Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T20:49:31.491Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why Should We Care About “Fluent Aphasia?” The Importance of Studying Connected Speech in Aphasiology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2006

Carlo Semenza
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Trieste, Italy

Extract

Fluent Aphasia, by Susan Edwards. 2006. New York: Cambridge University Press. 230 pp., $75.00 (HB).

The 21st-century aphasiologist may be surprised to find a new book with such a title as Fluent Aphasia. Three decades of cognitive neuropsychology advances would seem sufficient to relegate a fuzzy concept like “fluent aphasia” as a relic of the past, or, at best, restrict it to the realm of shorthand descriptions meant to facilitate daily communication among clinicians. Even among those accustomed to strict collaboration, a concept like “fluent aphasia” may cause misunderstanding. I personally learned about aphasia directly from the Boston school. Yet, when I speak with my Italian colleagues from Milan, whom I have known for most of my professional life, and with whom I have written quite a few papers on aphasia, I am often unsure about what they mean when they label an aphasic patient as “fluent.” I still remember Harold Goodglass telling me, in almost the same words Susan Edwards quotes from him, how “fluency is best rated in terms of the longest occasional uninterrupted strings of words that are produced.” My own understanding of his teaching included the concept that the presence of phonetic problems and/or abnormal articulation effort was not so essential in drawing the fluency-non fluency distinction: according to his rule of thumb, fluent aphasia should be diagnosed when a patient can produce at least five connected words. This rule derived from observations made in Goodglass, Quadfasel, and Timberlake's (1964) study on phrase length and type of aphasia, whereby it was shown that aphasics can be dichotomized on the basis of the number of words that they can typically utter as an uninterrupted string. Edwards reports that other people entertain different beliefs about what is fluent aphasia. Surely several, heterogeneous criteria such as phrase length, verbal agility, melodic line, amount of articulation effort, and grammatical form are differently weighted by different aphasiologists as they consider their diagnosis.

Type
BOOK REVIEWS
Copyright
© 2007 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Goodglass, H. (1993). Understanding aphasia. San Diego: Academic Press.
Goodglass, H., Quadfasel, F., and Timberlake, W. (1964). Phrase length and the type and severity of aphasia. Cortex, 1, 133153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar