Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T00:00:59.641Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

New Intrusion Analyses on the CVLT-3: Utility in Distinguishing the Memory Disorders of Alzheimer’s versus Huntington’s Disease

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 May 2019

Lisa V. Graves
Affiliation:
San Diego State University/University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, California, USA Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, California, USA
Heather M. Holden
Affiliation:
San Diego State University/University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, California, USA Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, California, USA
Emily J. Van Etten
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, USA
Lisa Delano-Wood
Affiliation:
San Diego State University/University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, California, USA Veterans Affairs Healthcare System, San Diego, California, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, California, USA
Mark W. Bondi
Affiliation:
San Diego State University/University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, California, USA Veterans Affairs Healthcare System, San Diego, California, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, California, USA
David P. Salmon
Affiliation:
San Diego State University/University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, California, USA Department of Neurosciences, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, USA
Jody Corey-Bloom
Affiliation:
San Diego State University/University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, California, USA Department of Neurosciences, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, California, USA
Paul E. Gilbert*
Affiliation:
San Diego State University/University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, California, USA Department of Psychology, San Diego State University, San Diego, California, USA
Dean C. Delis
Affiliation:
San Diego State University/University of California San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, San Diego, California, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of California San Diego School of Medicine, La Jolla, California, USA
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Dr. Paul E. Gilbert, SDSU/UC San Diego Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology, 6363 Alvarado Court, Suite 103, San Diego, CA, 92120. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Objectives: Research has shown that analyzing intrusion errors generated on verbal learning and memory measures is helpful for distinguishing between the memory disorders associated with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and other neurological disorders, including Huntington’s disease (HD). Moreover, preliminary evidence suggests that certain clinical populations may be prone to exhibit different types of intrusion errors. Methods: We examined the prevalence of two new California Verbal Learning Test-3 (CVLT-3) intrusion subtypes – across-trial novel intrusions and across/within trial repeated intrusions – in individuals with AD or HD. We hypothesized that the encoding/storage impairment associated with medial-temporal involvement in AD would result in a greater number of novel intrusions on the delayed recall trials of the CVLT-3, whereas the executive dysfunction associated with subcortical-frontal involvement in HD would result in a greater number of repeated intrusions across trials. Results: The AD group generated significantly more across-trial novel intrusions than across/within trial repeated intrusions on the delayed cued-recall trials, whereas the HD group showed the opposite pattern on the delayed free-recall trials. Conclusions: These new intrusion subtypes, combined with traditional memory analyses (e.g., recall versus recognition performance), promise to enhance our ability to distinguish between the memory disorders associated with primarily medial-temporal versus subcortical-frontal involvement.

