Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T18:16:43.641Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Subthalamic stimulation affects homophone meaning generation in Parkinson's disease

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 September 2008

JOANNA E. CASTNER
Affiliation:
Centre for Research into Language Processing and Linguistics, Division of Speech Pathology, School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
DAVID A. COPLAND
Affiliation:
Centre for Research into Language Processing and Linguistics, Division of Speech Pathology, School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
PETER A. SILBURN
Affiliation:
St. Andrew's War Memorial Hospital, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia The Wesley Hospital, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia School of Medicine, The University of Queensland, Herston, Queensland, Australia
TERRY J. COYNE
Affiliation:
St. Andrew's War Memorial Hospital, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia The Wesley Hospital, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
FELICITY SINCLAIR
Affiliation:
St. Andrew's War Memorial Hospital, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia The Wesley Hospital, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
HELEN J. CHENERY*
Affiliation:
Centre for Research into Language Processing and Linguistics, Division of Speech Pathology, School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia
*
Correspondence and reprint requests to: Helen Chenery, Division of Speech Pathology, School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, The University of Queensland, Brisbane, QLD 4072Australia. E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Deep brain stimulation (DBS) of the subthalamic nucleus (STN) in individuals with Parkinson's disease (PD) has often been associated with reduced verbal fluency performance. This study aimed to directly assess semantic switching as a function of STN stimulation in PD participants with the Homophone Meaning Generation Test (HMGT). Seventeen participants with PD who had received STN DBS completed the HMGT in on and off stimulation conditions. Twenty-one non-neurologically impaired participants acted as controls. PD participants (in both on and off stimulation conditions) generated significantly fewer meanings than control participants and consistent with the previous reports of verbal fluency impairment, PD participants produced fewer definitions in the on stimulation condition. PD participants (in both on and off stimulation conditions) also had greater difficulty generating definitions for nonhomographic homophones compared with homographic homographs. The results of this study indicate that STN stimulation exacerbates impairment in semantic switching. (JINS, 2008, 14, 890–894.)

Type
Brief Communication
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2008

INTRODUCTION

Over the past decade, deep brain stimulation (DBS) of the subthalamic nucleus (STN) has become a widely accepted treatment for medically intractable Parkinson's disease (PD). Numerous studies have now associated STN DBS with significant improvements in the motor complications of PD (see Kleiner-Fisman et al., Reference Kleiner-Fisman, Herzog, Fisman, Tamma, Lyons, Pahwa, Lang and Deuschl2006, for a review), yet changes in neuropsychological status as a function of STN stimulation are not as clearly defined. Whereas STN DBS has generally been considered as a cognitively safe procedure, declines in verbal fluency have frequently been reported (e.g., Cilia et al., Reference Cilia, Siri, Marotta, De Gaspari, Landi, Mariani, Benti, Isaias, Vergani, Pezzoli and Anotonini2007; De Gaspari et al., Reference De Gaspari, Siri, Di Gioia, Antonini, Isella, Pizzolato, Landi, Vergani, Gaini, Appollonio and Pezzoli2006; Saint-Cyr et al., Reference Saint-Cyr, Trepanier, Kumar, Lozano and Lang2000; Schroeder et al., Reference Schroeder, Kuehler, Lange, Haslinger, Tronnier, Krause, Pfister, Boecker and Ceballos-Baumann2003; Smeding et al., Reference Smeding, Speelman, Koning-Haanstra, Schuurman, Nijssen, van Laar and Schmand2006).

Saint-Cyr and colleagues (Reference Saint-Cyr, Trepanier, Kumar, Lozano and Lang2000) in a comprehensive study of the neuropsychological consequences of STN DBS, compared preoperative semantic and phonemic verbal fluency scores, including measures of switching (number of switches from one semantic subcategory to another) and clustering (mean number of words per semantic subcategory) with postoperative scores in 10 patients who received STN DBS. Verbal fluency performance declined postoperatively for the majority of patients and the subcomponent process of switching similarly declined, suggesting that STN DBS surgery is associated with greater difficulties switching from one semantic subcategory to another. This pattern of verbal fluency decline has since been replicated in a study including a larger sample size (De Gaspari et al., Reference De Gaspari, Siri, Di Gioia, Antonini, Isella, Pizzolato, Landi, Vergani, Gaini, Appollonio and Pezzoli2006).

