Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:21:37.689Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evolution of the interpersonal conflict paradigm

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2023

Mandeep K. Dhami*
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Henrik Olsson
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Human Development
*
*Send correspondence to Mandeep K. Dhami, University of Cambridge, Institute of Criminology, Sidgwick Avenue, Cambridge, England, UK, CB3 9DT. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Using Brunswik’s (1952) lens model framework, Hammond (1965) proposed interpersonal conflict theory to explain the nature, source, and resolution of disagreement or “cognitive conflict” between parties performing judgment tasks. An early review by Brehmer (1976) highlighted the potential of this approach in, for example, understanding the structure of cognitive conflicts, and the effect of task and person variables on judgment policy change and conflict resolution. However, our bibliographic and content reviews from 1976 to the present day demonstrate that research on cognitive conflict using the lens model has declined sharply, while research on “task conflict” has grown dramatically. There has also been a shift to less theoretical precision and methodological rigor. We discuss possible reasons for these developments, and suggest ways in which lens model research on cognitive conflict can be revitalized by borrowing from recent theoretical and methodological advances in the field of judgment and decision making.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
The authors license this article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License.
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors [2008] This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

It was during the cognitive revolution in psychology and the cold war period in political history when Reference HammondHammond (1965) proposed that conflicts between parties performing judgment tasks could be viewed as purely cognitive, thus making it unnecessary to examine the motivations and values of conflicting parties as social psychologists might do. In interpersonal conflict (IPC) theory, Reference HammondHammond (1965) outlined how this cognitive conflict could be construed within Reference BrunswikBrunswik’s (1952) lens model framework, as well as the experimental methods that researchers could use to study the nature, source, and resolution of disagreement between parties performing judgment tasks. Briefly, cognitive conflict represents differences in how parties conceptualize the solution to a problem. For instance, different parties may have different policies for solving a judgment problem in terms of the information they rely on. Inconsistency in how parties apply their judgment policies can also lead to disagreement. Thus, according to IPC theory, parties striving to make a joint judgment on the same task could conflict because they disagree in principle (in that they have different policies for how to solve the problem) and/or in practice (in that they are inconsistent in the application of their policies). Importantly, while cognitive conflict is different from conflict caused by motivational and value differences among parties, cognitive differences can evolve into motivational and value-laden conflicts. Under these circumstances, the underlying cognitive differences can be very difficult to detect and resolve.

An early review of research using IPC theory published in Psychological Bulletin by Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) highlighted the potential of this approach in advancing our understanding of cognitive conflict in both laboratory and real world settings. Despite this, since that time, IPC theory appears to have featured little in the growing field of judgment and decision making (JDM). For example, in a historical review of theories in the field, Reference Goldstein, Hogarth, Goldstein and HogarthGoldstein and Hogarth (1997) provide only a passing mention of IPC theory when considering developments in judgment research. The need for conflict theories, including cognitive conflict, seems apparent in an era characterized by international terrorism where, amongst other things, parties disagree about the level of threat, and how to manage and minimize it (Reference MandelMandel, 2005), and in an era characterized by a movement towards greater use of alternative dispute resolution. In fact, today, a theory of cognitive conflict could benefit from recent theoretical and methodological advances in the field of JDM. For instance, JDM researchers have shown that individuals are likely to use simple process models when performing judgment tasks (e.g., Reference Dhami and HarriesDhami & Harries, 2001; Reference FrijdaGarcia-Retamero & Dhami, in press; Reference Rieskamp, Hoffrage, Gigerenzer and ToddRieskamp & Hoffrage, 1999); and that non-cognitive factors such as emotions may affect how individuals make judgments (e.g., see Reference Loewenstein, Lerner, Davidson, Goldsmith and SchererLoewenstein & Lerner, 2003). In addition, researchers have employed new tools such as virtual environments and computer simulations when studying judgment behavior (e.g., see Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1992; Reference Mosler, Schwarz, Ammann and GutscherMosler, Schwarz, Ammann, & Gutscher, 2001).

Our goal is to evaluate the evolution of IPC theory from its inception to the present day. Specifically, we consider how research on cognitive conflict has developed in terms of its theoretical underpinnings and methodological stance, and we review the findings of empirical research on cognitive conflict. Our goal is modest in that we focus our efforts on cognitive conflict as it directly emerged from the IPC paradigm and related lens model framework. A review of conflict theories and research more generally are not within the scope of the present paper. The article is organized into three main parts. First, we consider the emergence of IPC theory from 1965 to 1976 by outlining its roots in Brunswikian psychology, the experimental methodology employed, and early research findings. Second, from 1976 to the present day, we trace the evolution of IPC theory and cognitive conflict research by conducting bibliographic and content reviews of publications that cite central articles by Reference HammondHammond (1965) and Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976). Finally, we discuss the future of IPC theory and cognitive conflict research by considering opportunities for theoretical advancement and methodological innovation offered currently in the field of JDM. We hope these will inspire future researchers.

2 Interpersonal conflict theory from Hammond, 1965 to Brehmer, 1976

In this section, we review the development of IPC theory from 1965 to 1976. We consider the roots of Reference HammondHammond’s (1965) IPC theory in Reference BrunswikBrunswik’s (1952) lens model framework, the experimental methods proposed to study cognitive conflict, and the main findings of the early body of research on cognitive conflict as reviewed by Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976).

2.1 Cognitive conflict and the lens model

Using Reference BrunswikBrunswik’s (1952) lens model framework as a basis for theory and method, Reference HammondHammond (1965) introduced IPC theory for understanding the nature, source, and resolution of cognitive conflict.Footnote 1 Figure 1 presents an adaptation of the lens model to the study of cognitive conflict (simplified for our purposes; see also Reference CookseyCooksey, 1996). For readers unfamiliar with this framework it is worth pointing out that the model shows a collection of cues diverging from a criterion in the environment, and these cues can be used by the different parties to predict the criterion. To the extent that a party’s cue utilization validities match the ecological validities of the cues, the party will be able to achieve the criterion (i.e., make accurate decisions). Conflict can also occur in the absence of an outcome criterion, and to the extent that the cue utilization validities differ across the different parties they will be in conflict (i.e., disagree in their decisions). In the real world, the environment is often complex in that there are multiple, inter-correlated cues that are only probabilistically related to the criterion.

Figure 1 Lens model for study of interpersonal conflict and interpersonal learning (adapted from Hammond [1965] and Hammond et al., [1966b]).

Analysis of cognitive conflict involves comparing the cognitive systems of the conflicting parties i.e., the right side of the lens model shown in Figure 1. In situations where there is no outcome criterion analysis would be restricted to the right side. There could of course be more than two parties in which case the model would include N-systems on the right side (Reference CookseyCooksey, 1996), and a party could also refer to a dyad or group of individuals (Reference Rohrbaugh, Brehmer and JoyceRohrbaugh, 1988).

The lens model equation shown below (Reference TuckerTucker, 1964; see also Reference CookseyCooksey, 1996) details how a comparison of two cognitive systems can be formally done:

(1)

This equation points out that agreement between parties, rA, is a function of two components, namely GR 1R 2, which is the linearly predictable component (when using multiple linear regression analysis) of each party’s judgments contributing to overall agreement, and , which is the unmodeled component of each party’s judgments contributing to overall agreement. Equation 1 can be, and often is, reduced to the first component if one assumes that the unmodeled component of agreement is zero. Policy similarity is measured by G, while R 1 and R 2 are measures of each party’s cognitive control over their judgment policies. The interpretation of C is more difficult as it could refer to several things such as the extent to which both party’s policies are similar but unmodeled, the extent to which both party’s policies are different and unmodeled, or a lack of unmodeled response variance in one or both parties.

Conflict may be due to systematic and non-systematic cognitive differences in the way parties solve the problem (Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1976). Systematic differences refer to stable or predictable features of policies such as differences in relative cue weights, form of function relating cue values to judgments, organizing principles (i.e., how cues are combined), and policy consistency/cognitive control. Here, the lack of policy similarity means that parties may disagree both in principle and practice. Non-systematic differences introduce randomness or unreliability into the application of policies. Here, the lack of cognitive control means that parties may disagree in practice even though they agree in principle (false disagreement) or they may agree in practice even though they disagree in principle (false agreement; Reference Hammond, Grassia and OskampHammond & Grassia, 1985).Footnote 2

The nature and extent of the conflict may change as parties interact with each other and the task, thus highlighting the importance of studying interpersonal learning and task characteristics when understanding cognitive conflict. Indeed, an individual’s ability to learn about another person’s behavior is central to conflict resolution (Reference Hammond, Wilkins and ToddHammond et al., 1966b), as is his/her ability to learn about the characteristics of the task.Footnote 3

According to Hammond (Reference Hammond, Todd, Wilkins and Mitchell1965, and later Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1976), in cognitive conflict research, the researcher’s task is to measure the nature and extent of conflict between parties; document their efforts to agree; measure the nature and extent of compromise/resolution; measure the nature and extent of changes in the cognitive systems of conflicting parties; and document the effect of task- and person-related factors on conflict, compromise, and change. Such analyses are not only of theoretical import, but can also contribute to strategies for dispute resolution.