Type
Brief Communication
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2019. 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Baldo, J.V., Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., & Shimamura, A.P. (2002). Memory performance on the California Verbal Learning Test-II: Findings from patients with focal frontal lesions. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 539546. doi: 10.1017.S1355617701020288 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bondi, M.W., Salmon, D.P., Galasko, D., Thomas, R.G., & Thal, L.J. (1999). Neuropsychological function and Apolipoprotein E genotype in the preclinical detection of Alzheimer’s disease. Psychology and Aging, 14(2), 295303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, P.A., Yu, L., Wilson, R.S., Leurgans, S.E., Schneider, J.A., & Bennett, D.A. (2018). Person-specific contribution of neuropathologies to cognitive loss in old age. Annals of Neurology, 83, 7483. doi: 10.1002/ana.25123 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brenowitz, W.D., Keene, C.D., Hawes, S.E., Hubbard, R.A., Longstreth, W.T. Jr., Woltjer, R.L., Crane, P.K., Larson, E.B., & Kukull, W.A. (2017). Alzheimer’s disease neuropathologic change, Lewy body disease, and vascular brain injury in clinic- and community-based samples. Neurobiology of Aging, 53, 8392. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.01.017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butters, N., Granholm, E., Salmon, D.P., Grant, I., & Wolfe, J. (1987). Episodic and semantic memory: A comparison of amnesic and demented patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 9(5), 479497. doi: 10.1080/01688638708410764 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, K.L., Price, C.C., Kaplan, E., & Libon, D.J. (2002). Error analysis of the nine-word California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-9) among older adults with and without dementia. Clinical Neuropsychology, 16(1), 8189. doi: 10.1076/clin.16.1.81.8330 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (1987). California Verbal Learning Test. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Massman, P.J., Butters, N., Salmon, D.P., Cermak, L.S., & Kramer, J.H. (1991). Profiles of demented and amnesic patients on the California Verbal Learning Test: Implications for the assessment of memory disorders. Psychological Assessment: A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 3(1), 1926. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.3.1.19 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test–II (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Delis, D.C., Jacobson, M., Bondi, M.W., Hamilton, J.M., & Salmon, D.P. (2003). The myth of testing construct validity using factor analysis or correlations with normal or mixed clinical populations: Lessons from memory assessment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9, 936946. doi: 10.1017/S1355617703960139 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D.C., Wetter, S.R., Jacobson, M.W., Peavy, G., Hamilton, J., Gongvatana, A., Kramer, J.H., Bondi, M.W., Corey-Bloom, J., & Salmon, D.P. (2005). Recall discriminability: Utility of a new CVLT-II measure in the differential diagnosis of dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 11, 708715. doi: 10.1017/S1355617705050812 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D.C., Kramer, J.H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B.A. (2017). California Verbal Learning Test–3, (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Hamilton, J.M., Salmon, D.P., Galasko, D., Delis, D.C., Hansen, L.A., Masliah, E., Thomas, R.G., & Thal, L.J. (2004). A comparison of episodic memory deficits in neuropathologically-confirmed dementia with Lewy bodies and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 689697. doi: 10.1017/S1355617704105043 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helkala, E.L., Laulumaa, V., Soininen, H., & Riekkinen, P.J. (1989). Different error pattern of episodic and semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease with dementia. Neuropsychologia, 27(10), 12411248. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(89)90036-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huntington Study Group. (1996). Unified Huntington’s disease rating scale: Reliability and consistency. Movement Disorders, 11, 136142. doi: 10.1002/mds.870110204 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, J.H., Delis, D.C., Blusewicz, M.J., Brandt, J., Ober, B., & Strauss, M. (1988). Verbal memory errors in Alzheimer’s and Huntington’s dementias. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4(1), 115. doi: 10.1080/87565648809540385 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, J.H., Levin, B.E., Brandt, J., & Delis, D.C. (1989). Differentiation of Alzheimer’s, Huntington’s, and Parkinson’s disease patients on the basis of verbal learning characteristics. Neuropsychology, 3(2), 111120. doi: 10.1037/h0091766 Google Scholar
Lafosse, J.M., Reed, B.R., Mungas, D., Sterling, S.B., Wahbeh, H., & Jagust, W.J. (1997). Fluency and memory differences between ischemic vascular dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology, 11(4), 514522. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.11.4.514 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libon, D.J., Bogdanoff, B., Bonavita, J., Skalina, S., Cloud, B.S., Resh, R., Cass, P., & Ball, S.K. (1997). Dementia associated with periventricular and deep white matter alterations: A subtype of subcortical dementia. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 12(3), 239250. doi: 10.1016/S0887-6177(96)00041-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libon, D.J., Bondi, M.W., Price, C.C., Lamar, M., Eppig, J., Wambach, D.M., Nieves, C., Delano-Wood, L., Giovannetti, T., Lippa, C., Kabasakalian, A., Cosentino, S., Swenson, R., & Penney, D.L. (2011). Verbal serial list learning in mild cognitive impairment: A profile analysis of interference, forgetting, and errors. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17(5), 905914. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000944 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Massman, P.J., Delis, D.C., Butters, N., Dupont, R.M., & Gillin, J.C. (1992). The subcortical dysfunction hypothesis of memory deficits in depression: Neuropsychological validation in a subgroup of patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 14(5), 687706. doi: 10.1080/01688639208402856 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia Rating Scale. Professional manual. Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., & Stadlan, M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCD–ADRDA work group. Neurology, 34, 939944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKhann, G.M., Knopman, D.S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B.T., Jack, C.R. Jr., Kawas, C.H., Klunk, W. E., Koroshetz, W.G., Manly, J.J., Mayeux, R., Mohs, R.C., Morris, J.C., Rossor, M. N., Scheltens, P., Carrillo, M.C., Thies, B., Weintraub, S., & Phelps, C.H. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroup. Alzheimer’s & Dementia, 7, 263269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, S.B. & DeShon, R.P. (2002). Combining effect size estimates in meta-analysis with repeated measures and independent-groups designs. Psychological Methods, 7, 105125. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.105 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouleau, I., Imbault, H., Lamframboise, M., & Bédard, M.A. (2001). Pattern of intrusions in verbal recall: Comparison of Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, and frontal lobe dementia. Brain and Cognition, 46(1–2), 244249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, K.R., Edmonds, E.C., Eppig, J., Salmon, D.P., & Bondi, M.W. (2018a). Using neuropsychological process scores to identify subtle cognitive decline and predict progression to mild cognitive impairment. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 64(1), 195204. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180229 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, K.R., Eppig, J., Edmonds, E.C., Jacobs, D.M., Libon, D.J., Au, R., Salmon, D.P., & Bondi, M.W. (2018b). Word-list intrusion errors predict progression to mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology, 32(2), 235245. doi: 10.1037/neu0000413 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed