Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T14:42:31.118Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Leisure activity engagement across adulthood predicts cognitive change after five years: Do gender and age matter?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 November 2022

Sharon Sanz Simon
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA The Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA The Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Seonjoo Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biostatistics, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Yian Gu
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA The Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA The Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA The Department of Epidemiology, Joseph P. Mailman School of Public Health, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Ashley Mensing
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Diala Noofoory
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Geneva M. Hidalgo Nazario
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Reshma S. Babukutty
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
Yaakov Stern*
Affiliation:
Cognitive Neuroscience Division, Department of Neurology, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA The Taub Institute for Research in Alzheimer’s Disease and the Aging Brain, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA The Gertrude H. Sergievsky Center, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA
*
Corresponding author: Yaakov Stern, email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Objective:

To examine the association between leisure activity (LA) frequency and cognitive trajectories over 5 years across adulthood, and whether gender and age moderate these associations.

Method:

A total of 234 cognitively healthy adults (21–80 years) completed a LA questionnaire at baseline and neuropsychological measures at baseline and after 5 years. Latent change score analysis was applied to generate latent variables estimating changes in different cognitive domains. For a secondary analysis, LA components’ scores were calculated, reflecting cognitive-intellectual, social, and physical activities. Regression analysis examined the association between baseline LA and cognitive change, and potential moderation of gender and age. In addition, we tested the influence of cortical gray matter thickness on the results.

Results:

We found that higher LA engagement was associated with slower cognitive decline for reasoning, speed, and memory, as well as better vocabulary across two time points. Regarding LA components, higher Social-LA and Intellectual-LA predicted slower rates of cognitive decline across different domains, while Physical-LA was not associated with cognitive change. Gender, but not age, moderated some of the associations observed. Our results remained the same after controlling for cortical gray matter thickness.

Conclusions:

We demonstrated a protective effect of LA engagement on cognitive trajectories over 5 years, independent from demographics and a measure of brain health. The effects were in part moderated by gender, but not age. Results should be replicated in larger and more diverse samples. Our findings support cognitive reserve hypothesis and have implications for future reserve-enhancing interventions.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © INS. Published by Cambridge University Press, 2022

Introduction

Leisure activity (LA) can be defined as activity individuals engage in during free time (Sala et al., Reference Sala, Jopp, Gobet, Ogawa, Ishioka, Masui, Inagaki, Nakagawa, Yasumoto, Ishizaki, Arai, Ikebe, Kamide and Gondo2019), for enjoyment or well-being, independent from work or activities of daily living (Verghese et al., Reference Verghese, LeValley, Derby, Kuslansky, Katz, Hall, Buschke and Lipton2006). It has been suggested that engaging in LA may protect cognitive health in aging. A theoretical account for this association is the cognitive reserve hypothesis (Stern et al., Reference Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartres-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat, Ewers, Franzmeier, Kempermann, Kremen, Okonkwo, Scarmeas, Soldan, Udeh-Momoh, Valenzuela, Vemuri, Vuoksimaa, Reserve, Protective Factors and Conceptual Frameworks2018), which states that reserve can be built up through a combination of life experiences (Stern, Reference Stern2012), such as education, occupation, and engaging in an active lifestyle consisting of physical activity, social relationships and cognitively demanding activities. These experiences may create a buffer against age-related cognitive decline and pathological processes by enhancing or compensating brain functioning and preserving cognitive ability.