2.2 Methodology for cognitive conflict research in the lens model framework

From the perspective of IPC theory, the method used to study cognitive conflict involves experimentation (Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1976; Reference CookseyCooksey, 1996; Reference HammondHammond, 1965; Reference Hammond, Brehmer, Rappoport and Summers1973; see also Reference Rohrbaugh, Brehmer and JoyceRohrbaugh, 1988, for group-based research methods). The standard experiment is divided into a training stage where parties are trained to think differently about a judgment task (i.e., develop a different set of cue-dependencies), and a conflict stage where the parties are brought together to attempt to arrive at a mutually agreeable solution to the problem.Footnote 4 More specifically, after each party has learned to solve the task alone they are brought together, unaware that they have different policies. The parties are then asked to co-operate on solving another set of problems which are actually different from the ones they each learned.Footnote 5 On every trial or judgment problem, they study the available information and make judgments of the criterion variable alone and then communicate these to one another (overt individual judgment). If they disagree, they must discuss the problem until they reach an acceptable joint response (joint judgment). They are then asked to reconsider their original decisions, and these revisions remain private (covert individual judgment). Finally, if there is an environmental criterion, they are presented with the correct solution. So, the parties must adapt to one another as well as adapt to the task in order to agree and achieve.

The researcher can precisely define and manipulate the quantity and quality of cognitive differences, and objectively measure cognitive conflict, compromise, and change. Furthermore, the researcher can add complexity to the experiment by, for example (as Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1976, noted), introducing payoffs, manipulating feedback, and involving groups. Thus, although this paradigm may not fully represent all relevant features of what are typically complex problems, it can provide a reasonable analysis of some definable aspects. As such, Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) and Reference HammondHammond (1965) both claimed IPC theory may be used to guide research into real world conflicts.

The basic data collected from a typical experiment includes the joint judgment, and the overt and covert individual judgments made by the parties before and after this (Reference HammondHammond, 1965). As Reference HammondHammond (1965) noted, these measures can be used to study the extent and nature of cognitive conflict, compromise, and change with respect to the task, and with respect to the other party. (There are overt and covert measures of compromise, conflict, and change). For instance, conflict can be measured by comparison of each party’s overt individual judgments. A comparison of each party’s overt and covert individual judgments and the joint judgments provides a measure of compromise at the overt and covert levels, respectively. Furthermore, a comparison of each party’s (overt and covert) individual judgments and the criterion (where available) and the other party’s judgments provides a measure of cognitive change with respect to the task and other party, respectively. Reference HammondHammond (1965) also pointed out that the measures could be derived on both an inter- and intra-trial basis (i.e., comparison of each party’s responses averaged across trials or comparison of each party’s response on each trial, respectively), and that analyses could examine both external and internal dynamics such as the effect of interpersonal learning (Reference Hammond, Wilkins and ToddHammond et al., 1966b) and feedback (Todd, Hammond, & Wilkins, 1966). Indeed, the early research conducted by Hammond and colleagues focused on such topics (Reference Hammond, Todd, Wilkins and MitchellHammond et al., 1966a).

2.3 Early findings of cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition

After Hammond’s initial research on cognitive conflict, Brehmer and colleagues conducted an intensive series of studies. In 1976 Brehmer reviewed the research that had been conducted on cognitive conflict using IPC theory. By then, research had examined issues concerning: (a) the structure of cognitive conflicts; (b) the relative importance of the task and the other party in affecting policy change and conflict resolution; (c) the effect of task characteristics on cognitive conflict; and (d) the effect of person characteristics on cognitive conflict. Research had also begun to study (e) how cognitive conflict could be resolved via supports/aids. We describe the main findings below.

First, conflict may persist due to non-systematic cognitive differences even when parties are motivated to agree, and actually do agree in principle. Indeed, while parties reduce the systematic differences in their policies (i.e., there is policy similarity), over time the inconsistency of their policies increases thus leading to little reduction in the amount of conflict although the structure of the conflict has altered (e.g., Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1969). This is because parties tend to decrease their dependency on their old policies at a faster rate than they increase their application of a new policy that is compatible with each others’ (e.g., Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1972).

Second, policy change itself does not signify willingness to compromise but rather a desire to achieve, although compromise is sought when accuracy is not clearly observable/obtainable. When one party is initially trained in the optimal policy and the other is not, the latter will learn from the former if the task is highly predictable (e.g., Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1973a). However, if task predictability is low, the parties start off by decreasing dependency on their initial policies. Here, based on feedback, the party with the optimal policy soon appropriately switches back to his/her original policy, and the other party also learns from feedback (e.g., Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1974). When there is no feedback, parties may compromise: this reduces conflict without leading to observable inaccuracy (e.g., Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1971).

Third, formal (surface and system) task characteristics can influence each party’s policy development and the ease with which they can achieve, and such characteristics alone can explain cognitive conflict. Reference Hammond, Brehmer, Rappoport and SummersHammond and Brehmer (1973) did not find much evidence for substantive or content task characteristics influencing cognitive conflict. Surface characteristics refer to the number of cues, the metric level of cues, and the inter-cue correlations, while system characteristics refer to the distribution of cue validities, forms of functions relating cues to the criterion, organizing principles, and task predictability. For example, there is greater agreement despite less reduction of policy differences when the cues are inter-correlated than when they are orthogonal (Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1975). This may be because cue inter-correlations enable the parties to achieve with little change of their original policies (Mumpower & Hammond, 1974). In addition, there is less agreement between parties when task predictability is low because each party’s policies are less consistent rather because of any systematic differences in their policies (e.g., Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1975). Similar findings have been observed for tasks that require policies with nonlinear function forms which tend to be more difficult to develop (e.g., Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1973b).

Fourth, traditional individual difference variables such as gender do not affect measures of cognitive conflict (Hammond & Brehmer, 1973).

Finally, cognitive aids may be useful for reducing conflict. Reference Hammond, Brehmer, Rappoport and SummersHammond and Brehmer (1973) applied the technique of cognitive feedback (Todd & Reference HammondHammond, 1965) and developed a cognitive aid to conflict resolution called POLICY.Footnote 6 This interactive computer program enables parties to express their policies, compare them, change them, and discover the effects of such changes on conflict (see Reference Rohrbaugh, Brehmer and JoyceRohrbaugh, 1988, for group decision support systems). Cognitive feedback involves providing information about the task (i.e., ecological validities, intercue correlations, predictability, and cue-criterion function forms), the party’s judgment policy (i.e., utilization validities, cognitive control/consistency, and cue-judgment function forms), and the match between them (i.e., achievement, and its linear and nonlinear components) (Balzer, Doherty, & O’Connor, 1989; Doherty & Balzer, 1988). Such feedback can help to speed conflict reduction (Balke, Hammond, & Meyer, 1973).

In 1969, Leon Rappoport warned that “if the cognitive conflict model is to serve as anything more than a laboratory analogue, it must be determined whether socially-induced (i.e., “natural”) cognitive differences generate the same conflict phenomena as laboratory induced (i.e., “artificial”) cognitive differences” (p. 143). In fact, as Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) noted, many of the findings that were observed in the laboratory on simulated tasks were also obtained in naturalistic environments or real tasks, particularly for use in policy development (e.g., Adelman, Stewart, & Reference Hammond, Stewart, Brehmer, Steinmann, Kaplan and SchwartzHammond, 1975; Reference Balke, Hammond and MeyerBalke et al., 1973; Brown & Hammond, 1968; Steinmann, Smith, Jurdem, & Reference Hammond, Stewart, Brehmer, Steinmann, Kaplan and SchwartzHammond, 1975). Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) concluded his review with avenues for future research including examining the antecedents and consequences of policy inconsistency, and further analysis of real world conflicts.

