frequently but made longer posts. Importantly, their posts were also more likely to be liked by others, suggesting social benefits to having high AOT on social media.

In general, these results are consistent with ways that AOT manifests in other contexts (e.g., Baron, in press; Stanovich, 2017). We do not mean to imply that the behaviors negatively associated with AOT are necessarily maladaptive; in particular, positive personal relationships are a key aspect of well-being (e.g,. Ryff, 1995). However, high-AOT users are specifically distinguished by their avoidance of the fast-pace and immediacy of much online communication, both in terms of frequency of posting and the breadth of their objects of discussion, consistent with a trait tendency to cognitively reflect (Reference Campitelli and LabollitaCampitelli & Labollita, 2010).

It is important to reiterate that our methods in study 2 are correlational, and therefore we cannot make any claims of causality between AOT and these behaviors. Even more important, however, is to acknowledge the difficulty of isolating the single individual difference most directly related to behavioural outcomes. As we discuss above, AOT is conceptually similar to and strongly intercorrelated with a number of other constructs (e.g. need for cognition and need for closure); it also is related to demographic variables such as gender and education level. For these two reasons, it is beyond the scope of this paper to determine whether AOT (or any other construct) is the fundamental causal factor of these outcomes. Rather, our focus is on providing insight to the potential psychological reasons a person may use social media in different ways.

4 General discussion

Across two studies, we demonstrated that Actively Open-Minded Thinking was associated with benefits both in responding to and at creating social media content. In Study 1, AOT was found to enable more accurate assessments of other users, based solely on social media text: in other words, high-AOT people were better able to interpret and reason about social media text. In Study 2, we composed a three-dimensional picture of how AOT affects online behavior in general social media tendencies, language use, and profile image selection. AOT was associated with more thoughtful, better-liked tweets; high-AOT people were more skillful at writing tweets that people react well to. Although these results do not suggest that high AOT is beneficial in all situations that may arise on social media, these results are a first step for studying individual differences that allow social media users to navigate the potentially overwhelming amount of information inherent to social media platforms such as Twitter (e.g., Reference Jones, Ravid and RafaeliJones, Ravid & Rafaeli, 2004). AOT may serve as an “antidote” to the detail-level, fast-paced, inattentive mindset facilitated by digital social media (e.g., Reference Jones, Ravid and RafaeliJones, Ravid & Rafaeli, 2004; Reference Kaufman and FlanaganKaufman & Flanagan, 2016; Reference Zhong, Hardin and SunZhong, Hardin & Sun, 2011).

One particularly compelling future direction involves analyzing the effect of trait AOT on how people identify or deal with the increasingly troubling problem of biased or false information available on social media (e.g., Reference MaheshwariMaheshwari, 2016; El-Bermawi, 2016). AOT may moderate the extent to which people are open to new perspectives and viewpoints instead of treating their social media spaces as “echo chambers” which merely reinforce and ossify their pre-existing views and values (Reference Barberá, Jost, Nagler, Tucker and BonneauBarbera et al. 2015; Reference Dehghani, Johnson, Hoover, Sagi, Garten, Parmar, Vaisey, Iliev and GrahamDehghani et al. 2016). Also, users motivated to think more deeply about information may be more likely to recognize and ignore unsubstantiated or false information online (e.g., Reference Qazvinian, Rosengren, Radev and MeiQazvinian, Rosengren, Radev & Mei, 2011; Reference Starbird, Maddock, Orand, Achterman and MasonStarbird et al., 2014). A promising step in this direction has been recently reported by Bronstein and colleagues (2018), who found that users’ AOT is positively associated with their ability to distinguish ’fake news’ headlines from real headlines. The skills involved in being a good citizen, community member, and consumer of information increasingly are needed on social media; a closed-minded mindset can interfere with these skills. Our studies provide an important first look at traits that facilitate them on text-based social media. Eventually, it may be possible to use these findings to automatically track AOT and its related behaviors over time using supervised learning techniques (Reference Mcauliffe and BleiMcauliffe & Blei, 2008).

Our studies do have limitations. For ethical reasons, we recruited only Twitter users who willingly shared their Twitter handles, which means our sample may not be fully representative of the general population; likewise, the Twitter population itself is a non-representative sample of the English-speaking population. Also, despite Twitter’s popularity, its format is primarily short messages, and so AOT’s effect may not be identical to those on other social media platforms, which allow longer messages or have a greater emphasis on pictures or video. In the future, it will be important to extend these methods to other forms of social media. Furthermore, as we stated above, our interpretations in Study 2 were made in light of AOT’s effect on the outcomes, but it is possible other traits or characteristics correlated with AOT may be more direct factors (or they may exert their effects in part through AOT itself).