As an alternative test of verbal generation, Warrington (Reference Warrington2000) proposed the use of a Homophone Meaning Generation Test (HMGT) which requires the generation of multiple definitions to words that have many meanings (e.g., slip). The HMGT, therefore, assesses the ability to switch between one independent semantic representation and another, linked at a lexical level by a common word form. In contrast to verbal fluency paradigms, the HMGT is an un-timed measure. As suggested by Warrington (Reference Warrington2000), the use of an un-timed measure is advantageous when applied to a population with speech production difficulties and in the case of the present study this allows for the consideration of the presence of dysarthria or bradykinesia associated with PD or alterations in motor speech impairment as a result of DBS. Therefore, more accurate comparisons can be made between PD and control performance and also between on and off stimulation assessment if the test used is less dependent on measures of vocal (or speech) performance or speed of response. Furthermore, the HMGT is advantageous over verbal fluency measures as it directly targets semantic switching capacity rather than the many subcomponent measures (such as response initiation, strategy formation, and attention) that may influence outcomes in verbal fluency performance. The HMGT is also reported to be highly sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction (Warrington, Reference Warrington2000) and highly correlated with phonemic and semantic verbal fluency tests (Kave et al., Reference Kave, Avraham, Kukulansky-Segal and Herzberg2007).

Whelan and colleagues (Reference Whelan, Murdoch, Theodoros, Hall and Silburn2003) first assessed the ability to generate definitions from homophones in PD as a function of STN DBS surgery. The results were not consistent with verbal fluency declines reported elsewhere, as four of the five participants improved in their ability to provide definitions to homophones when receiving STN stimulation compared with preoperative results. However, the results of this study should be interpreted with caution considering the small sample size and potential shortcomings of the assessment.

The purpose of the present study was to investigate homophone meaning generation in a larger cohort of PD participants who had received surgery for bilateral subthalamic DBS. Homophone meaning generation skills were also measured in on- and off-stimulation conditions, providing a direct measure of the effects of STN stimulation on semantic switching unconfounded by the effects of DBS surgery.

METHODS

Participants

Seventeen participants (13 males) diagnosed with PD were assessed at least 4 months after receiving bilateral STN DBS surgery (refer to Coyne et al., Reference Coyne, Silburn, Cook, Silberstein, Mellick, Sinclair, Fracchia, Wasson and Stanwell2006, for details of the surgical procedure). All PD participants had undergone extensive neurological and psychiatric evaluation before surgery for DBS. They met the strict inclusion criteria for admission to the DBS program, which included no evidence of significant psychiatric symptoms. Each participant was tested with their stimulators turned on and again with their stimulators turned off. The order of test condition was counterbalanced with at least 6 weeks separating the two testing sessions. The stimulators were turned off for at least 1 hr before the commencement of assessment in the off stimulation condition. Four participants were no longer taking levodopa medication and the remaining 13 participants were tested while taking their usual medication and levodopa dosage remained constant for each testing session. Demographic and disease characteristics of the PD participants are presented in Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic and disease characteristics of Parkinson's disease participants and controls

Note

DBS = Deep Brain Stimulation; UPDRS = Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale; H & Y = Hoehn and Yahr.

Twenty-one normal control participants (13 males) were matched for age and education to the PD participants (Table 1). All control participants were right handed, had no history of neurological impairment, were not taking any medication deemed to affect neurological functioning, had no self-reported hearing loss, and had self-reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision. All participants provided informed consent for participation, which was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki, and this project was approved by the appropriate University and Hospital Ethics Committees.

Procedure

PD and control participants completed the HMGT (Warrington, Reference Warrington2000) which consists of a total of eight homophones (tick, tip, slip, form, plain, bored, right, and sent). Four of the eight homophones were also homographic (e.g., slip) and the remaining four had more than one possible spelling (e.g., right/write/rite). Each word was presented auditorily, and participants were required to generate as many definitions as possible. Responses to each homophone were un-timed and participants were required to indicate when they had exhausted all possible meanings for each item.