3 Interpersonal conflict theory and cognitive conflict research post 1976

Here, we trace the evolution of IPC theory after 1976 to the present day to determine what further contributions cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition has made since Brehmer’s 1976 review. For example, have researchers followed up on the suggestions initially made by Reference HammondHammond (1965) that IPC theory can tell us something about real world political conflicts? Have researchers conducted research on the antecedents and consequences of policy inconsistency as suggested by Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976)? Are there other ideas beyond those of the lens model that are guiding cognitive conflict research today? To answer these questions, we used a combination of bibliographic and content reviews of publications since 1976 that cite the central articles by Reference HammondHammond (1965) and Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976). Thus, we focus on cognitive conflict research as it directly emerged from the IPC paradigm and related lens model framework. While the content review can shed light on the theoretical, empirical, and methodological contributions made since 1976, the bibliographic review indicates the “influence” or “importance” of the contributions. The bibliographic review also helps us to identify new research fronts in cognitive conflict research emerging from the work of Reference HammondHammond (1965) and Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976). The main limitation of this approach, however, is that it can exclude relevant publications by virtue of them not citing the central articles of interest. Later, we discuss how this limitation excluded potentially relevant work on negotiation.

We conducted a “cited reference” search on the ISI Web of Knowledge, Web of Sciences Databases (Science Citation Index Expanded, Social Sciences Citation Index, and Arts & Humanities Citation Index) to identify relevant journal publications in the period after 1976, to 2007 that cited Reference HammondHammond (1965) or Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976).Footnote 7 Publications before 1976 were also added in order to provide a full picture of the evolution of cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition. Overall, our searches resulted in 192 hits, with 141 publications dating after 1976. After 1976, 39 publications cited Reference HammondHammond (1965), 102 cited Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976), and 15 cited both authors (i.e., were repeats). Thus, excluding the repeats there were a total of 177 publications (192 minus the 15 repeats) with 126 publications dated after 1976. (A list of the 177 publications is available from the second author.)

First, we conducted a bibliographic review of the 177 publications using CiteSpace II (Reference ChenChen, 2004, 2006) which is a bibliometric tool that visualizes trends and turning points in scientific literatures based on citations. The input was bibliographic records from the publications and the outputs include illustrations of co-citation networks either in a cluster view or in a time zone view.Footnote 8 In CiteSpace II, the entire time interval is sliced into equal length segments in which citations and co-citations are calculated. In our analysis we used two year segments. In each time slice the co-citation network is determined by three thresholds, citation (c), co-citation (cc) and co-citation coefficient thresholds (ccc; this threshold determines the cosine coefficients in the normalization of the co-citation counts). The thresholds can be set for three points in time with linear interpolation between them. The resulting networks in each time slice can then be pruned by using the Pathfinder algorithm or the minimum spanning tree algorithm. The networks in each time slice are then merged into a synthesized network. As our main objective was to illustrate the network of the most central publications, we present figures with pruned (using the Pathfinder algorithm) co-citation networks based on high thresholds. That is, the resulting merged network shows only the most important publications in terms of citations and co-citations during the time period. In the merged network, individual publications are represented as tree rings where the thickness of a ring is proportional to the number of citations in a given time slice. The size of the outermost ring and the size of the font of the publication label are proportional to the betweenness centrality of the publication. The betweenness centrality measure is a graph theoretical property that specifies the importance of a node’s position in a network (Reference ChenChen, 2006). The color of the connecting lines between the citation trees represents the year of the first co-citation of the publication.

Second, we conducted a content review of those publications since 1976 on cognitive conflict in the lens model tradition, and which had cited Reference HammondHammond (1965) or Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976). After examining the 126 publications, only 17 were deemed relevant to this review. As described below, the remainder (109) were either publications on cognitive conflict but not in the lens model tradition or on topics related to (but not directly on) cognitive conflict such as interpersonal learning, group decision making, and decision aids. Our content review summarizes the methods and main findings of the 17 relevant publications.

3.1 Bibliographic review

The main results of the bibliometric analysis are presented in Figure 2, which shows the cluster view of a co-citation analysis from 1965 to 2007. Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) and Reference HammondHammond (1965) are the two most central articles as they were the basis of the selection procedure. The publications on IPC theory or cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition can be found on the left. These are mostly from the 1960s and 70s. Indeed, there appears in recent years to be a decline in cognitive conflict research using the lens model tradition, and few central articles were published in the years after Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976). Although 126 publications have cited Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) or Reference HammondHammond (1965) over the past 30 years, few of these actually examine cognitive conflict in the lens model tradition. Of the 17 publications that we classified as relevant to the content review we report below, there were two highs of 3 publications in 1977 and 1979 and then a sharp decline to one or zero each year following that.

Figure 2 Co-citation network of publications 1965–2007 (2 years slice, parameters c, cc, ccv: 3, 2, 25; 3, 3, 25; 4, 4, 25)

The upper left and the upper right of Figure 2 shows publications largely concerned with JDM, only some of which are related to cognitive conflict (but not directly on the topic itself). Here, for instance, researchers have examined how cognitive conflict may affect a third person’s judgments. For example, Reference CosierCosier (1978) studied the effect of different ways in which expert advice could conflict and the effect of their degree of accurate knowledge of the environment on subjects’ predictions of the criterion (see also Schwenk & Cosier, 1980). Cosier, Ruble, and Aplin (1978, Study 1) examined perceived helpfulness of expert advice under high and low conflict. Researchers also investigated factors that may impact judgment policies which have implications for future research on cognitive conflict (Hagafors & Brehmer, 1983), and shown how judgment analysis can be used to study expert judgment (Adelman & Mumpower, 1979). However, most of the publications on the upper left and right of Figure 2 are unrelated to cognitive conflict. For instance, Reference Dinkage and ZillerDinkage and Ziller (1989) explored US and German children’s conceptualizations of war and peace via photographs.

Most interestingly, the bottom right of Figure 2 shows that a new research front on group conflict appears to have emerged which also apparently examines cognitive conflict. It is in the mid-1990s, after the publication of Jehn’s (1995) article on the benefits and detriments of intragroup conflict, and the earlier book by Reference McGrathMcGrath (1984) on the interaction and performance of groups, that we can observe this new research front. These new central articles and their offshoots are at the bottom right of Figure 2. As we will discuss later, this new research front is not grounded in the lens model tradition and, although they still occasionally cite Brehmer (1976; and, rarely, Reference HammondHammond, 1965), these researchers use different theoretical frameworks and research tools than those used by researchers studying cognitive conflict in the lens model tradition. In fact, these researchers are not as interested in cognitive conflict as defined in the lens model tradition.

3.2 Content review

As mentioned above, we also conducted a content review of the 17 (out of 126) publications classified as being on cognitive conflict in the lens model tradition, which cited Reference HammondHammond (1965) or Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976). The Appendix presents a summary of the main aims, methods, and findings of these studies. (The main publications before 1976 were reviewed in Section 1).

All 17 publications reported studies that appeared to have moved beyond the theoretical issues reviewed by Brehmer in 1976 to investigate a new set of problems (except perhaps Reference Rose, Menasco and CurryRose et al., 1982). First, nine studies examined the effect of some form of intervention on cognitive conflict or judgment performance. Reference Cosier and RoseCosier and Rose (1977) examined the effect of cognitive conflict and goal conflict on judgment performance, and found less prediction error under high (than low) cognitive conflict in earlier trials, and under no-goal conflict. Holzworth’s (1983) study measured the impact of task predictability and mediation on conflict reduction, and reported that, while there was no significant effect of mediation, agreement was greater under more (than less) predictable tasks. Reference AlexanderAlexander (1979) measured the effect of communication technique on conflict reduction, and found that dyads trained in the “region of validity” technique showed greater conflict reduction than those not trained as such. Reference HarmonHarmon (1998) studied the effect of decision making method and communication medium on group satisfaction and agreement, and found that audio-communication (as opposed to face-to-face communication) increased satisfaction while policy modeling decision methods improved agreement over conventional decision making methods. Reference Harmon and RohrbaughHarmon and Rohrbaugh (1990) and Reference Sengupta and Te’eniSengupta and Te’eni (1993) studied the effect of cognitive feedback on group JDM. Whereas cognitive feedback increased group cognitive control, it did not increase agreement, and shared feedback did not improve group judgment accuracy over individual feedback/no feedback, but it did increase agreement. Reference Reagan-CirincioneReagan-Cirincione (1994) and Reference Bose and ParadiceBose and Paradice (1999) measured the effectiveness of group decision aids or support systems on group performance, which revealed that such aids were effective. Reference Andersson and BrehmerAndersson and Brehmer (1979) compared the effect of individual and interpersonal learning on policy change, and reported no significant differential effects of these types of learning.