The benefits of widespread mediated communication are obvious: it connects people to new communities, facilitates the convenient and fast spread of information, and allows people to start to build relationships that otherwise could not exist. However, the downsides — information overload, an overly fast pace of communication, distant and abstract communication partners — can be dangerous. Our studies begin to suggest that an orientation toward thinking deeply and openly allows users to sidestep some of these problems and have online interactions characterized by depth, accuracy, and openness.

Footnotes

This work was supported by a grant from the Templeton Religion Trust (ID #TRT0048).

1 For a longer list, see Baron (in press), and Reference StanovichStanovich (2016).

2 For quality control, we interspersed several authors who directly stated their group category (e.g., a male author saying “My beard is almost to the point where I can make other men jealous of my sweet beard”). If participants misidentified two of these unambiguous authors, they were unable to participate further and their data are not included in our results. 16, 8, 20, and 40 raters failed the attention checks in Studies 1a, 1b, 1c, and 1d, respectively.

3 Because each response is nested both in the author of the tweet and the worker who rated the tweet, each of these datasets are cross-classified. However, low ICCs at the worker level (0.05 – 0.005) provided insufficient evidence of dependency to require nesting, compared to the high ICCs observed at the author level (0.19 – 0.62). As the ICC for cross-classified models is calculated pooling variances across all levels, this does not imply a lack of any significant effects at the worker level – merely that the variance at the author level is far larger.

4 This sample was taken from a larger set of 4026 participants. Participants were eliminated from analysis if they chose not to share their Twitter handle with the researchers or if they listed a handle with over 5000 followers or if the handle was a verified account (suggesting that they listed a celebrity’s account) or if they failed to complete the survey.

5 To keep low-activity users from skewing the sample, we eliminated from all analysis involving text-derived features 275 participants who had posted less than 1000 words in total.

6 The researchers originally used an automatic face recognition program, Face++ (faceplusplus.com), but it was unreliable, falsely rejecting images that clearly contained faces. Thus, two human raters coded each picture for the presence of faces. For the 39 pictures on which they disagreed (mostly due to heavily shadowed figures or pictures where slivers of a face are visible), a third rater broke the tie.

References

Aitchison, J. (1986) The Statistical Analysis of Compositional Data. New York, NY: Chapman and Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakshy, E., Messing, S., & Adamic, L. A. (2015). Exposure to ideologically diverse news and opinion on Facebook. Science, 348, 11301132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barberá, P., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., Tucker, J. A., & Bonneau, R. (2015). Tweeting from left to right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber? Psychological Science, 26(10), 15311542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, J. (1993). Why teach thinking? — An essay. Applied Psychology, 42(3), 191214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (in press). Actively open-minded thinking in politics. Cognition.Google Scholar
Bronstein, M. V., Pennycook, G., Bear, A., Rand, D. G., & Cannon, T. D. (in press). Belief in fake news is associated with delusionality, dogmatism, religious fundamentalism, and reduced analytic thinking. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition.Google Scholar
Burger, J. D., Henderson, J., Kim, G., & Zarrella, G. (2011, July). Discriminating gender on Twitter. In Proceedings of the conference on empirical methods in natural language processing (pp. 13011309). Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 116131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campitelli, G., & Labollita, M. (2010). Correlations of cognitive reflection with judgments and choices. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(3), 182191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, J., Preotiuc-Pietro, D., Flekova, L., Giorgi, S., Hagan, C., Kern, M. L., Buffone, A., Ungar, L., & Seligman, M. E. (2016). Real men don’t say “cute.” Using automatic language analysis to isolate inaccurate aspects of stereotypes. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(3), 310322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dai, J., Li, Z., & Rocke, D. (2006). Hierarchical logistic regression modeling with SAS GLIMMIX. In Proceedings of the Thirty-first Annual SAS Users Group International Conference. Cary, North Carolina: SAS Institute Inc.Google Scholar
Darbyshire, D., Kirk, C., Wall, H. J., & Kaye, L. K. (2016). Don’t judge a (face) book by its cover: Exploring judgement accuracy of others’ personality on Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 58, 380387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehghani, M., Johnson, K., Hoover, J., Sagi, E., Garten, J., Parmar, N. J., Vaisey, S., Iliev, R., & Graham, J. (2016). Purity homophily in social networks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(3), 366375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duggan, G. B., & Payne, S. J. (2011, May). Skim reading by satisficing: evidence from eye tracking. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 1141—1150). ACM.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eichstaedt, J. C., Kern, M. L., Tobolsky, V., Yaden, D. B., Schwartz, H. A., Park, G., Smith, L. K., Buffone, A., Iwry, J., Seligman, M. E. P & Ungar, L. H.. (2017). From Hypothesis-Testing to Hypothesis-generation with text analysis: A review and quantitative comparison of open and closed-vocabulary approaches. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
El-Bermawy, M. (2016, November 18). Your filter bubble is destroying democracy [blogpost]. Wired Magazine. Retrieved from https://www.wired.com/2016/11/filter-bubble-destroying-democracy/Google Scholar
Epley, N., & Kruger, J. (2005). When what you type isn’t what they read: The perseverance of stereotypes and expectancies over e-mail. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41(4), 414422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farnadi, G., Zoghbi, S., Moens, M. F., & De Cock, M. (2013). Recognising personality traits using facebook status updates. In Proceedings of the Workshop on Computational Personality Recognition (WCPR13) at the 7th International AAAI Conference on Weblogs and Social Media (ICWSM) 1418.Google Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 11491160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flaxman, S., Goel, S., & Rao, J. M. (2016). Filter bubbles, echo chambers, and online news consumption. Public Opinion Quarterly, 80, 298320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, D. J., Nyhan, B., & Reifler, J. (2017). The nature and origins of misperceptions: Understanding false and unsupported beliefs about politics. Political Psychology, 38(S1), 127150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haran, U., Ritov, I., & Mellers, B. A. (2013). The role of actively open-minded thinking in information acquisition, accuracy, and calibration. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(3), 188201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmuth, B., Gouhier, T. C., Scyphers, S., & Mocarski, J. (2016). Trust, tribalism and tweets: has political polarization made science a “wedge issue”?. Climate Change Responses, 3, 317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, Q., Ravid, G., & Rafaeli, S. (2004). Information overload and the message dynamics of online interaction spaces: A theoretical model and empirical exploration. Information Systems Research, 15(2), 194210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J. T. (2017). Ideological asymmetries and the essence of political psychology. Political Psychology, 38, 167208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, G., & Flanagan, M. (2016, May). High-low split: Divergent cognitive construal levels triggered by digital and non-digital platforms. In Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 2773—2777). ACM.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kern, M. L., Eichstaedt, J. C., Schwartz, H. A., Dziurzynski, L., Ungar, L. H., Stillwell, D. J., Kosinski, M., Ramones, S. M., & Seligman, M. E. (2014). The online social self: An open vocabulary approach to personality. Assessment, 21(2), 158169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruger, J., Epley, N., Parker, J., & Ng, Z. W. (2005). Egocentrism over e-mail: Can we communicate as well as we think? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(6), 925936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). The psychology of closed mindedness. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Liu, L., Preotiuc-Pietro, D., Samani, Z. R., Moghaddam, M. E., & Ungar, L. (2016). Analyzing personality through social media profile picture choice. In Proceedings of the Tenth International AAAI Conference on Weblogs and Social Media (ICWSM), 211—220.Google Scholar
Maheshwari, S. (2016, November 20). How Fake News Goes Viral: A Case Study. New York Times. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2016/11/20/business/media/how-fake-news-spreads.html.Google Scholar
Mcauliffe, J. D., & Blei, D. M. (2008). Supervised topic models. In Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (pp. 121128).Google Scholar
Mellers, B., Stone, E., Atanasov, P., Rohrbaugh, N., Metz, S. E., Ungar, L., Bisop, M., Horowitz, M., Merkle, E. & Tetlock, P. (2015). The psychology of intelligence analysis: Drivers of prediction accuracy in world politics. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 21, 114.Google ScholarPubMed
