Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T19:33:35.823Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Willingness to pay for female-made wine: Evidence from an online experiment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 March 2024

Alicia Gallais
Affiliation:
Kedge Business School, Centre of Excellence Food, Wine and Hospitality, Talence, France
Florine Livat*
Affiliation:
Kedge Business School, Centre of Excellence Food, Wine and Hospitality, Talence, France
*
Corresponding author: Florine Livat, email: [email protected]

Abstract

The wine industry, considered to be male-dominated, has seen a growing share of women winemakers. Using a randomized online experiment, we investigate how the producer’s gender influences consumers’ willingness to pay for the wine. Gender can be identified either from the first name of the producer or from a gendered group of wine producers. Using a Tobit and a double-hurdle model, our results suggest that consumers’ willingness to pay is lower for wine produced by female winemaker groups. This reduction appears to be particularly pronounced when the consumer is male.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of American Association of Wine Economists.

I. Introduction

Physical brand design characteristics, such as logos, font, color, and brand name, can influence consumers’ perceptions of brand masculinity and femininity (Lieven et al., Reference Lieven, Grohmann, Herrmann, Landwehr and Van Tilburg2015). Some wine descriptors used by experts can be classified as feminine or masculine (Masset, Terrier, and Livat, Reference Masset, Terrier and Livat2023). In the wine industry, the name of the winemaker or the owner of the winery (the producer hereafter) can appear on the label, enabling the consumer to infer the producer’s gender from the first name (Ackermann and Zimmer, Reference Ackermann and Zimmer2021; Cassidy, Kelly, and Sharoni, Reference Cassidy, Kelly and Sharoni1999). Because gender stereotypes might give rise to biased judgments and decisions, transactions on product markets can be affected by such gender recognition. Through a randomized online experiment, this article investigates how the producer’s gender might influence consumers’ willingness to pay (WTP) for wine.

Industries themselves can be gendered. Collins (Reference Collins2015, p. 416) notes that male-dominated industries “share a history of favoring and privileging men over women.” This is the case for construction workers (Denissen, Reference Denissen2010), aeronautical engineers (Wright, Reference Wright2016), and those who work in the wine industry (Bryant and Garnham, Reference Bryant and Garnham2014). By contrast, librarians, flight attendants, nurses, and primary school teachers are deemed female-dominated occupations (Simpson, Reference Simpson2014). However, most of these industries have seen a rising share of women (men) entering male- (female)-dominated industries and occupations. This is the case in the wine industry, where women outnumber men in many viticulture and oenology degree programs, representing up to 60% of a cohort (Escudier, Reference Escudier2014). In a male- (female)-dominated industry, it is unclear how male- or female-produced goods are valued by consumers.

This article analyzes how consumers value wines when they know the gender of the producer. In some respects, wine is a craft and a cultural product (Charters et al., Reference Charters, Demossier, Dutton, Harding, Maguire, Marks and Unwin2022; Marks, Reference Marks2011). The name of the producer, which can carry information about his/her gender, can be used to differentiate wines. Here, we investigate consumers’ WTP for a wine with (1) a gender-neutral label, (2) a label highlighting a male producer, and (3) a label highlighting a female producer. Information about the producer’s gender can be provided either through his/her individual name or through a collective in which producers decide to group together. Indeed, female winemakers create gendered groups to provide support and mentoring and to collectively showcase wines made by women (Le Brun, Guétat-Bernard, and Annes, Reference Le Brun, Guétat-Bernard and Annes2019). Santos, Marques, and Ratten (Reference Santos, Marques and Ratten2019) have shown that, in Portugal, some female wine producers capitalize on gender by displaying their names on labels, showing affirmation and self-identification, individually and/or collectively, through coalitions. In this way, the gender of the producer can be seen as a salient dimension of a product that can be used as a means of differentiation.

Our contribution to the literature is twofold. On the one hand, previous research has widely investigated WTP for the characteristics of wine, in particular the corporate social responsibility aspects of wine production, mainly from an environmental perspective (e.g., Lim and Reed, Reference Lim and Reed2020; Sogari, Mora, and Menozzi, Reference Sogari, Mora and Menozzi2016). Social aspects of sustainability have been less investigated (the study of fair-trade wines is an exception; see, for instance, Back et al. (Reference Back, Liu, Niklas, Storchmann and Vink2019)). To the best of our knowledge, the valuation of the gender of the wine producer, in particular in the context of a group of female producers, has not been studied so far. On the other hand, women influence the wine industry. Even as a minority in the Champagne industry, women affect the way markets function (Ody-Brasier and Fernandez-Mateo, Reference Ody-Brasier and Fernandez-Mateo2017). Although it is often assumed that wine prestige and reputation require a strong male identity (Chauvin, Reference Chauvin2011; Simonnet-Toussaint, Reference Simonnet-Toussaint2006), women are gaining authority and recognition in the industry. More empirical evidence is needed because most of the existing research is qualitative (see Livat and Jaffré (Reference Livat, Jaffré, Charters, Demossier, Dutton, Harding, Smith Maguire, Marks and Unwin2022) for a review). In particular, even if differences between female and male wine judges (Bodington and Malfeito-Ferreira, Reference Bodington and Malfeito-Ferreira2018), as well as the influence of female CEOs (Galbreath, Reference Galbreath2015), have been studied, the consumer response remains unknown.