Second, five studies investigated group conflict (Bose & Paradice, 1999; Reference Reagan-CirincioneReagan-Cirincione, 1994; Reference HarmonHarmon, 1998; Harmon & Rohrbaugh, 1990; Sengupta & Te’eni, 1993). These reported on the effectiveness of cognitive feedback (i.e., availability of feedback and whether it is shared), and group decision aids (where group discussion was aided by a facilitator and computer analyst) or support systems (where there is computerized collection and communication of individual judgments, amongst other things), as well as decision making method (i.e., structured policy modeling or not) and communication medium (i.e., audio or face-to-face).

Third, two studies examined potential perceptual influences on cognitive conflict. Reference Dhir and MarkmanDhir and Markman (1984) studied marital conflict in task definition rather than judgment performance. They found that feedback of their spouses’ perception of the task had a differential impact on husbands’ and wives’ ability to correctly predict their spouses’ judgment policies. Reference Qualls and JaffeQualls and Jaffe (1992) examined how husbands’ and wives’ pre-existing perceptions influenced conflict in joint purchase decisions. Here, similar perceptions led to less conflict and these couples resolved conflict differently than couples with dissimilar perceptions.

Finally, some studies also included measures of interpersonal learning as well as interpersonal conflict (Reference AlexanderAlexander, 1979; Reference GillisGillis, 1979b; Gillis & Moss, 1978; Reference McCarthyMcCarthy, 1977).

Methodologically, most researchers diverged from the experimental method proposed by Reference HammondHammond (1965) in several ways. First, in seven studies there was no training stage where participants learned to perform the judgment task (Dhir & Markman, 1984; Reference HarmonHarmon, 1998; Harmon & Rohrbaugh, 1990; Reference McCarthyMcCarthy, 1977; Reference Reagan-CirincioneReagan-Cirincione, 1994; Reference Summers, Ashworth and Feldman-SummersSummers et al., 1977; Qualls & Jaffe, 1992). Second, and relatedly, in over half of the studies parties were not trained to hold different judgment policies. Rather, in some studies parties were brought together based on their existing policy differences (Bose & Paradice, 1999; Reference HarmonHarmon, 1998; Harmon & Rohrbaugh, 1990; Reference McCarthyMcCarthy, 1977; Reference Reagan-CirincioneReagan-Cirincione, 1994). Gillis and colleagues paired participants according to the medication they were prescribed (Reference GillisGillis, 1979a; 1979b; Gillis & Moss, 1978). Reference Dhir and MarkmanDhir and Markman (1984) and Reference Qualls and JaffeQualls and Jaffe (1992) studied married couples. These methodological departures represent more than superficial deviations. Rather, they can reduce the researcher’s control over the study of cognitive conflict by, for instance, introducing unwanted (and potentially unknown) variability in how different parties perform the task and in the degree of existing conflict between parties.

Finally, in seven studies parties did not interact at the conflict stage. Rather, participants were either given a simulated person’s judgments in conflict to their own (Cosier & Rose, 1977; Reference Rose, Menasco and CurryRose et al., 1982) or participants’ responses were paired (Reference Summers, Ashworth and Feldman-SummersSummers et al., 1977). In McCarthy’s (1977) study, joint judgments were optional, and Reference Andersson and BrehmerAndersson and Brehmer (1979) examined how individual learning compared to interpersonal learning. Reference Dhir and MarkmanDhir and Markman (1984) and Reference Qualls and JaffeQualls and Jaffe (1992) simply paired individuals’ judgments. This elimination of the interpersonal communication between conflicting parties means that relevant issues such as interpersonal learning cannot be addressed in the study of cognitive conflict, compromise, and change.

Before summarizing the findings of this content review, it is worth pointing out that, since the bibliographic review technique used for initial selection of publications was limited to those that cited the articles by Reference HammondHammond (1965) and Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976), some potentially relevant work on negotiation was excluded (Darling, Mumpower, Rohrbaugh, & Vari, 1999; Milter, Darling, & Reference Mumpower and RohrbaughMumpower, 1996; Mumpower & Rohrbaugh, 1996). This work reiterates the importance of the task environment when understanding negotiation or conflict behavior (Reference Mumpower, Brehmer and JoyceMumpower, 1988; 1991). Negotiation tasks do not always have an outcome criterion, or it may be irrelevant. Characteristics of negotiation tasks are often subjectively interpreted by the conflicting parties, and these characteristics (interpretations) may change as the parties interact. The task structure in turn affects the most appropriate negotiation strategy. Thus, in negotiation tasks parties must agree on what the task is and how to solve it. This work expands or redefines the terminology for discussing conflict resolution: for example, settlements may be efficient, have joint utility or equality, and strategies may involve compromise or logrolling/horsetrading (where parties make trade-offs so they each obtain a desirable outcome). Controlling for formal task characteristics, substantive task characteristics (i.e., cover story) can affect negotiators’ ability to reach efficient settlements (Reference Milter, Darling and MumpowerMilter et al., 1996). This work has also described procedures to support conflict resolution in multi-party negotiations in real-world public policy settings (Reference Darling, Mumpower, Rohrbaugh and VariDarling et al., 1999).

In sum, although our bibliographic review indicates that after 1976 relatively few studies were published on cognitive conflict in the lens model tradition our content review suggests that several new contributions were made by this small body of literature. In fact, the literature went beyond the issues studied in the earlier work reviewed by Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) in several interesting ways. However, for unknown reasons, no-one followed up on the suggestions initially made by Reference HammondHammond (1965) that IPC theory can tell us something about political conflicts, which nowadays may focus on identifying and managing threats to national and global security, although the work on group conflict and negotiation sometimes deals with public policy issues (e.g., Reference Darling, Mumpower, Rohrbaugh and VariDarling et al., 1999; see also Reference Hammond, Grassia and OskampHammond and Grassia, 1985 for public policy examples). Similarly, few researchers directly examined the antecedents and consequences of policy inconsistency as suggested by Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976). Reference Karelaia and HogarthKarelaia and Hogarth (2008) recently examined the impact of several factors such as outcome feedback and cue redundancy on policy inconsistency, which may be worth exploring in the context of cognitive conflict research. Researchers also often departed from the experimental method described by Reference HammondHammond (1965). Rather than representing useful innovations these departures appear to dilute the control that the researcher has over the experimental situation in, for instance, knowing the precise sources of conflict, and limit the study of important issues in cognitive conflict such as interpersonal learning.

3.3 Research on group conflict: A paradigm shift in cognitive conflict research

Beyond the small body of published literature on cognitive conflict in the lens model tradition conducted after 1976, the bottom right of Figure 2 revealed that in the mid-1990s there was an emerging research front on group conflict that apparently examines cognitive conflict. The central publications were by Reference McGrathMcGrath (1984) and Reference JehnJehn (1995). However, this new research front is not grounded in the lens model tradition and, although they still occasionally cite Brehmer (1976; and rarely Reference HammondHammond, 1965), these researchers use different theoretical frameworks and research tools than those used by researchers studying cognitive conflict in the lens model tradition. It is worth briefly reviewing the new central publications in order to assess the degree to which this research front, which has attracted more researchers than the lens model tradition, marks a theoretical and methodological advance in cognitive conflict research.

McGrath’s (1984) book reviews the theoretically grounded empirical literature on small groups, and summarizes the methods used to study small groups. He notes that when a group’s task is to resolve conflicts, as is often the case, IPC theory (which he confusingly refers to as “social judgment theory” throughout) is relevant to understanding the negotiation process. IPC theory is thus reviewed in a chapter entitled “Cognitive conflict tasks: Resolving conflicts of viewpoint within the group.” Here, a passing reference is made to Brunswik’s (1955; whose name is misspelled throughout) lens model, and articles by Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) and Hammond et al. (1966a, 1975) are summarized. The experimental method associated with IPC theory is also summarized. In addition, with reference to a study by Reference RohrbaughRohrbaugh (1979), Reference McGrathMcGrath (1984) concludes that the cognitive feedback approach used by IPC theorists to improve group judgment is not very effective. Overall, McGrath (1984, p. 66, p. 89, p. 93) calls the work on IPC “limited,” noting that much of the research has been conducted only on “two-person groups,” and he calls the method used “very elaborate.”

Thus, McGrath’s (1984) book introduced IPC theory and its associated method to researchers interested in studying group JDM. However, this was just one of several approaches reviewed by McGrath, and he was somewhat critical of it. It is no surprise therefore, that few researchers interested in group JDM have studied conflict in the lens model tradition. In fact, later, Jehn’s (1995) reference to Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) is merely to point out that he (and others) suggest that the relationship between conflict and performance is influenced by the type of task a group performs. Similarly, others refer to Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) simply as a means of suggesting that cognitive conflict may result in affective conflict (Reference AmasonAmason, 1996). Generally, the research questions, theoretical insights, and experimental method of IPC theory were overlooked in the central articles by Reference McGrathMcGrath (1984) and Reference JehnJehn (1995), and with the exception of work by Rohrbaugh and colleagues (Reference Rohrbaugh, Brehmer and JoyceRohrbaugh, 1988, and Harmon & Rohrbaugh, 1990), abandoned in recent research on group conflict (e.g., Pelled, Eisenhardt, & Xin, 1999).

