Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:55:11.588Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Supervisory attentional control following carbon monoxide poisoning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2004

KERRY JONES
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Science, Faculty of Science and Technology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia
GLYNDA JANE KINSELLA
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Science, Faculty of Science and Technology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia Department of Psychology, Caulfield General Medical Centre, Caulfield, Victoria, Australia
BEN ONG
Affiliation:
School of Psychological Science, Faculty of Science and Technology, La Trobe University, Bundoora, Victoria, Australia
CARLOS SCHEINKESTEL
Affiliation:
Department of Intensive Care and Hyperbaric Medicine, The Alfred Hospital, Commercial Road, Prahran, Victoria, Australia

Abstract

This study tested the hypothesis that carbon monoxide poisoning would produce a deficit of attentional control, the supervisory attention system, as indexed by attention switching and attentional scheduling, and that routine attentional orienting would be unaffected. Seventy-three cases of carbon monoxide poisoning were assessed at 3 days and 1 month post poisoning on tasks of attentional orienting, and tasks of the supervisory attention system. The results were compared to a group of 53 healthy community participants. A deficit of the supervisory attentional system was documented on a task of attention switching in survivors of both deliberate and accidental CO poisoning, leaving attentional scheduling intact. There was no deficit of attentional orienting in the current study. Alteration of consciousness was found to predict subsequent supervisory attention system impairment in correlation analyses, and the deficit was persistent for a 1 month follow-up period. (JINS, 2004, 10, 843–850.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2004 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Burgess, P.W. & Shallice, T. (1996). Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 34, 263273.Google Scholar
Caine, D. & Watson, F.D.G. (2000). Neuropsychological and neuropathological sequelae of cerebral anoxia: A critical review. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 8699.Google Scholar
Cooper, R. & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 297338.Google Scholar
Dunham, M.D. & Johnstone, B. (1999). Variability of neuropsychological deficits associated with carbon monoxide poisoning: Four case reports. Brain Injury, 13, 917925.Google Scholar
Durvasula, R.S., Myers, J.F., Satz, P., Miller, E.N., Morgenstern, H., Richardson, M.A., Evans, G., & Forney, D. (2000). HIV-1, cocaine, and neuropsychological performance in African American men. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 6, 322335.Google Scholar
Gale, S.D. & Hopkins, R.O. (2004). Effects of hypoxia on the brain: Neuroimaging and neuropsychological findings following carbon monoxide poisoning and obstructive sleep apnea. Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society, 10, 6071.Google Scholar
Gale, S.D., Hopkins, R.O., Weaver, L.K., Bigler, E.D., Booth, E.J., & Blatter, D.D. (1999). MRI, Quantitative MRI, SPECT, and neuropsychological findings following carbon monoxide poisoning. Brain Injury, 13, 229243.Google Scholar
Gauntlett-Gilbert, J., Roberts, R.C., & Brown, V.J. (1999). Mechanisms underlying attentional set-shifting in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia, 37, 605616.Google Scholar
Haldane, J.B.S. (1927). Carbon monoxide as a tissue poison. Journal of Biochemistry, 21, 10681075.Google Scholar
Jones, J.S., Lagasse, J., & Zimmerman, G. (1994). Computed topographic findings after acute carbon monoxide poisoning. American Journal of Emergency Medicine, 12, 448451.Google Scholar
Kesler, S.R., Hopkins, R.O., Weaver, L.K., Blatter, D.D., Edge-Booth, H., & Bigler, E.D. (2001). Verbal memory deficits associated with fornix atrophy in carbon monoxide poisoning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 640646.Google Scholar
Lacritz, L.H., Cullum, M., Frol, A.B., Dewey, R.B., & Giller, C.A. (2000). Neuropsychological outcome following unilateral stereotactic pallidotomy in intractable Parkinson's disease. Brain and Cognition, 42, 364378.Google Scholar
La Plane, D., Baulac, M., Widlocher, D., & Dubois, B. (1984). Pure psychic akinesia with bilateral lesions of basal ganglia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 47, 377385.Google Scholar