In this article, using a randomized online experiment, we show that the gender of the producer matters in wine valuation. Overall, our results suggest that male consumers express a lower WTP for wines made by a winemaker from a female-only group of producers.

The remainder of the article is organized as follows: The background is provided in Section II; Section III describes our online experiment; Section IV presents our empirical analysis; and Section V is dedicated to the discussion and conclusion.

II. Background

A. Gendered products

According to the self-congruency theory, individuals have a desire for products and brands that reflect their own image or identity, including their gender (Cowart, Fox, and Wilson, Reference Cowart, Fox and Wilson2008; Fugate and Phillips, Reference Fugate and Phillips2010). Lieven et al. (Reference Lieven, Grohmann, Herrmann, Landwehr and Van Tilburg2015) show that product category perception matters and that congruence between brand and product category masculinity and femininity is associated with positive consumer responses. Neale, Robbie, and Martin (Reference Neale, Robbie and Martin2016) show that masculine consumers prefer masculine brands and react negatively to feminine brands; these authors also describe an asymmetric, incongruent brand rejection because feminine consumers are more accepting of masculine brands. Considering craft beer as male-typed and cupcakes as female-typed product markets, Tak, Correll, and Soule (Reference Tak, Correll and Soule2019) measure a negative asymmetric bias: products made by women are disadvantaged in male-typed markets, but products made by men are not disadvantaged in female-typed markets.

In the case of books, Weinberg and Kapelner (Reference Weinberg and Kapelner2018) find evidence that there is an unequal distribution of published books by male and female authors by genre. The authors also found that genres written predominantly by female authors are devaluated and books published by female authors are priced lower than their male counterparts; however, there is less inequality in independent publishing than in traditional publishing. On the opposite side, Kapelner and Weinberg (Reference Kapelner and Weinberg2019) show that the assessment of quality, interest, and WTP does not vary according to the author’s gender, in male- as well as female-dominated genres. However, gender bias also occurs in academia, where the perceived scientific quality of scholarly communications is higher for male authors, with no influence shown by the respondent’s gender (Knobloch-Westerwick, Glynn, and Huge, Reference Knobloch-Westerwick, Glynn and Huge2013). A similar bias can be associated with female sellers at eBay auctions; Kricheli-Katz and Regev (Reference Kricheli-Katz and Regev2016) show that women sellers receive fewer bids and lower final prices than male sellers—with similar characteristics of the same good—even if the price gap varies among product categories. The authors conclude that “people tend to assign a lower value to products when sold by women rather than by men” (Kricheli-Katz and Regev, Reference Kricheli-Katz and Regev2016, p. 7), which is a form of discrimination.

B. Collective claim of gender

Previous research has focused on an individual signature or mention of the producer’s name. In the wine industry, some female winemakers team up to create producer groups and use these networks to communicate the features of their wines to consumers, narrated in a feminine, cohesive, and united voice (Santos, Marques, and Ratten, Reference Santos, Marques and Ratten2019). A study of the Gaillac wine region in Southwest France highlights that traditional male-oriented professional organizations do not address the priorities of women working there (Le Brun, Guétat-Bernard, and Annes, Reference Le Brun, Guétat-Bernard and Annes2019). Moreover, as wine fellowships have traditionally excluded women from wine rituals, female winemakers lack interaction with the governance of the industry, as stressed by Pavel (Reference Pavel2012) in the French context. This is in line with several studies analyzing the role of female farmers in North American professional organizations, which show that they are underrepresented, feel isolated, and are not taken seriously (e.g., Sachs et al., Reference Sachs, Barbercheck, Braiser, Kiernan, Terman, Sachs, Barbercheck, Brasier, Kiernan and Terman2016). In these situations, horizontal networks might help women gain and demonstrate their professionalism, a key aspect of women’s advancement in the wine industry (Duarte Alonso, Kok, and Galbreath, Reference Duarte Alonso, Kok and Galbreath2021). As such, as women’s numbers grow in the wine industry, they are implicitly incentivized to coalesce and create new networks claiming collectively their identity, including gender identity.

C. Willingness to pay (WTP)

WTP is assessed as the amount of money a consumer is willing to part with to gain an equivalent utility derived from a product (Lusk and Hudson, Reference Lusk and Hudson2004). There are many empirical strategies to assess the WTP, including those that are incentive-compatible and generate revealed preference data, such as the Vickrey auction (e.g., Vecchio (Reference Vecchio2013) for an application to wine) and the Becker-DeGroot-Marschak mechanism (see Bazoche et al. (Reference Bazoche, Combris, Giraud-Héraud, Traversac, Giraud-Héraud and Pichery2013) for an application to wine). Others are non-incentive compatible and generate stated preference data—that is, contingent evaluation (e.g., Loureiro (Reference Loureiro2003) for an application to wine) and discrete choice experiments (Capitello et al., Reference Capitello, Agnoli, Charters and Begalli2021; Train, Reference Train2009)—where both the payment of the good as well as its provision are hypothetical. The former, associated with revealed preferences through actual payment, promises higher validity than the latter, which is likely to overstate WTP in hypothetical valuation questions.

However, contingent evaluation remains widely used in wine economics. For instance, Thiene et al. (Reference Thiene, Galletto, Scarpa and Boatto2013, p. 297) include the following question in their survey: “How much are you willing to pay for a bottle of IGT/CDO Prosecco wine in a restaurant, supermarket, winery, and wine shop?” Lusk (Reference Lusk2003) proposes to add “cheap talk” to eliminate the hypothetical bias associated with the stated WTP: a short text that warns individuals about the hypothetical bias phenomenon prior to asking the valuation question. Empirical evidence suggests that cheap talk can be effective in mitigating this bias (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Allen, Stevens and Weatherhead2005; Silva et al., Reference Silva, Nayga, Campbell and Park2011). This is the strategy we adopt for our online experiment.

III. Online experiment

We designed a 2 x 5 (i.e., 10 wines) between-subject randomized online experiment. We presented two different label designs, that is, traditional and modern, and five sets of information about the producer, including no producer information, which we used as a reference point.

Participants were French-speaking respondents because French is a gendered language. French grammar distinguishes nouns by gender, most of which are associated with a male or female gender tag. Anything that is attached to a noun (pronouns, adjectives, and verbs) will also reflect the assigned gender (DeFranza, Mishra, and Mishra, Reference DeFranza, Mishra and Mishra2020). For instance, vigneron is the French noun for a male winemaker, and vigneronne is the feminine variant; the spelling as well as the pronunciation are different. This is why we decided to run the online experiment in France and in the French-speaking part of Belgium (Wallonia). In addition to language, using these two countries enabled us to gather data from a wine-producing country (France) and a non-producing country (Belgium), as well as examine any cultural differences.

The labels used for the experiment contain exactly the same information (winery name, appellation of origin, vintage, and other mandatory information), except for the information related to the winemaker. Two styles of labels were also used (traditional vs. original; see Celhay and Passebois, Reference Celhay and Passebois2011). Because we use fictitious labels and producer names, our online experiment relies on a kind of deception. If deception appears as standard in the psychology and marketing fields, it is often thought that it should be banned in experimental economics (Cooper, Reference Cooper2014). Indeed, control of the experiment is lost if subjects doubt experimental instructions and material and form their own theories about payoffs. However, Cooper (Reference Cooper2014, p. 113) notes that deception can be considered acceptable when the following four rules are met: “(1) The deception does not harm subjects beyond what is typical for an economic experiment without deception. (2) The study would be prohibitively difficult to conduct without deception. (3) Subjects are adequately debriefed after the fact about the presence of deception. (4) The value of the study is sufficiently high to merit the potential costs associated with the use of deception.” Here, rules (1) and (2) are met. The kind of ambiguity used here is found in most wine experiments, as observed by Masset and Raub (Reference Masset and Raub2023). Regarding the last rule, Cooper (Reference Cooper2014) notes that the value of a study lies in the eye of the beholder. But he also mentions that “most economists would argue that the discrimination studies using deception were sufficiently important […] that the use of deception was justified” (p. 114). Given that our aim is to analyze if WTP varies with information about the gender of the producer, we believe that deception is necessary to investigate this kind of discrimination. Rule 3 is trickier: given the number of respondents, who remain anonymous in an online setting, it is hard to organize a debrief. A professional panelist who sent out the survey mentioned that those who participate in online studies might use some kind of misinformation and/or ambiguity. On the one hand, study participants have to certify that they are committed to answering spontaneously and independently from previous studies they might have participated in. On the other hand, the participant manages the frequency of survey participation requests to avoid any learning process, generating the belief that experimenters are liars.

Each wine (except the reference points) appears with a sentence indicating who made the wine or whether the producer belonged to a particular producer group, as shown in Table 1. Wines 1 and 6 are the reference points, as no information about the winemaker is provided. Wines 2 and 7 are made by winemaker Georges Cadieux, and wines 3 and 8 are made by winemaker Nathalie Panetier. Both names come from a randomly generated list. In French, the first names Georges and Nathalie are, respectively, masculine and feminine. Moreover, their phonetic structure suggests a male versus female personality, respectively, as shown by Wu, Klink, and Guo (Reference Wu, Klink and Guo2013). On wines 4 and 9, the sticker Fémivin refers to a group of female winemakers only. To our knowledge, in France, no such producer group exists for male winemakers. However, Vignerons Indépendants is a French winemaking trade association that includes the noun “male winemakers” in its name (vignerons), even if it is not dedicated to male winemakers only. We use its sticker on wines 5 and 10 as a reverse of the female winemaker group.

Table 1. 2 x 5 between-subject online experiment, WTP (euros), and perceived quality for each wine (N = 1,351)

The survey took place from March 25, 2021, to April 2, 2021, under almost unrestricted circumstances (i.e., no strict lockdown due to the COVID-19 pandemic in France and Belgium). Respondents from France and Belgium were randomly drawn from a panel of consumers selected by the company Respondi, a professional panelist that sends out survey links by email. Respondents are rewarded only if they fully complete the survey with points that are transformed into gift cards usable in several stores. A screening question enables the selecting of respondents who have consumed wine over the last three months. Each respondent was assigned randomly to one of the ten conditions. The final sample consisted of 1,000 French and 500 Belgian respondents, with a distribution among experimental conditions that ranged from 9.33% to 10.51% (see Table 1). We excluded respondents who preferred not to declare their income, which left us with 1,351 observations.