As the central article by Reference JehnJehn (1995) demonstrates, cognitive conflict is often defined in terms of “task conflict.” According to Jehn (1995, p. 258)

“Task conflict exists when there are disagreements among group members about the content of the tasks being performed, including differences in viewpoints, ideas, and opinions.”

This concept was measured by Jehn (1995, p. 268) using a short scale that includes items such as “How often do people in your work unit disagree about opinions regarding the work being done?” “How frequently are there conflicts about ideas in your work unit?” And, “How much conflict about the work you do is there in your work unit?” Responses are provided on 5-point scales anchored by 1 = “none” and 5 = “a lot.” Others have used similar measures (e.g., Reference Pelled, Eisenhardt and XinPelled et al., 1999). Therefore, the new concept of task conflict is somewhat vague and ill-defined, and its measurement is not very precise. For instance, it is unclear how participants interpret concepts such as “conflict,” “opinions,” and “ideas,” and there is no clear differentiation of different aspects of the phenomenon of task conflict. Finally, its measurement is on a short, subjectively interpreted scale. This approach clearly departs from the precise definition of cognitive conflict provided by IPC theory as the relation between cognitive systems which is measured quantitatively in terms of agreement (rA), policy similarity (G), and cognitive control (R 1 and R 2), and which clearly differentiates between different aspects of the phenomenon (e.g., policy similarity versus cognitive control).

Furthermore, this group of researchers are largely interested in questions pertaining to the impact of task (cognitive) conflict on outcomes such as work satisfaction, liking of other group members, intentions to remain in the group, performance (Reference JehnJehn, 1995), and emotional conflict (Reference Pelled, Eisenhardt and XinPelled et al., 1999). They have typically used quantitative questionnaire and qualitative observational and interview methods as well as archival analysis to examine the nature and effects of existing conflict within work groups. For instance, Reference JehnJehn (1995) measured individuals’ performance via appraisal ratings, departmental records, and supervisors’ ratings. The experimental method is rarely employed. This makes it difficult to advance causal theories, and their findings remain limited to description and prediction. Indeed, these researchers typically use multiple regression and other correlational techniques for data analysis, which is why the statistical textbook by Cohen and Reference Cohen and CohenCohen (1983) also appears in the bottom right of Figure 2.Footnote 9

Therefore, while researchers working on group conflict have focused on conflict in real world settings such as organizations which lends external validity to their findings, the main focus in this new research front on group conflict is not necessarily cognitive conflict, but task conflict. It could be argued that the methods employed do not represent an advance, and that the theories, as they currently stand, are limited. As such we believe the research front on group conflict revealed at the bottom right of Figure 2 does not really represent a constructive paradigm shift in cognitive conflict research.

4 The future of interpersonal conflict theory and cognitive conflict research

In this final part, we offer possible reasons for the sharp decline of cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition, and we discuss the future of this research and IPC theory in the context of the growing field of JDM. Specifically, we consider some of the opportunities for theoretical advancement and methodological innovation in cognitive conflict research.

At a meeting in 2006 of the Brunswik Society, a small international group of scholars dedicated to Brunswikian psychology, researchers offered possible explanations for the historical decline of cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition and the neglect of IPC theory. In particular, researchers recalled that they felt most of the important and interesting questions concerning cognitive conflict had already been sufficiently addressed in Hammond’s and Brehmer’s early work, thus leaving little scope for new insights. Researchers also reminisced that at the time there were several other areas of Brunswikian-related research available for exploration which were more appealing such as the study of clinical judgment (Reference HammondHammond, 1955), multiple cue probability learning (Hammond & Summers, 1965), cognitive feedback (Todd & Reference HammondHammond, 1965), interpersonal learning (Reference Hammond, Royce and RozeboomHammond, 1972; Reference Hammond, Wilkins and ToddHammond et al., 1966b), and social judgment theory (Reference Hammond, Stewart, Brehmer, Steinmann, Kaplan and SchwartzHammond et al., 1975). From a more practical perspective, researchers noted that cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition was challenging. For instance, echoing McGrath’s (1984) criticism, researchers complained that the proposed experimental method was inefficient since it required many participants and much time. Furthermore, researchers noted that later generations of students were not always sufficiently trained to conduct the relatively complex statistical analysis required by the lens model equation.

It is however, premature to conclude that cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition conducted to date has provided a complete picture of cognitive conflict, compromise, and change. None of the questions addressed in the early work have been fully explored. For instance, the effects of task characteristics on conflict, compromise, and change need to be examined more comprehensively, as do the role of person characteristics. And, much more research is needed on real world conflicts. In addition, there are other empirical questions that have yet to be addressed, which may contribute to a theoretical understanding of how conflict, compromise, and change are influenced by parties’ subjective interpretations of events, and their opportunity to learn the task. For instance, what is the role of task definition in cognitive conflict? What are the antecedents and consequences of perceived rather than actual conflict? What is the effect of lack of feedback or delayed feedback on compromise and change? Is the nature of compromise and change different when parties conflict in a problem where there is no outcome criterion? Beyond this, conducting cognitive conflict research in the context of the fast expanding field of JDM lends several opportunities. By taking into account recent theoretical and empirical developments researchers can make theoretical advancements to IPC theory and integrate it with other approaches, as well as study emergent research questions. Moreover, by adopting new methodological innovations researchers can overcome some of the practical challenges to conducting cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition.

4.1 Potential for theoretical advancement

Several developments in the field of JDM may be used to advance IPC theory. Two such inter-related developments, which were partly inspired by Brunswikian psychology, concern the nature of the cognitive models that are constructed when investigating JDM. Typically, researchers working in the lens model tradition have developed models of cognition using statistical regression techniques (Reference CookseyCooksey, 1996). These are static, structural models that describe how people weight and combine information but not how they search for it, and they suggest people use the same cues in the same way for deciding on different judgment problems in a task. Using these models, researchers have portrayed the judgment process as a linear, compensatory integration of multiple cues (Brehmer & Reference Brehmer, Brehmer, Brehmer and JoyceBrehmer, 1988).

However, recently, it has been argued that regression models do not provide a psychologically plausible or flexible and adaptive description of human JDM (Dhami & Harries, 2001; Gigerenzer, Todd, & the ABC Research Group, 1999), even though they can capture simple processes (Hogarth & Karelaia, 2007). In fact, it has been demonstrated that people frequently use simple heuristics (e.g., Reference DhamiDhami, 2003; Dhami & Ayton, 2001), especially under certain circumstances (e.g., Garcia-Retamero & Dhami, in press; Rieskamp & Hoffrage, 1999). These are dynamic, process models that describe information search, stop, and decision making. They suggest that people may use different cues in different ways for deciding on different judgment problems in a task. Often, these “fast and frugal” heuristics, as they are called, portray the judgment process as non-compensatory, such that people base decisions on one cue alone.

Brunswik (1955, 1956) did not rule out the use of other models, neither did Hammond (1955; 1996b), and Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1979) recognized this. Thus, alternative visions of the lens model have recently been proposed (Dhami & Harries, 2001; Gigerenzer & Kurz, 2001), and simple heuristics have been adopted by some researchers in other areas of Brunswikian-related research such as social judgment theory research (e.g., Dhami & Ayton, 2001; Dhami & Harries, 2001; Reference Kee, Jenkins, McIlwaine, Patterson, Harper and ShieldsKee et al., 2003). Similarly, it may be worth employing a simple heuristics approach to IPC theory, and examining how simple cognitive strategies fare in conflict situations. Researchers can examine the extent to which the findings of past cognitive conflict research generalize to situations where (one or both) conflicting parties use non-compensatory strategies. Indeed, advocates of the simple heuristics approach have argued for the superiority of these simple cognitive strategies relative to regression models in terms of, for example, achievement/accuracy (e.g., Czerlinski, Gigerenzer, & Goldstein, 1999; Gigerenzer & Goldstein, 1996), and so a simple heuristics approach to IPC theory and cognitive conflict research can indicate whether simple heuristics are also valuable in social (conflict) situations. Researchers can also investigate the pattern of information search and stop during conflict, compromise, and change. The fact that simple heuristics do not require the type of statistical analysis necessary for regression analysis also means that less numerically minded students may feel competent to study cognitive conflict.

Another development in the field of JDM that can be used to advance IPC theory concerns the nature of the factors that are used to explain cognitive conflict, compromise, and change. While working during the “cognitive revolution” in psychology, Reference HammondHammond (1965) strived to show how cognition alone was relevant to conflict. In his vision of the future he saw that “conflict between men will be derived from their cognitive differences” (Reference HammondHammond, 1965, p. 65). Similarly, Brehmer (1979, p. 1000) concluded that “cognitive factors may produce conflict and that cognitive factors alone may cause prolonged disagreement, even in the absence of differences in interest or emotional factors.”

However, there has been a growing recognition in the field of JDM of the importance of non-cognitive factors such as emotions (Loewenstein & Lerner, 2003). Emotions experienced at the point of JDM as well as emotions that an individual expects to experience from the outcome of his/her decision may impact the cognitive process and judgment behavior. For instance, in the context of risk, Loewenstein, Hsee, Weber, and Welch (2001) claim that anticipatory emotions can have a direct impact on judgment behavior. Furthermore, they state that both anticipated and anticipatory emotions can have an indirect impact on judgment behavior via influencing the cognitive process. Research using both a valence-based and an emotion-specific approach tends to support these claims (e.g., Reference Clore, Martin and TesserClore, 1992; Johnson & Tversky, 1983; Lerner & Keltner, 2000; Reference ZajoncZajonc, 1980). For example, experienced anger may lead people to be risk-seeking, while expected regret may lead them to be risk-averse. There is also evidence to suggest that emotions can have both benefits and drawbacks for JDM (e.g., Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1997; Frijda; 1986; Reference LoewensteinLoewenstein, 1996; Reference SlovicSlovic, 2001). For example, expected emotions can help the individual make decisions that take consequences into account, but forecasts need to be relevant and accurate. Immediate emotions can help him/her focus on important events, provide useful information and motivation, but can drive the individual to act contrary to long-term goals and can influence forecasts.

It may be worth expanding IPC theory to include non-cognitive factors, and in particular, examining the role of emotions in conflict situations. Researchers could examine the direct and indirect impact that specific expected and experienced emotions (e.g., anger, regret, sadness, and happiness) have on the willingness to compromise and agree (or capitulate), ability to change, and on consistency. While negative emotions may lead conflict to continue and make agreement difficult to reach, positive emotions may facilitate compromise and change. In fact, emotions may also alter the conflict resolution strategies that parties use, and how they perceive and interact with one another (e.g., Reference ForgasForgas, 1998; van Kleef, de Dreu, & Manstead, 2006). Emotions may also impact the party’s perception of the outcome. Finally, inclusion of emotional factors in IPC theory and cognitive conflict research can indicate further potential sources of expected and experienced emotions (i.e., from aspects of the conflict situation), which may be useful to those specializing in emotion research.

The potential for IPC theory and cognitive conflict research to employ process models that describe conflicting parties as using simple heuristics and being influenced by emotions is compatible with Hammond’s (1996a, 2000) cognitive continuum theory (CCT). CCT is a recent Brunswikian-related development which highlights the interplay between characteristics of the task and modes of cognition. Cognition can be placed on a continuum from the intuitive to the analytic, although the most common type incorporates elements of both and is called quasirationality. Intuitive cognition is characterized by, for example, low cognitive control and awareness of cognitive activity but high speed of processing; whereas analytic cognition involves for example, high cognitive control and awareness of cognitive activity but slow speed of processing. Tasks can induce certain modes of cognition, and successful performance on a task inhibits movement along the continuum while failure may stimulate transition to other modes of cognition. Importantly, performance is contingent on the correspondence between the task properties and the individual’s cognitive mode. It has been suggested that certain task characteristics such as having more than five cues, inter-cue correlations, pictorial presentation of information, many decision alternatives, no outcome feedback, familiarity with the task, and time pressure all induce an intuitive mode of cognition. The reverse of these induces an analytic mode of cognition, whereas a combination of the two types of task characteristics will induce quasirationality. Simple heuristics and emotions imply an intuitive mode of cognition, and it may be worthwhile studying how this mode relative to others may impact conflict resolution.

4.2 Potential for methodological innovation

Technological advances mean that researchers in the field of JDM have a wider variety of methodological tools available to them to develop and test their theories. As Reference Dhami, Hertwig and HoffrageDhami et al. (2004) point out, although experimentation is important, it has been supplemented by other methods such as virtual environments and computer simulations. Computer generated “microworlds” can simulate the conditions of naturalistic environments that participants repeatedly interact with. As with research on naturalistic decision making (Zsambok & Klein, 1997), virtual environments can be characterized by time pressure, limited information, uncertainty, limited resources, imprecise goals, high stakes, and dynamic conditions. Expert or professional JDM (individual or group) such as fire commanders and military leaders can be examined, as well as that of novices. However, instead of relying on descriptive accounts via observations, interviews, case studies, and “cognitive task analysis” as is the case in the subfield of naturalistic decision making, researchers using microworlds can manipulate and control aspects of the task and conflict situation, as well as obtain repeated measurements. Microworlds have been used in JDM to, for example, study dynamic decision making and judgment biases (e.g., Reference BrehmerBrehmer, 1992; Brehmer & Dörner, 1993; Fiedler, Walther, Freytag, & Plessner, 2002). Researchers can use microworlds to test their understanding of cognitive conflict, compromise, and change in naturalistic settings.

Computer simulation has been shown to be a particularly useful method of theory development since it allows precise and transparent specification of a theory, and rigorous and efficient testing of its implications (Reference Mosler, Schwarz, Ammann and GutscherMosler et al., 2001). Simulation models can be validated by comparison with past research findings, and the models can be used to design further research studies. Simulations have practical advantages: For instance, they can model dynamic processes, involve repeated trials, allow within-subjects analysis, and allow examination of individual or group behavior. Computer simulations have been used in JDM to, for example, model the overconfidence effect and hindsight bias (e.g., Hertwig, Fanselow, & Hoffrage, 2003; Pohl, Einsenhauer, & Hardt, 2003), as well as the outcomes of using different negotiation strategies (e.g., Darling & Mumpower, 1992). Similarly, computer simulations can be employed by researchers to develop and test the above suggested advancements of IPC theory, namely by including simple heuristics and emotions.

5 Concluding remarks

As our review shows, Hammond’s (1965) IPC theory and his vision of cognitive conflict research have, since Brehmer’s (1976) review, been “lost in translation” and neglected altogether. This is regrettable since there is a need for conflict theories, including cognitive conflict, in the current socio-political era characterized by international terrorism where parties disagree about the level of threat, and how to manage and minimize it, and where there is a growing movement towards use of alternative dispute resolution techniques. Already equipped with a precise theory and rigorous method, researchers studying cognitive conflict in the lens model tradition can make the most of opportunities offered in the growing field of JDM to consider conflicts where parties may use more or less simple strategies and where they may be influenced by emotions in natural environments. We hope the present paper inspires future researchers of cognitive conflict.

Appendix

Content review of cognitive conflict research in the lens model tradition. (Only relevant changes to the standard method involving a training and conflict stage described in Section 1 of the paper are noted here.)

Footnotes

*

We would like to thank Robin Hogarth, Jeryl Mumpower, Jon Baron, and an anonymous reviewer for their helpful comments on an earlier draft of this paper.

1 Reference HammondHammond (1965) applied the earlier developed multiple cue probability learning paradigm based on the lens model framework (Hammond & Summers, 1965) and the technique of cognitive feedback (Todd & Reference HammondHammond, 1965) to judgment in social situations, namely conflict situations (see also Hammond, Wilkins, and Todd [1966b] for the related study of interpersonal learning). These historical antecedents of IPC theory differ from those noted by others. For instance, Reference BrehmerBrehmer (1976) claimed that IPC theory was guided by the conceptual framework of social judgment theory (Hammond, Stewart, Brehmer, & Reference Steinmann, Smith, Jurdem and HammondSteinmann, 1975), and Reference Mumpower and StewartMumpower and Stewart (1996) stated that cognitive conflict research was rooted in cognitive continuum theory (Reference HammondHammond, 1996a).

3 Reference Mumpower and StewartMumpower and Stewart (1996) point out that systematic differences in policies may be due to missing or poor feedback, missing or poor information, bias in subjective evaluations of one’s own policy, and redundancy of information. Non-systematic differences may increase when the task is unpredictable or requires use of a large amount of information, especially in a nonlinear way, and when the party is learning to solve a novel task or a familiar task in a novel way. (See also Hammond’s [1996a] cognitive continuum theory for how task characteristics may influence judgment).