Okdie, B. M., Guadagno, R. E., Bernieri, F. J., Geers, A. L., & Mclarney-Vesotski, A. R. (2011). Getting to know you: face-to-face versus online interactions. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 153159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, M., Groom, C., Handelman, L. & Pennebaker, J. (2009). Gender differences in language use: An analysis of 14,000 text samples. Discourse Processes 45(3), 211236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, G., Schwartz, H. A., Eichstaedt, J. C., Kern, M. L., Stillwell, D. J., Kosinski, M., Ungar, L. H., & Seligman, M. E. (2014). Automatic personality assessment through social media language. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 934952.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennebaker, J., Francis, M. & Booth, R. (2001). Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC 2001. Mahway: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Pennington, J., Socher, R., & Manning, C. D. (2014). Glove: Global vectors for word representation. In Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP), 1532—1543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preoţiuc-Pietro, D., Volkova, S., Lampos, V., Bachrach, Y., Aletras, N. (2015). Studying user income through language, behaviour and affect in social media. PLoS ONE 10(9).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preoţiuc-Pietro, D., Lampos, V., & Aletras, N. (2015). An Analysis of the user occupational class through Twitter content. In Proceedings of the 53rd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics and the 7th International Joint Conference on Natural Language Processing (ACL), 1754—1764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qazvinian, V., Rosengren, E., Radev, D. R., & Mei, Q. (2011). Rumor has it: Identifying misinformation in microblogs. In Proceedings of the Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP), 1589—1599.Google Scholar
Quinn, P. C., Yahr, J., Kuhn, A., Slater, A. M., & Pascalis, O. (2002). Representation of the Gender of Human Faces by Infants: A Preference for Female. Perception, 31, 11091121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quercia, D., Kosinski, M., Stillwell, D., & Crowcroft, J. (2011). Our Twitter profiles, our selves: Predicting personality with Twitter. In Privacy, Security, Risk and Trust (PASSAT) and 2011 IEEE Third International Conference on Social Computing (SocialCom), IEEE. 180—185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sap, M., Park, G., Eichstaedt, J. C., Kern, M. L., Stillwell, D. J., Kosinski, M., Ungar, L. H., & Schwartz, H. A. (2014). Developing Age and Gender Predictive Lexica over Social Media. In Proceedings of the 2014 Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (EMNLP), 1146—1151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, H. A., & Ungar, L. H. (2015). Data-driven content analysis of social media: a systematic overview of automated methods. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 659, 7894.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shin, J., Jian, L., Driscoll, K., & Bar, F. (2016). Political rumoring on Twitter during the 2012 US presidential election: Rumor diffusion and correction. New Media & Society, 19, 12141235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simes, R. J. (1986). An improved Bonferroni procedure for multiple tests of significance. Biometrika, 73(3), 751754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sparrow, B., Liu, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2011). Google effects on memory: Cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science, 333, 776778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1986). Reducing social context cues: Electronic mail in organizational communication. Management Science, 32(11), 14921512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2016). The comprehensive assessment of rational thinking. Educational Psychologist, 51(1), 2334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(2), 342357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starbird, K., Maddock, J., Orand, M., Achterman, P., & Mason, R. M. (2014). Rumors, false flags, and digital vigilantes: Misinformation on Twitter after the 2013 Boston Marathon bombing. In Proceedings of the iConference 2014, 654—662.Google Scholar
Tausczik, Y. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29(1), 2454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The Office for National Statistics (n.d.) Standard occupational classification 2010 (SOC2010). Retrieved from http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/guide-method/classifications/current-standard-classifications/soc2010/index.html (1 December 2016).Google Scholar
Thompson, M. M., Naccarato, M. E., Parker, K. C., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2001). The personal need for structure and personal fear of invalidity measures: Historical perspectives, current applications, and future directions. In Moskowitz, G.B. (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: The Princeton symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition (pp. 1939). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Tranel, D., Damasio, A. R., & Damasio, H. (1988). Intact recognition of facial expression, gender, and age in patients with impaired recognition of face identity. Neurology, 38, 690690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tskhay, K. O., & Rule, N. O. (2014). Perceptions of personality in text-based media and osn: A meta-Analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 49, 2530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaccari, C., Valeriani, A., Barberá, P., Jost, J. T., Nagler, J., & Tucker, J. A. (2016). Of echo chambers and contrarian clubs: Exposure to political disagreement among German and Italian users of twitter. Social Media+Society, 2(3), 124.Google Scholar
Vega, V., McCracken, K., Nass, C. and Labs, L. (2008, May). Multitasking effects on visual working memory, working memory and executive control. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the International Communication Association, Montreal, Quebec, Canada.Google Scholar
Von Luxburg, U. (2007). A tutorial on spectral clustering. Statistics and Computing, 17(4), 395416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal effects in computer-mediated interaction: A relational perspective. Communication Research, 19, 5290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walther, J. B. (1993). Impression development in computer-mediated interaction. Western Journal of Communication, 57, 381398.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of personality and social psychology, 67, 10491062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhong, B., Hardin, M., & Sun, T. (2011). Less effortful thinking leads to more social networking? The associations between the use of social network sites and personality traits. Computers in Human Behavior, 27, 12651271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1: Sample task for Study 1.

Figure 1

Table 1: Twitter behavioral measures and descriptive information, grouped by type.

Figure 2

Table 2: Aot’s relationships, above and beyond age and gender.

Figure 3

Figure 2: The 12 topics most strongly negatively correlated with AOT. All topics significant at Simes-corrected p < .01. Size of word within topic indicates frequency within data.

Figure 4

Figure 3: The 12 topics most strongly positively associated with AOT. All topics significant at Simes-corrected p < .01. Size of word within topic indicates frequency within data.

Figure 5

Table 3: AOT’s relationship with profile picture features, above and beyond age and gender.

Supplementary material: File

Carpenter et al. supplementary material

Carpenter et al. supplementary material 1
Download Carpenter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 243.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Carpenter et al. supplementary material

Carpenter et al. supplementary material 2
Download Carpenter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 660.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Carpenter et al. supplementary material

Carpenter et al. supplementary material 3
Download Carpenter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 218.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Carpenter et al. supplementary material

Carpenter et al. supplementary material 4
Download Carpenter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 609.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Carpenter et al. supplementary material

Carpenter et al. supplementary material 5
Download Carpenter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 920 Bytes