The survey started with a randomized experiment. Respondents were asked first to assess how they perceive the quality of the wine over a 5-point scale (from 1—poor quality to 5—excellent quality) and if they were willing to purchase the wine using a 5-point Likert scale (from 1—extremely unlikely to purchase to 5—extremely likely to purchase). Then, cheap talk was used right before the valuation question, when respondents were invited to give their WTP, in euros, for the same bottle of wine and to indicate 0 if they were not willing to pay for the wine; 100 euros was the maximum possible amount. Average WTP, perceived quality (PQ), measured on a 5-point scale, and WTP measured on a 5-point Likert scale are shown in Table 1 for each wine. An attention check question was also included in this part of the questionnaire.

Sociodemographics, as well as wine-related characteristics and opinions of respondents, were questioned in the final section of the questionnaire. We know the respondents’ wine consumption frequency and if they were used to consuming red wine. Objective wine knowledge was reflected in a score of over 5 points obtained from a set of five questions inspired by Velikova, Howell, and Dodd’s (Reference Velikova, Howell and Dodd2015) scale. Respondents received 1 point for each correct answer, 0 otherwise. The questions are presented in Appendix 1. The survey also included a question about the perceived quality of Bordeaux wines as a whole as a proxy for the collective reputation of the wine industry (Landon and Smith, Reference Landon and Smith1997, Reference Landon and Smith1998).

Summary statistics of sociodemographic and wine-related characteristics and opinions are presented in Tables 2 and 3. The sample is in line with the age and gender proportions of the population of interest (in this case, wine consumers) in the two countries studied (Wine Intelligence, 2022, 2023). We note that the average amount of money usually spent for a bottle of wine is similarly distributed across all conditions (details available from the authors on request).

Table 2. Descriptive characteristics of respondents (N = 1,351)

Table 3. Wine characteristics and opinions of respondents (N = 1,351)

IV. Empirical analysis

To analyze the effect of the winemaker’s gender on the consumer’s WTP, we estimated a series of regressions in which the WTP was a function of the presented label, controlling for sociodemographic and wine-related characteristics and opinions of the respondents. Two empirical issues required additional attention. First, WTP is a left- and right-censored variable because respondents declared 0 euros when they were not willing to pay for the bottle of wine, and 100 euros was the maximum amount they could declare. Said differently, our dependent variable has a corner at 0 and, in such cases, the distribution of WTP exhibits a spike at 0 with about 5% of respondents, as shown in Figure 1. This implies that we should use some censored regression model, like the Tobit-type models.

Figure 1. Distribution of WTP (N = 1,351).

Second, our sample might include protest responses as well as out-of-market respondents. Indeed, protest responses are identified with respondents who claimed that they were willing to purchase the good but then declared a 0 WTPFootnote 1; some other respondents opted out of the market, claiming that they were not willing to purchase the good, but some declared a non-0 WTPFootnote 2 (see Figure 2). In that case, a spike-based modeling approach à la Kriström (Reference Kriström1997) can be implemented. It uses a hurdle model that combines discrete and continuous parts (Brown and Taylor, Reference Brown and Taylor2000), that is, a set of two equations: a discrete component models the participation decision, estimating the probability that a respondent will state a positive WTP, and a continuous component models the contribution decision, with WTP as a dependent variable only for those respondents stating a positive WTP. These two equations can be estimated separately in two stages (Reiser and Shechter, Reference Reiser and Shechter1999; for an application, see Brown and Taylor, (Reference Brown and Taylor2000)). Such an empirical strategy excludes respondents willing to purchase the good but only willing to pay 0 because of current circumstances or characteristics. Moreover, it can overestimate the effect because it produces higher values than other models (Bengochea-Morancho, Fuertes-Eugenio, and Del Saz-Salazar, Reference Bengochea-Morancho, Fuertes-Eugenio and Del Saz-Salazar2005). Along the same vein, another possibility is to estimate a double-hurdle model (Cragg, Reference Cragg1971): the first hurdle refers to the participation decision, the second one to the contribution decision, and both decisions are modeled simultaneously. The double-hurdle model enables distinguishing among respondents declaring a WTP of 0 because they do not want to purchase the good and those for whom current circumstances or characteristics dictate a WTP of 0 but would like to purchase the good, as suggested by Engel and Moffatt (Reference Engel and Moffatt2014). In Figure 2, we present only the second part of the double-hurdle model, that is, the contribution decision, to explain differences in WTP.

Figure 2. Sequence of valuation questions with percentage of respondents.