2 Research on interpersonal learning is also conducted within the lens model framework shown in Figure 1 (Reference Earle, Rappoport and SummersEarle, 1973; Reference Hammond, Wilkins and ToddHammond et al., 1966b; Reference Hammond, Royce and RozeboomHammond, 1972). Here, instead of making a joint decision, as would be done in a study on cognitive conflict, participants are asked to predict the other person’s response. Comparison of the prediction with the other person’s actual response provides a measure of interpersonal knowledge. Research on multiple-cue probability learning demonstrates how people learn about the task (Hammond & Summers, 1965). Characteristics of the task that individuals must learn include the cues, cue values, cue distributions, cue inter-correlations, and ecological validities (Dhami, Hertwig, & Hoffrage, 2004).

4 Participants may also be selected because they have conflicting policies, and so the training stage is eliminated.

5 Alternatively, the parties may be presented with a set of problems that only one party has learned, thereby requiring the other party to capitulate.

6 This was originally called COGNOGRAPH. The emphasis is on teaching consistent new policies. However, the effectiveness of this aid has not been empirically tested (Brehmer & Reference Brehmer, Brehmer, Brehmer and JoyceBrehmer, 1988).

7 Searches were as of October 27, 2007.

8 Here, we provide only a brief overview of the steps involved in analyses using CiteSpace II, since only some of it’s basic features were required for present purposes. The reader is referred to Reference ChenChen (2006) for a detailed overview of CiteSpace II.

9 Although research in the lens model tradition has also traditionally employed correlational tools, these are used in conjunction with experimental techniques.

References

Adelman, L., & Mumpower, J. (1979). The analysis of expert judgment. Technological Forecasting and Social Change, 15, 191204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adelman, L., Stewart, T. R., & Hammond, K. R. (1975). A case history of the application of social judgment theory to policy formulation. Policy Science, 6, 137159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, E. R. (1979). The reduction of cognitive conflict: Effects of various types of communication. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 23, 120138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amason, A. C. (1996). Distinguishing the effects of functional and dysfunctional conflict on strategic decision making: Resolving a paradox for top management teams. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 123148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersson, H., & Brehmer, B. (1979). Note on the policies acquired in interpersonal learning. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 24, 195201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balke, W. M., Hammond, K. R., & Meyer, G. D. (1973). An alternative approach to labor-management relation. Administration Science Quarterly, 18, 311327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balzer, W. K., Doherty, M. E., & O’ConnorR., Jr. R., Jr. (1989). Effects of cognitive feedback on performance. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 410433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A. R. (1997). Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science, 275, 12931295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bose, U., & Paradice, D. B. (1999). The effects of integrating cognitive feedback and multi-attribute utility-based multicriteria decision-making methods in GDSS. Group Decision and Negotiation, 8, 157182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, A., & Brehmer, B. (1988). What have we learned about human judgment from thirty years of policy capturing. In Brehmer, B.& Joyce, C. R. B. (Eds.), Human judgment: The SJT view, pp. 75114. Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, B. (1969). The roles of policy differences and inconsistency in policy conflict. (Umeå Psychological Reports No.18). Umeå, Sweden: University of Umeå.Google Scholar
Brehmer, B. (1971). Effects of communication and feedback on cognitive conflict. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 12, 205216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, B. (1972). Policy conflict as a function of policy similarity and policy complexity. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 13, 208221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, B. (1973a). The effect of cue validity on interpersonal learning of inference tasks with linear and nonlinear cues. American Journal of Psychology, 86, 2948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, B. (1973b). Policy conflict and policy change as a function of task characteristics. II. The effect of task predictability. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 14, 220227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, B. (1974). The effects of cue intercorrelation on interpersonal learning of probabilistic inference tasks. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 12, 397412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, B. (1975). Policy conflict and policy change as a function of task characteristics. IV. The effect of cue intercorrelations. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 16, 8596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brehmer, B. (1976). Social judgment theory and the analysis of interpersonal conflict. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 9851003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, B. (1979). Preliminaries to a psychology of inference. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 20, 193210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehmer, B. (1992). Dynamic decision making: Human control of complex systems. Acta Psychologica, 81, 211241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brehmer, B., & Dörner, D. (1993). Experiments with computer-simulated microworlds: Escaping both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study. Computers in Human Behavior, 9, 171184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, L., & Hammond, K. R. (1968). A supra-linguistic method for reducing intragroup conflict. (Report No. 108). Boulder: University of Colorado, Institute of Behavioral Science, Program on Cognitive Processes.Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1952). The conceptual framework of psychology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1955). Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. Psychological Review, 62, 193217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1956). Perception and the representative design of psychological experiments. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, C. (2004). Searching for intellectual turning points: Progressive knowledge domain visualization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 53035310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, C. (2006). CiteSpace II: Detecting and visualizing emerging trends and transient patterns in scientific literature. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 57, 359377.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clore, G. L. (1992). Cognitive phenomenology: Feelings and the construction of judgment. In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), The construction of social judgments, pp. 133163. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cooksey, R. W. (1996). Judgment analysis: Theory, methods, and applications. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd Ed.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Cosier, R. A. (1978). The effects of three potential aids for making strategic decisions on prediction accuracy. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 22, 295306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosier, R. A, & Rose, G. L. (1977). Cognitive conflict and goal conflict effects on task performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 19, 378391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosier, R. A., Ruble, T. L., & Aplin, J. C. (1978). An evaluation of the effectiveness of dialectical inquiry systems. Management Science, 24, 14831490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Czerlinski, J., Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. G. (1999). How good are simple heuristics? In Gigerenzer, G. Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group, Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 97118). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Darling, T. A., & Mumpower, J. L. (1992). Simulating process and outcome for two-party contract negotiations. Control and Cybernetics, 21, 151184.Google Scholar
Darling, T. A., Mumpower, J. L., Rohrbaugh, J., & Vari, A. (1999). Negotiation support for multi-party resource allocation: Developing recommendations for decreasing transportation-related air pollution in Budapest. Group Decision and Negotiation, 8, 5175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhami, M. K. (2003). Psychological models of professional decision making. Psychological Science, 14, 175180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dhami, M. K., & Ayton, P. (2001). Bailing and jailing the fast and frugal way. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 14, 141168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhami, M. K., & Harries, C. (2001). Fast and frugal versus regression models of human judgment. Thinking and Reasoning, 7, 527.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhami, M. K., Hertwig, R., & Hoffrage, U. (2004). The role of representative design in an ecological approach to cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 959988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhir, K. S., & Markman, H. J. (1984). Application of social judgment theory to understanding and treating marital conflict. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 46, 597610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinkage, R. I., & Ziller, R. C. (1989). Explicating cognitive conflict through photo-communication: The meaning of war and peace in Germany and the United States. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 33, 309317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doherty, M. E., & Balzer, W. K. (1988). Cognitive feedback. In Brehmer, B. & Joyce, C. R. B. (Eds.), Human judgment: The SJT view, pp. 163197). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Earle, T. C. (1973). Interpersonal learning. In Rappoport, L. & Summers, D. A. (Eds.), Human judgment and social interaction (pp. 240266). New York: Holt Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (1998). On feeling good and getting your way: Mood effects on negotiator cognition and behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 565577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K., Walther, E., Freytag, P., & Plessner, H. (2002). Judgment biases in a simulated classroom — A cognitive-environmental approach. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 527561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Garcia-Retamero, R., & Dhami, M. K. (In press). Take-the-best in expert-novice decision strategies for residential burglary. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., & Goldstein, D. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: Models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103, 650669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gigerenzer, G., & Kurz, E. M. (2001). Vicarious functioning reconsidered: A fast and frugal lens model. In Hammond, K. R. & Stewart, T. R. (Eds.), The essential Brunswik: Beginnings, explications, applications, pp. 342348. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gillis, J. S. (1979a). Antipsychotic drugs and conflict resolution: A study of the comparative effects of Trifluoperazine, Haloperidol, and Thioridazine. Research Communications in Psychology, Psychiatry, and Behavior, 4, 320.Google Scholar
Gillis, J. S. (1979b). Effects of Trifluoperazine and Fluphenazine injection of conflict resolution and interpersonal learning with schizophrenics. Current Therapeutic Research — Clinical and Experimental, 26, 547556.Google Scholar
Gillis, J. S., & Moss, C. D. (1978). An Experimental study of effects of Amitriptyline-Perphenazine and Amitriptyline-Haloperidol combinations on interpersonal learning. Current Therapeutic Research — Clinical and Experimental, 23, 261270.Google Scholar
Goldstein, W. M., & Hogarth, R. M. (1997). Judgment and decision research: Some historical context. In Goldstein, W. M. & Hogarth, R. M. (Eds.), Research on judgment and decision making (pp. 365). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hagafors, R., & Brehmer, B. (1983). Does having to justify one’s judgments change the nature of the judgment process? Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 31, 223232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (1955). Probabilistic functioning and the clinical method. Psychological Review, 62, 255262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, K. R. (1965). New directions in research on conflict resolution. Journal of Social Issues, 21, 4466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (1972). Inductive knowing. In Royce, J. R. & Rozeboom, W. W. (Eds.), The psychology of knowing (pp. 285320). New York: Gordon and Breach.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (1973). The cognitive conflict paradigm. In Rappoport, L. & Summers, D. A. (Eds.), Human judgment and social interaction (pp. 188205). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (1996a). Human judgment and social policy: Irreducible uncertainty, inevitable error, unavoidable injustice. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (1996b). Upon reflection. Thinking and Reasoning, 2, 239248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (2000). Judgments under stress. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R., & Brehmer, B. (1973). Quasi-rationality and distrust: Implications for international conflict. In Rappoport, L. & Summers, D. (Eds.), Human judgment and decision processes, pp. 338391. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R., & Grassia, J. (1985). The cognitive side of conflict: From theory to resolution of policy disputes. In Oskamp, S. (Ed.), Applied Social Psychology Annual (Vol. 6) (pp. 233254). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hammond, K. R., Stewart, T. R., Brehmer, B., & Steinmann, D. O. (1975). Social judgment theory. In Kaplan, M. F. & Schwartz, S. (Eds.), Human judgment and decision processes (pp. 271317). New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, K. R., & Summers, D. A. (1965). Cognitive dependence on linear and nonlinear cues. Psychological Review, 72, 215224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammond, K. R., Todd, F. J., Wilkins, M., & Mitchell, T. O. (1966a). Cognitive conflict between persons: Application of the “lens model” paradigm. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2, 343360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hammond, K. R., Wilkins, M. M., & Todd, F. J. (1966b). A research paradigm for the study of interpersonal learning. Psychological Bulletin, 65, 221232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmon, J. (1998). Electronic meetings and intense group conflict: Effects of a policy-modeling performance support system and an audio communication support system on satisfaction and agreement. Group Decision and Negotiation, 7, 131155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmon, J., & Rohrbaugh, J. (1990). Social judgment analysis and small group decision making: Cognitive feedback effects on individual and collective performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 46, 3454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hertwig, R., Fanselow, C., & Hoffrage, U. (2003). Hindsight bias: How knowledge and heuristics affect our reconstruction of the past. Memory, 11, 357377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogarth, R. M., & Karelaia, N. (2007). Heuristic and linear models of judgment: Matching rules and environments. Psychological Review, 114, 733758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holzworth, J. (1983). Intervention in a cognitive conflict. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 32, 216231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jehn, K. A. (1995). A multimethod examination of the benefits and detriments of intragroup conflict. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 256282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. J., & Tversky, A. (1983). Affect, generalization, and the perception of risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 2031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karelaia, N., & Hogarth, R. M. (2008). Determinants of linear judgment: a meta-analysis of lens studies. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 404426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kee, F., Jenkins, J., McIlwaine, S., Patterson, C., Harper, S., & Shields, M. (2003). Fast and frugal models of clinical judgment in novice and expert physicians. Medical Decision Making, 23, 293300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 473493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65, 272292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loewenstein, G. F., Hsee, C. K., Weber, E. U., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, G. F., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In. Davidson, R. J. Goldsmith, H. H., & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 619642). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mandel, D. R. (2005). Threats to democracy: A judgment and decision-making perspective. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 5, 209222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, H. (1977). Some situational factors improving cognitive conflict reduction and interpersonal understanding. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 21, 217234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrath, J. E. (1984). Groups: Interaction and performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Milter, R. G., Darling, T. A., & Mumpower, J. L. (1996). The effects of substantive task characteristics on negotiators’ ability to reach efficient agreements. Acta Psychologica, 93, 207228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mosler, H. J., Schwarz, K., Ammann, F., & Gutscher, H. (2001). Computer simulation as a method of further developing a theory: Simulating the elaboration likelihood model. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 201215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumpower, J. L. (1988). An analysis of the judgmental components of negotiation and a proposed judgmentally-oriented approach to mediation. In Brehmer, B. & Joyce, C. R. B. (Eds.), Human judgment: The SJT view (pp. 465502). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumpower, J. L. (1991). The judgment policies of negotiators and the structure of negotiation problems. Management Science, 37, 13041324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumpower, J. L., & Hammond, K. R. (1974). Entangled task dimensions: An impediment to interpersonal learning. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 11, 377389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumpower, J. L., & Rohrbaugh, J. (1996). Negotiation and design: Supporting resource allocation decisions through analytical mediation. Group Decision and Negotiation, 5, 385409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mumpower, J. L., & Stewart, T. R. (1996). Expert judgment and disagreement. Thinking and Reasoning, 2, 191211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelled, L. H., Eisenhardt, K. E., & Xin, K. R. (1999). Exploring the black box: An analysis of work group diversity, conflict, and performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44, 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pohl, R. F., Einsenhauer, M., & Hardt, O. (2003). SARA: A cognitive process model to simulate the anchoring effect and hindsight bias. Memory, 11, 337356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qualls, W. J., & Jaffe, F. (1992). Measuring conflict in household decision behavior: Read my lips and read my mind. Advances in Consumer Research, 19, 522531.Google Scholar
Rappoport, L. (1969). Cognitive conflict as a function of socially-induced cognitive differences. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 13, 143148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reagan-Cirincione, P. (1994). Improving the accuracy of group judgment: A process intervention combining group facilitation, social judgment analysis, and information technology. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58, 246270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rieskamp, J., & Hoffrage, U. (1999). When do people use simple heuristics, and how can we tell? In Gigerenzer, G. Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group. Simple heuristics that make us smart (pp. 141167). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rohrbaugh, J. (1979). Improving the quality of group judgment: Social judgment analysis and the Delphi technique. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 24, 7392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohrbaugh, J. (1988). Cognitive conflict tasks and small group processes. In Brehmer, B. & Joyce, C. R. B. (Eds.), Human judgment: The SJT view (pp. 199226). Amsterdam: North-Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, G. L., Menasco, M. B., & Curry, D. J. (1982). When disagreement facilitates performance in judgment tasks: Effects of different forms of cognitive conflict, information environments, and human information processing characteristics. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 29, 287306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwenk, C. R., & Cosier, R. A. (1980). Effects of the expert, devil’s advocate, and dialectical inquiry methods on prediction performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 26, 409424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sengupta, K., & Te’eni, D. (1993). Cognitive feedback in GDSS: Improving control and convergence. MIS Quarterly, March, 87113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P. (2001). Cigarette smokers: Rational actors or rational fools? In P. Slovic (Ed.), Smoking: Risk, perception, and policy (pp. 97124). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Steinmann, D. O., Smith, T. H., Jurdem, L. G., & Hammond, K. R. (1975). Application and evaluation of social judgment theory in policy formulation: An example (Report No. 174). Boulder: University of Colorado, Institute of Behavioral Science, Program on Human Judgment and Social Interaction.Google Scholar
Summers, D. A., Ashworth, C. D, & Feldman-Summers, S. (1977). Judgment processes and interpersonal conflict related to societal problem solutions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 7, 163174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todd, F. J., & Hammond, K. R. (1965). Differential feedback in two multiple-cue probability learning tasks. Behavioral Science, 10, 429435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todd, F. J., Hammond, K. R., & Wilkins, M. M. (1966). Differential effects of ambiguous and exact feedback on two-person conflict and compromise. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 10, 8897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, L. R. (1964). A suggested alternative formulation in the developments by Hursch, Hammond and Hursch, and by Hammond, Hursch and Todd. Psychological Review, 71, 528530.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Kleef, G. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2006). Supplication and appeasement in conflict and negotiation: The interpersonal effects of disappointment, worry, guilt, and regret. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 124142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zsambok, C. E., & Klein, G. (1997). Naturalistic decision making. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1 Lens model for study of interpersonal conflict and interpersonal learning (adapted from Hammond [1965] and Hammond et al., [1966b]).

Figure 1

Figure 2 Co-citation network of publications 1965–2007 (2 years slice, parameters c, cc, ccv: 3, 2, 25; 3, 3, 25; 4, 4, 25)

Figure 2