Homophone definitions were considered correct if they were representative of any definition considered appropriate in Australian English according to the 2001 Macquarie Dictionary. Each correct definition was awarded one point and summed to create a total for same spelling and different spelling homophones.

RESULTS

Mean and standard deviations for same and different spelling homophones for PD participants and controls are presented in Table 2. In addition, the distribution of individual PD participant total scores is presented in Figure 1.

Table 2. Mean (SD) scores on the Homophone Meaning Generation Test for PD participants and controls

Fig. 1. Distribution of total Homophone Meaning Generation Test scores for patients with Parkinson's disease in on and off stimulation conditions. * indicates that the participant was no longer taking levodopa medication.

An analysis of variance (ANOVA) was first conducted on homophone meaning generation scores with session order, session type, and homophone type as factors, to determine the presence of assessment order effects for the PD participants. Order effects did not reach significance [F(1,60) = 7.767; p = .263] and there were no significant interactions between order effects and other factors of interest (p > .5); therefore, assessment order effects were not included in further analyses.

To determine the effects of STN stimulation on homophone meaning generation for PD participants, a repeated measures 2 × 2 ANOVA was performed on homophone meaning generation scores as a function of homophone type (same vs. different spelling) and stimulation condition (on vs. off stimulation). A significant main effect for stimulation revealed that STN stimulation was associated with an overall decline in the number of definitions provided compared with the off stimulation condition irrespective of homophone type [F(1,16) = 5.633; p < .05]. There was also a main effect for homophone type [F(1,16) = 12.712; p < .005], suggesting the presence of a greater number of definitions generated for same spelling homophones (M = 12.706; SD = 2.714) compared with different spelling homophones (M = 11.265; SD = 2.274). An interaction between homophone type and stimulation condition did not reach significance [F(1,16) = .983; p > .05]. For control participants, a paired samples t test revealed no significant difference between the number of meanings generated for same or different spelling homophones [t(20) = 1.105; p = .282].

DISCUSSION

The results of the present study reveal a general deficit in the generation of meanings of homophonic words by PD participants in both on- and off-stimulation conditions, thereby suggesting that homophone meaning generation is similarly affected by frontal–subcortical dysfunction associated with PD. Furthermore, STN stimulation resulted in a further decline on the HMGT, suggesting that STN stimulation acts to further impair semantic switching in PD.

Impaired performance on the HMGT is consistent with the numerous studies that have linked STN stimulation with impaired performance on verbal fluency tests (e.g., Cilia et al., Reference Cilia, Siri, Marotta, De Gaspari, Landi, Mariani, Benti, Isaias, Vergani, Pezzoli and Anotonini2007; De Gaspari et al., Reference De Gaspari, Siri, Di Gioia, Antonini, Isella, Pizzolato, Landi, Vergani, Gaini, Appollonio and Pezzoli2006; Saint-Cyr et al., Reference Saint-Cyr, Trepanier, Kumar, Lozano and Lang2000; Schroeder et al., Reference Schroeder, Kuehler, Lange, Haslinger, Tronnier, Krause, Pfister, Boecker and Ceballos-Baumann2003; Smeding et al., Reference Smeding, Speelman, Koning-Haanstra, Schuurman, Nijssen, van Laar and Schmand2006). While performance on both semantic and phonemic fluency tests is related to performance on the HMGT (Kave et al., Reference Kave, Avraham, Kukulansky-Segal and Herzberg2007), there are some distinct differences between them. The HMGT requires the generation of a series of independent semantic meanings from a homophone, whereas category fluency requires switching within a single semantic domain. Therefore, HMGT is likely to require a greater capacity to switch between semantic domains than category fluency. Whereas phonemic fluency potentially engages cognitive switching to a similar extent to the HMGT, reliance on lexical knowledge is not as high (Kave et al., Reference Kave, Avraham, Kukulansky-Segal and Herzberg2007).