Clinch and Murphy (Reference Clinch and Murphy2001) recommend using a range of models to infer WTP. In Table 4, we present robust estimation results using the Tobit and the double-hurdle modeling approaches for two empirical specifications: model 1 includes a dummy variable for each label, and model 2 assesses each label against the gender of the respondent. All specifications include the same set of sociodemographic characteristics of respondents: gender, age, country (France or Belgium), and monthly net household income. They also contain the following wine-related indicators: wine consumption frequency, if the respondent is the primary wine shopper, level of objective wine knowledge, and the average amount of money usually spent on a bottle of wine. We also include a collective reputation indicator and control for the perceived quality of each bottle of wine because it is traditionally a determinant of WTP for wine (e.g., Lunardo and Rickard, Reference Lunardo and Rickard2019) and for the kind of label (traditional vs. modern). In Table 4, we report only the coefficients for the variables of interest. More detailed results are available in Appendix 2.

Table 4. Estimation results

Robust standard errors in parentheses.

*** p < 0.01, **p < 0.05, *p < 0.1

As expected, the perceived quality of each specific label increased the WTP significantly, and the collective reputation of Bordeaux wines, such as the label’s style, did not prove significant. We also got a negative effect from age, from France compared to Belgium, and from the level of objective wine knowledge. Unsurprisingly, the effect of the average amount of money spent on a bottle of wine was positive.

The Tobit estimation of model 1 highlights a price reduction associated with the group of female winemakers (–1.17 euros, significant at 10% only), although it is not significant in the double-hurdle model. When assessed against the gender of the consumer (model 2), our estimation results suggest that male respondents ask for a price reduction when the wine is produced by a female producer group (–1.4 euros with the Tobit model, significant at 10%, almost –3 euros with the double-hurdle model, significant at 5%). The Tobit estimation of model 2 also highlights that female consumers devalue wines when the name of a female winemaker appears on the label (–1.8 euros, significant at 5%), but this is not confirmed by the double-hurdle model. Here, we get a result in line with the empirical evidence provided by Tak, Correll, and Soule (Reference Tak, Correll and Soule2019): female products, produced in a male-typed industry, are disadvantaged by male consumers. Our results show that this is the case when women form an all-female producer group. As such, the study respondents react differently to the group of producers than to a single female producer.

V. Discussion and conclusion

Our results suggest that the gender of the winemaker matters in wine valuation. Specifically, the wine industry is still male-dominated, and it seems that collective strategies supporting women are not appreciated by consumers. Interestingly, we do not find any significant difference in the WTP between products made by male and female winemakers when the gender information is communicated through the producer’s first name. This can suggest that the winemaker’s name is associated with a craftsperson genuinely making the wine. In that case, the name is a means to build authenticity (Maguire, Reference Maguire2018), whatever the gender of the producer, which makes no difference in terms of WTP.

We get different results for a group of female producers only. When a female winemaker claims to belong to a group of women making wine, consumers, especially male consumers, want a price reduction for the group’s wine. Mentoring and networking can help female winemakers address several challenges not considered by traditional professional organizations (Le Brun, Guétat-Bernard, and Annes, Reference Le Brun, Guétat-Bernard and Annes2019). The Women Do Wine group (France), the Diversity in Wine Leadership Forum (North America), the Australian Women in Wine Awards, and the Women in Wine Leadership Symposium (U.S.-based but international) seek to support and mentor women and highlight their role in the wine industry, but these groups of women are new and operate in only a few wine-producing countries (Livat and Jaffré, Reference Livat, Jaffré, Charters, Demossier, Dutton, Harding, Smith Maguire, Marks and Unwin2022). We think that they are not well enough known at this time and that there is a lack of familiarity and fluency on the consumers’ side, which can produce the desire for a price reduction. Moreover, because they challenge professional institutions and do not align with traditional communication strategies, these groups of female producers can signal much more than information about the producer: they can draw a political statement and aim at lobbying (English, Reference English2022). Hence, belonging to these organizations can be perceived as activism, associated with negative stereotypes (e.g., eccentric and militant; see, for instance, Bashir et al. (Reference Bashir, Lockwood, Chasteen, Nadolny and Noyes2013)), and generate a backlash discourse (Dyer and Hurd, Reference Dyer and Hurd2018), especially on the male consumer side.

Previous research has shown that, in California, female winemakers are more highly acclaimed by experts proportional to their presence in the field than male winemakers (Gilbert & Gilbert, Reference Gilbert and Gilbert2015), suggesting some inverse discrimination. However, transactions on product markets can be affected by biased judgments and decisions. If the quality of wines made by female winemakers does not matter from the experts’ point of view, consumers might demand a price reduction, which reflects negative discrimination. This is highlighted by our results in the case of male consumers of products from female winemaker groups. This specific discrimination might also come from their peers, that is, female consumers, which deserves further research.