Lishman, W.A. (1998). Organic psychiatry: The psychological consequences of cerebral disorder. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Llorente, A.M., Miller, E.N., D'Elia, L.F., Selnes, O.A., Wesch, J., Becker, J.T., & Satz, P. (1998). Slowed information processing in HIV-1 disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 20, 6072.Google Scholar
McDonald, C., Brown, G.G., & Gorrel, J.M. (1996). Impaired set-shifting in Parkinson's disease: New evidence from a lexical decision task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 18, 793809.Google Scholar
McNair, D.M., Lorr, M., & Droppleman, L.F. (1981). Profile of mood states. San Diego, CA: Educational and Industrial Testing Service.
Mark, P. (1992). Carbon monoxide poisoning: A review. South Pacific Undersea Medical Society Journal, 22, 2734.Google Scholar
Messier, L.D. & Myers, R.A.M. (1991). A neuropsychological screening battery for emergency assessment of carbon monoxide poisoned patients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 675654.Google Scholar
Miller, E.N. (1996). The Californian Computerized Assessment Package (CALCAP). Los Angeles, CA: Author.
Miller, E.N., Satz, P., & Visscher, P.H. (1991). Computerized and conventional neuropsychological assessment of HIV-1 infected homosexual men. Neurology, 41, 16081616.Google Scholar
Miura, T., Mitomo, M., Kawai, R., & Harada, K. (1985). CT of the brain in acute carbon monoxide intoxication: Characteristic features and prognosis. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 6, 739742.Google Scholar
Murata, S., Narabayashi, I., Asaba, H., Naritomi, H., Hiraishi, K., & Sakai, T. (1993). Magnetic resonance imaging findings on carbon monoxide intoxication. Journal of Neuroimaging, 3, 128131.Google Scholar
Nelson, H.E. & Willison, J.R. (1991). The National Adult Reading Test (NART) manual. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.
Owen, A.M., Doyon, J., Dagher, A., Sadikot, A., & Evans A.C. (1998). Abnormal basal ganglia outflow in Parkinson's disease identified with PET: Implications for higher cortical functions. Brain, 121, 949965.Google Scholar
Owen, A.D., Roberts, A.C., Hodges, J.R., Summers, B.A., Polkey, C.E., & Robbins T.W. (1993). Contrasting mechanisms of impaired attentional set-shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage or Parkinson's disease. Brain, 116, 11591175.Google Scholar
Parkinson, R.B., Hopkins, R.O., Cleavinger, B.S., Weaver, L.K., Victoroff, J., Foley, J.F., & Bigler, E.D. (2002). White matter hyperintensities and neuropsychological outcome following carbon monoxide poisoning. Neurology, 58, 15251532.Google Scholar
Plotnik, M., Flash, T., Inzelberg, R., Schechtman, E., & Korozyn, A.D. (1998). Motor switching abilities in Parkinson's disease and old age: Temporal aspects. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 65, 328337.Google Scholar
Raskin, S.A., Borod, J.C., & Tweedy, J.R. (1992). Set-shifting and spatial orientation in patients with Parkinson's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14, 801821.Google Scholar
Seger, D. & Welch, L. (1994). Carbon monoxide controversies: Neuropsychologic testing, mechanism of toxicity, and hyperbaric oxygen. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 24, 242248.Google Scholar
Serra-Mestres, J. & Ring, H.W. (1999). Vulnerability to emotionally negative stimuli in Parkinson's disease: An investigation using the Emotional Stroop Task. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology and Behavioural Neurology, 12, 5257.Google Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Silver, D.A.T., Cross, M., Fox, B., & Paxton, R.M. (1996). Computed tomography of the brain in acute carbon monoxide poisoning. Clinical Radiology, 51, 480483.Google Scholar
Starkstein, S.E., Berthier, M.L., & Leiguarda, R. (1989). Psychic akinesia following bilateral pallidal lesions. International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 19, 155164.Google Scholar
Taylor, R. & Holgate, R. (1987). Carbon monoxide poisoning: Asymmetric and unilateral changes on CT. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 9, 975977.Google Scholar
Tom, T., Clark, R.I., Abedon, S., & Wong, W. (1996). Neuroimaging characteristics in carbon monoxide toxicity. Journal of Neuroimaging, 6, 161166.Google Scholar
Vieregge, P., Klostermann, W., Blumm, R.G., & Borgis, K.J. (1989). Carbon monoxide poisoning: Clinical, neurophysiological and brain imaging observations in acute disease and follow-up. Journal of Neurology, 236, 478481.Google Scholar