Behaviorally, verbal fluency deficits associated with STN stimulation have been hypothesized to be influenced by motor speech impairment subsequent to STN stimulation, impairment in lexical retrieval, or cognitive switching from one semantic concept to another (Warrington, Reference Warrington2000). The use of the HMGT in the present study substantially eliminates the influence of speech production demands or bradykinesia. The HMGT stresses semantic switching skills but performance might also be affected by the lexical retrieval demands of providing definitions. Category fluency tasks require switching semantic subdomains within a larger semantic domain according to strict semantic criteria, phonemic fluency tasks require switching between semantic domains on the basis of lexical search (Troyer et al., Reference Troyer, Moscovitch, Winocur, Alexander and Stuss1998); and both tasks require an ability to suppress habitual responses (Perrett, Reference Perrett1974; Ross et al., Reference Ross, Calhoun, Cox, Wenner, Kono and Pleasant2007). STN stimulation has previously been reported to improve the ability to inhibit dominant semantic meanings (as measured by the Hayling Test) (Castner et al., Reference Castner, Copland, Silburn, Coyne, Sinclair and Chenery2007). Therefore, it is unlikely that the impairment in semantic switching associated with STN stimulation is related to difficulties in inhibiting habitual responses.

Why DBS facilitates movement but impairs semantic switching is unknown. STN stimulation might impair semantic switching by at least two mechanisms: the spread of current to adjacent neural regions (Perriol et al., Reference Perriol, Krystkowiak, Defebvre, Blond, Destée and Dujardin2006), and the spread of current within the STN itself to directly affect nonmotor basal ganglia–thalamocortical circuits (Perriol et al., Reference Perriol, Krystkowiak, Defebvre, Blond, Destée and Dujardin2006; Schroeder et al., Reference Schroeder, Kuehler, Lange, Haslinger, Tronnier, Krause, Pfister, Boecker and Ceballos-Baumann2003). It is also possible that basal ganglia–thalamocortical circuits are not as segregated as originally believed (Parsons et al., Reference Parsons, Rogers, Braaten, Woods and Troster2006).

An unexpected result of the present study was a relatively greater deficit in ability to generate definitions for homophones that could be spelled multiple ways. This deficit was seen in PD participants only, and in both on- and off-stimulation conditions. This finding contrasts with the results of Warrington's study (Reference Warrington2000) of subjects with focal frontal pathology, in whom there was no evidence of a homophone type effect. It also contrasts with the results of the study by Kave et al. (Reference Kave, Avraham, Kukulansky-Segal and Herzberg2007) in which normal subjects achieved higher scores for Hebrew nonhomographic homophones. These disparate findings may reflect statistical limitations of the studies, the effect of different pathologies, or language-specific differences.

The greater difficulty with differently spelled homophones exhibited by PD subjects may reflect the larger cognitive switches required in generating definitions for this type of homophone. For example, performing well on different spelling homophones (such as scent/sent/cent) requires a shift from one orthographic lexical representation to another in addition to a semantic switch to enable the generation of the various meanings across and within lexical representations.

In conclusion, our study suggests that subjects with PD have impaired lexical–semantic switching ability relative to age- and education-matched control subjects, reflected in their impaired performance on the HMGT. This impairment is increased with STN DBS, and it is greatest with homophone definition involving differently spelled homophones. These results extend and clarify the results of prior studies using phonemic and semantic fluency tests because the use of the HMGT substantially eliminates confounds attributable to the potentially deleterious effects of bradykinesia and dysarthria on word fluency.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank St. Andrew's War Memorial Hospital and The Wesley Hospital for their support of this research, as well as Erin Smith and Kim Parton for their assistance with data collection and technical support. This research was supported by a University of Queensland Research Development Grant. Joanna Castner is a recipient of a Smart State PhD Scholarship awarded by the Queensland Government Department of State Development, Trade, and Innovation. David Copland is supported by a NHMRC Career Development Award.