The study could be replicated in other countries with a gendered language but a high gender balance, such as the French-speaking part of Switzerland, a country with one of the lowest UN Gender Inequality Index rankings (United Nations, 2022). English-speaking countries, that is, those using a non-gendered language such as the United States, should also be studied. In the same way, a laboratory experiment could be carried out to overcome the hypothetical situation. This would involve a blind tasting of the same wine presented as having been made by a male winemaker on the one hand and a female winemaker on the other. Complementary research could investigate in depth the place and role of women in the wine industry and, more generally, how the growing diversity affects the industry. For example, some field experiments would be helpful to analyze if and how a female wine salesperson compared to a male one influences purchasing behavior in the same wine store. It could be used to study how a female wine tour guide or a female wine steward in a restaurant, compared to a male one, affects customer satisfaction. These questions also make sense for other minorities in the wine industry, for example, people of color, people who are disabled, and LGBTQIA+ people, because the industry is mainly dominated by white people (Inglis and Ho, Reference Inglis, Ho, Charters, Demossier, Dutton, Harding, Smith Maguire, Marks and Unwin2022).

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge helpful comments and suggestions from the editors and an anonymous referee. The article has also benefited from discussions with Haiyan Song and Olivier Gergaud, as well as AAWE 2022 conference and ARWHM 2022 workshop participants. All remaining errors are the authors’ own responsibility.

Appendix 1

Questions used to measure objective wine knowledge

The order of answers has been randomized. The correct answer appears in bold.

  1. 1) In your opinion, the term “Merlot” refers to:

    1. a) A wine-producing area

    2. b) A red grape variety

    3. c) A red-fruit aroma

    4. d) I don’t know

  2. 2) In your opinion, the term “Sancerre” refers to

    1. a) A wine-producing area

    2. b) A red grape variety

    3. c) A white grape variety

    4. d) I don’t know

  3. 3) In your opinion, the term “Chardonnay” refers to

    1. a) A vine variety for the production of red wine

    2. b) A vine variety for the production of white wine

    3. c) A wine-producing area

    4. d) I don’t know

  4. 4) Where do you think the Napa Valley wine region is?

    1. a) In Italy

    2. b) In Argentina

    3. c) In England

    4. d) In California

    5. e) In Australia

    6. f) In France

    7. g) In South Africa

    8. h) I don’t know

  5. 5) Which Latin American country do you think is best known for producing wine from the Malbec grape variety?

    1. a) Uruguay

    2. b) Chile

    3. c) Peru

    4. d) Argentina

    5. e) I don’t know

Appendix 2Appendix 2

Detailed estimation results

Robust standard errors in parentheses.

*** p<0.01, **p<0.05, *p<0.1

Footnotes

Robust standard errors in parentheses.

*** p<0.01, **p<0.05, *p<0.1

1 These are called protest responses because they are often highlighted in the case of the WTP for the provision of public goods. Respondents might be protesting about the valuation exercise even if they hold positive values for the good. In that case, WTP does not convey correct information on the respondents’ preferences, and it is necessary to discriminate between individuals who are not interested in the good and protesters.

2 The sequence of valuation questions can be asked in reversed order, which does not change the empirical methodology (Reiser and Shechter, Reference Reiser and Shechter1999).

References

Ackermann, T., and Zimmer, C. (2021). The sound of gender–Correlations of name phonology and gender across languages. Linguistics, 59(4), 11431177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Back, R. M., Liu, X., Niklas, B., Storchmann, K., and Vink, N. (2019). Margins of fair trade wine along the supply chain: Evidence from South African wine in the US market. Journal of Wine Economics, 14(3), 274297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bashir, N. Y., Lockwood, P., Chasteen, A. L., Nadolny, D., and Noyes, I. (2013). The ironic impact of activists: Negative stereotypes reduce social change influence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(7), 614626.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bazoche, P., Combris, P., Giraud-Héraud, E., and Traversac, J. B. (2013). Willingness to pay for appellation of origin: Results of an experiment with pinot noir wines in France and Germany. In Giraud-Héraud, E. and Pichery, M.-C. (eds.), Wine Economics: Quantitative Studies and Empirical Applications, 129145. London: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bengochea-Morancho, A., Fuertes-Eugenio, A. M., and Del Saz-Salazar, S. (2005). A comparison of empirical models used to infer the willingness to pay in contingent valuation. Empirical Economics, 30(1), 235244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodington, J., and Malfeito-Ferreira, M. (2018). Do female and male judges assign the same ratings to the same wines? Large sample results. Journal of Wine Economics, 13(4), 403408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, K. M., and Taylor, L. O. (2000). Do as you say, say as you do: Evidence on gender differences in actual and stated contributions to public goods. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 43(1), 127139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, L., and Garnham, B. (2014). The embodiment of women in wine: Gender inequality and gendered inscriptions of the working body in a corporate wine organization. Gender, Work & Organization, 21(5), 411426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capitello, R., Agnoli, L., Charters, S., and Begalli, D. (2021). Labelling environmental and terroir attributes: Young Italian consumers’ wine preferences. Journal of Cleaner Production, 304, .CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, K. W., Kelly, M. H., and Sharoni, L. A. J. (1999). Inferring gender from name phonology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 362381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Celhay, F., and Passebois, J. (2011). Wine labelling: Is it time to break with tradition? A study of the moderating role of perceived risk. International Journal of Wine Business Research, 23(4), 318337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charters, S., Demossier, M., Dutton, J., Harding, G., Maguire, J. S., Marks, D., and Unwin, T. (eds.). (2022). The Routledge Handbook of Wine and Culture. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chauvin, P. M. (2011). Réputation et division du travail. Ethnologie Française, 41(1), 131140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clinch, J., and Murphy, A. (2001). Modelling winners and losers in contingent valuation of public goods: Appropriate welfare measures and econometric analysis. Economic Journal, 111(470), 420443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, J. C. (2015). Characteristics of “masculinized” industries: Gay men as a provocative exception to male privilege and gendered rules. Human Resource Development Review, 14(4), 415441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, D. J. (2014). A note on deception in economic experiments. Journal of Wine Economics, 9(2), 111114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowart, K. O., Fox, G. L., and Wilson, A. E. (2008). A structural look at consumer innovativeness and self‐congruence in new product purchases. Psychology & Marketing, 25(12), 11111130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cragg, J. G. (1971). Some statistical models for limited dependent variables with application to the demand for durable goods. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 39(5), 829844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeFranza, D., Mishra, H., and Mishra, A. (2020). How language shapes prejudice against women: An examination across 45 world languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 119(1), 722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denissen, A. M. (2010). The right tools for the job: Constructing gender meanings and identities in the male-dominated building trades. Human Relations, 63(7), 10511069.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duarte Alonso, A., Kok, S., and Galbreath, J. (2021). Entrepreneurial women in the wine industry: A study in emerging economies. International Journal of Wine Business Research, 33(2), 197216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyer, S., and Hurd, F. (2018). Changing perceptions about feminists and (still not) claiming a feminist identity. Gender and Education, 30(4), 435449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, C., and Moffatt, P. G. (2014). dhreg, xtdhreg, and bootdhreg: Commands to implement double-hurdle regression. Stata Journal, 14(4), 778797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
English, A. (2022). Lobbying beyond the legislature: Challenges and biases in women’s organizations’ participation in rulemaking. Politics & Gender, 18(4), 10771111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Escudier, J. L. (2014). La résistible accession des femmes à l’acquisition des Savoirs Viticoles 1950–2010. Pour, 2, 127134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fugate, D. L., and Phillips, J. (2010). Product gender perceptions and antecedents of product gender congruence. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 27(3), 251261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galbreath, J. (2015). A study of women in top business roles: The case of the wine industry. International Journal of Wine Business Research, 27(2), 143158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, L. A., and Gilbert, J. C. (2015). A case study of California’s major wineries: Assessing the progress and prospects for winemakers who are women. American Association of Wine Economists Working Paper No. 178, May.Google Scholar
Inglis, D., and Ho, H. K. (2022). Beyond white: On wine and ethnicity. In Charters, S., Demossier, M., Dutton, J., Harding, G., Smith Maguire, J., Marks, D., and Unwin, T. (eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Wine and Culture, 415423. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapelner, A., and Weinberg, D. (2019). Do readers judge books by author gender? Results from a randomized experiment. Socius, 5, https://doi.org/10.1177/2378023119868893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knobloch-Westerwick, S., Glynn, C. J., and Huge, M. (2013). The Matilda effect in science communication: An experiment on gender bias in publication quality perceptions and collaboration interest. Science Communication, 35(5), 603625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kricheli-Katz, T., and Regev, T. (2016). How many cents on the dollar? Women and men in product markets. Science Advances, 2(2), 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kriström, B. (1997). Spike models in contingent valuation. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 79(3), 10131023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landon, S., and Smith, C. E. (1997). The use of quality and reputation indicators by consumers: The case of Bordeaux wine. Journal of Consumer Policy, 20(3), 289323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landon, S., and Smith, C. E. (1998). Quality expectations, reputation, and price. Southern Economic Journal, 64(3), 628647.Google Scholar
Le Brun, C., Guétat-Bernard, H., and Annes, A. (2019). L’Emergence de collectifs féminins en viticulture: Vers un renouvellement de la structuration de la filière? Interrogations? Revue Pluridisciplinaire de Sciences Humaines et Sociales, 29.Google Scholar
Lieven, T., Grohmann, B., Herrmann, A., Landwehr, J. R., and Van Tilburg, M. (2015). The effect of brand design on brand gender perceptions and brand preference. European Journal of Marketing, 49(1/2), 146169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lim, K. H., and Reed, M. (2020). Do ecolabels cheapen wines? Journal of Cleaner Production, 245, .CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livat, F., and Jaffré, C. (2022). Women in wine … occasionally: Gendered roles in the wine industry. In Charters, S., Demossier, M., Dutton, J., Harding, G., Smith Maguire, J., Marks, D., and Unwin, T. (eds.), Routledge Handbook of Wine and Culture, 320330. London: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loureiro, M. L. (2003). Rethinking new wines: Implications of local and environmentally friendly labels. Food Policy, 28(5–6), 547560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lunardo, R., and Rickard, B. (2019). How do consumers respond to fun wine labels? British Food Journal, 22(8), 26032619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lusk, J. L. (2003). Effects of cheap talk on consumer willingness‐to‐pay for golden rice. American Journal of Agricultural Economics, 85(4), 840856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lusk, J. L., and Hudson, D. (2004). Willingness‐to‐pay estimates and their relevance to agribusiness decision making. Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy, 26(2), 152169.Google Scholar
Maguire, J. S. (2018). Wine, the authenticity taste regime, and rendering craft. In The Organization of Craft Work: Identities, Meanings, and Materiality, 6078. Abingdon, UK: Taylor & Francis Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marks, D. (2011). Competitiveness and the market for Central and Eastern European wines: A cultural good in the global wine market. Journal of Wine Research, 22(3), 245263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masset, P., and Raub, S. (2023). The impact of wine tasters’ expectations on wine quality ratings and willingness-to-pay. Journal of Wine Economics, 18(2), 156172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masset, P., Terrier, L, and Livat, F. (2023). Can a wine be feminine? Gendered wine descriptors and quality, price and aging potential. Journal of Wine Economics, 18(4), forthcoming.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, J. J., Allen, P. G., Stevens, T. H., and Weatherhead, D. (2005). A meta-analysis of hypothetical bias in stated preference valuation. Environmental and Resource Economics, 30, 313325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neale, L., Robbie, R., and Martin, B. (2016). Gender identity and brand incongruence: When in doubt, pursue masculinity. Journal of Strategic Marketing, 24(5), 347359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ody-Brasier, A., and Fernandez-Mateo, I. (2017). When being in the minority pays off: Relationships among sellers and price setting in the champagne industry. American Sociological Review, 82(1), 147178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pavel, S. M. (2012). Women’s entrepreneurship: The rise of women in the global wine industry. Cross-Cultural Management Journal, 14(1), 2128.Google Scholar
Reiser, B., and Shechter, M. (1999). Incorporating zero values in the economic valuation of environmental program benefits. Environmetrics: The Official Journal of the International Environmetrics Society, 10(1), 87101.3.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sachs, C., Barbercheck, M., Braiser, K., Kiernan, N. E., and Terman, A. R. (2016). Constructing a new table: Women farmers negotiate agricultural institutions and organizations, creating new agricultural networks. In Sachs, C. E., Barbercheck, M.E., Brasier, K., Kiernan, N. E., and Terman, A. Rachel (eds.), The Rise of Women Farmers and Sustainable Agriculture, 95139. Iowa City: University of Iowa Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santos, G., Marques, C. S., and Ratten, V. (2019). Entrepreneurial women’s networks: The case of D’Uva—Portugal wine girls. International Journal of Entrepreneurial Behaviour and Research, 25(2), 298322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, A., Nayga, R. M. Jr., Campbell, B. L., and Park, J. L. (2011). Revisiting cheap talk with new evidence from a field experiment. Journal of Agricultural and Resource Economics, 36(2), 280291.Google Scholar
Simonnet-Toussaint, C. (2006). Le vin sur le divan. Des Représentations sociales aux représentations intimes. Bordeaux: Féret.Google Scholar
Simpson, R. (2014). Gender, space and identity: Male cabin crew and service work. Gender in Management: An International Journal, 29(5), 291300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sogari, G., Mora, C., and Menozzi, D. (2016). Factors driving sustainable choice: The case of wine. British Food Journal, 118(3), 632646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tak, E., Correll, S. J., and Soule, S. A. (2019). Gender inequality in product markets: When and how status beliefs transfer to products. Social Forces, 98(2), 548577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thiene, M., Galletto, L., Scarpa, R., and Boatto, V. (2013). Determinants of WTP for Prosecco wine: A latent class regression with attitudinal responses. British Food Journal, 115(2), 279299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Train, K. E. (2009). Discrete Choice Methods with Simulation. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
United Nations (2022). Gender Inequality Index (GII). Available at https://hdr.undp.org/data-center/thematic-composite-indices/gender-inequality-index#/indicies/GII. Accessed August 29 , 2023.Google Scholar
Vecchio, R. (2013). Determinants of willingness-to-pay for sustainable wine: Evidence from experimental auctions. Wine Economics and Policy, 2(2), 8592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velikova, N., Howell, R. D., and Dodd, T. (2015). The development of an objective wine knowledge scale: The item response theory approach. International Journal of Wine Business Research, 27(2), 103124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinberg, D. B., and Kapelner, A. (2018). Comparing gender discrimination and inequality in indie and traditional publishing. PloS One, 13(4), .CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wine Intelligence (2022). Belgium Wine Landscapes. July. London: IWSR Group.Google Scholar
Wine Intelligence (2023). France Wine Landscapes. July. London: IWSR Group.Google Scholar
Wright, T. (2016). Effective interventions for change. In Gender and Sexuality in Male-Dominated Occupations, 221254. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
Wu, L., Klink, R. R., and Guo, J. (2013). Creating gender brand personality with brand names: The effects of phonetic symbolism. Journal of Marketing Theory and Practice, 21(3), 319330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. 2 x 5 between-subject online experiment, WTP (euros), and perceived quality for each wine (N = 1,351)

Figure 1

Table 2. Descriptive characteristics of respondents (N = 1,351)

Figure 2

Table 3. Wine characteristics and opinions of respondents (N = 1,351)

Figure 3

Figure 1. Distribution of WTP (N = 1,351).

Figure 4

Figure 2. Sequence of valuation questions with percentage of respondents.

Figure 5

Table 4. Estimation results

Figure 6

*** Detailed estimation results