References

REFERENCES

Castner, J.E., Copland, D.A., Silburn, P.A., Coyne, T.J., Sinclair, F., & Chenery, H.J. (2007). Lexical-semantic inhibitory mechanisms in Parkinson's disease as a function of subthalamic stimulation. Neuropsychologia, 45, 31673177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cilia, R., Siri, C., Marotta, G., De Gaspari, D., Landi, A., Mariani, C.B., Benti, R., Isaias, I.U., Vergani, F., Pezzoli, G., & Anotonini, A. (2007). Brain networks underlining verbal fluency decline during STN-DBS in Parkinson's disease: An ECD-SPECT study. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 13, 290294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coyne, T., Silburn, P., Cook, R., Silberstein, P., Mellick, G., Sinclair, F., Fracchia, G., Wasson, D., & Stanwell, P. (2006). Rapid subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation lead placement utilising CT/MRI fusion, microelectrode recording and test stimulation. Acta Neurochirurgica Supplementum, 99, 4950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Gaspari, D., Siri, C., Di Gioia, M., Antonini, A., Isella, V., Pizzolato, A., Landi, A., Vergani, F., Gaini, S.M., Appollonio, I.M., & Pezzoli, G. (2006). Clinical correlates and cognitive underpinnings of verbal fluency impairment after chronic subthalamic stimulation in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 12, 289295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kave, G., Avraham, A., Kukulansky-Segal, D., & Herzberg, O. (2007). How does the homophone meaning generation test associate with the phonemic and semantic fluency test? A quantitative and qualitative analysis. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 424432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleiner-Fisman, G., Herzog, J., Fisman, D.N., Tamma, F., Lyons, K.E., Pahwa, R., Lang, A.E., & Deuschl, G. (2006). Subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation: Summary and meta-analysis of outcomes. Movement Disorders, 21(Suppl. 14), S290S304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parsons, T.D., Rogers, S.A., Braaten, A.J., Woods, S.P., & Troster, A.I. (2006). Cognitive sequelae of subthalamic nucleus deep brain stimulation in Parkinson's disease: A meta-analysis. Lancet Neurology, 5, 578588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perrett, E. (1974). The left frontal lobe in man and the suppression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour. Neuropsychologia, 12, 342345.Google Scholar
Perriol, M.P., Krystkowiak, P., Defebvre, L., Blond, S., Destée, A., & Dujardin, K. (2006). Stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in Parkinson's disease: Cognitive and affective changes are not linked to the motor outcome. Parkinsonism and Related Disorders, 12, 205210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, T.P., Calhoun, E., Cox, T., Wenner, C., Kono, W., & Pleasant, M. (2007). The reliability and validity of qualitative scores for the Controlled Oral Word Association Test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 475488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saint-Cyr, J.A., Trepanier, L.L., Kumar, R., Lozano, A.M., & Lang, A.E. (2000). Neuropsychological consequences of chronic bilateral stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus in Parkinson's disease. Brain, 123, 20912108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schroeder, U., Kuehler, A., Lange, K.W., Haslinger, B., Tronnier, V.M., Krause, M., Pfister, R., Boecker, H., & Ceballos-Baumann, A.O. (2003). Subthalamic nucleus stimulation affects a frontotemporal network: A PET study. Annals of Neurology, 54, 445450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smeding, H.M., Speelman, J.D., Koning-Haanstra, M., Schuurman, P.R., Nijssen, P., van Laar, T., & Schmand, B. (2006). Neuropsychological effects of bilateral STN stimulation in Parkinson disease: A controlled study. Neurology, 66, 18301836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Troyer, A.K., Moscovitch, M., Winocur, G., Alexander, M.P., & Stuss, D. (1998). Clustering and switching on verbal fluency: The effects of focal frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 36, 499504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E.K. (2000). Homophone meaning generation: A new test of verbal switching for the detection of frontal lobe dysfunction. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 643648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whelan, B.M., Murdoch, B.E., Theodoros, D.G., Hall, B., & Silburn, P. (2003). Defining a role for the subthalamic nucleus within operative theoretical models of subcortical participation in language. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 74, 15431550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic and disease characteristics of Parkinson's disease participants and controls

Figure 1

Table 2. Mean (SD) scores on the Homophone Meaning Generation Test for PD participants and controls

Figure 2

Fig. 1. Distribution of total Homophone Meaning Generation Test scores for patients with Parkinson's disease in on and off stimulation conditions. * indicates that the participant was no longer taking levodopa medication.