Modifiable lifestyle factors have been suggested to protect brain health and influence dementia incidence, with an estimation that twelve factors account for 40% of dementia cases worldwide (Livingston et al., Reference Livingston, Huntley, Sommerlad, Ames, Ballard, Banerjee, Brayne, Burns, Cohen-Mansfield, Cooper, Costafreda, Dias, Fox, Gitlin, Howard, Kales, Kivimaki, Larson, Ogunniyi and Mukadam2020). Among these factors are education/intellectual enrichment, physical activity, and social engagement; aspects that are commonly incorporated in LA and can be targeted in low-cost interventions (Belleville et al., Reference Belleville, Moussard, Ansaldo, Belchior, Bherer, Bier, Bohbot, Bruneau, Cuddy, Gilbert, Jokel, Mahalingam, McGilton, Murphy, Naglie, Rochon, Troyer and Anderson2019; Kivipelto et al., Reference Kivipelto, Mangialasche and Ngandu2018; Leanos et al., Reference Leanos, Kurum, Strickland-Hughes, Ditta, Nguyen, Felix, Yum, Rebok and Wu2020; Park et al., Reference Park, Lodi-Smith, Drew, Haber, Hebrank, Bischof and Aamodt2014). Despite the cumulative evidence of LA as a protective factor for dementia (Fratiglioni et al., Reference Fratiglioni, Marseglia and Dekhtyar2020), in the context of healthy aging there is conflicting evidence on the association between LA engagement and cognitive decline. While cross-sectional studies show positive associations between cognitive function and LA engagement (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Anstey, Christensen and Windsor2012; Casaletto et al., Reference Casaletto, Renteria, Pa, Tom, Harrati, Armstrong, Rajan, Mungas, Walters, Kramer and Zahodne2020; Gow et al., Reference Gow, Avlund and Mortensen2014; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Cimino, Benitez, Brown, Gibbons, Kennison, Shirk, Atri, Robitaille, Macdonald, Lindwall, Zelinski, Willis, Schaie, Johansson, Dixon, Mungas, Hofer and Piccinin2012), longitudinal data has not always supported this finding. Some studies found that LA at baseline was not associated with longitudinal change in cognitive performance in healthy older adults (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Anstey, Christensen and Windsor2012; Gow et al., Reference Gow, Avlund and Mortensen2014; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Cimino, Benitez, Brown, Gibbons, Kennison, Shirk, Atri, Robitaille, Macdonald, Lindwall, Zelinski, Willis, Schaie, Johansson, Dixon, Mungas, Hofer and Piccinin2012). Others showed that LA engagement significantly slowed the rate of decline in different cognitive domains, such as language, executive functions (Ihle et al., Reference Ihle, Fagot, Vallet, Ballhausen, Mella, Baeriswyl, Sauter, Oris, Maurer and Kliegel2019; Lifshitz-Vahav et al., Reference Lifshitz-Vahav, Shrira and Bodner2017; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Jin, Hendrie, Liang, Yang, Cheng, Unverzagt, Ma, Hall, Murrell, Li, Bian, Pei and Gao2013), processing speed (Ghisletta et al., Reference Ghisletta, Bickel and Lovden2006; Lovden et al., Reference Lovden, Ghisletta and Lindenberger2005; Small et al., Reference Small, Dixon, McArdle and Grimm2012), and episodic memory (Lifshitz-Vahav et al., Reference Lifshitz-Vahav, Shrira and Bodner2017; Richards et al., Reference Richards, Hardy and Wadsworth2003; Small et al., Reference Small, Dixon, McArdle and Grimm2012; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Jin, Hendrie, Liang, Yang, Cheng, Unverzagt, Ma, Hall, Murrell, Li, Bian, Pei and Gao2013).

The inconsistency in the field may be attributed to the heterogeneity of the LA measures (Fratiglioni et al., Reference Fratiglioni, Marseglia and Dekhtyar2020; Ghisletta et al., Reference Ghisletta, Bickel and Lovden2006). For instance, studies frequently select different LA items and focus on certain components independently (e.g., intellectual, physical, or social), while others evaluate them as an ensemble (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Anstey, Christensen and Windsor2012; Gow et al., Reference Gow, Avlund and Mortensen2014). Although examining individual LA components is critical to better understanding its specific effects and potential mechanisms (Casaletto et al., Reference Casaletto, Renteria, Pa, Tom, Harrati, Armstrong, Rajan, Mungas, Walters, Kramer and Zahodne2020), doing so may not capture the diversity and potential interactions between the different LAs, which typically co-occur in real-life. LA participation can differ regarding duration, frequency, and intensity or level of effort, aspects that are not well captured since many studies investigate LA engagement based on binary responses (e.g., “yes vs. no” or “none vs. some”) (Armstrong et al., Reference Armstrong, Tom, Harrati, Casaletto, Pa, Arce Renteria, Gu, Rajan, Schupf, Fieo, Weuve, Simonsick, Manly, Stern and Zahodne2021; Gow, Mortensen, et al., Reference Gow, Mortensen and Avlund2012; Scarmeas et al., Reference Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly and Stern2001).

It is worth mentioning that although physical activity and exercise are often used as interchangeable terms, they are not the same (Caspersen et al., Reference Caspersen, Powell and Christenson1985). Physical activity is defined as a broad term that refers to body movement produced by the contraction of skeletal muscles and increases energy expenditure. Exercise is a component of physical activity and refers to a planned, structured, and repetitive movement to improve or maintain physical fitness. In addition, physical LA has been measured based on self-report scales but also through objective metrics that track activity during the day/week. Our study focuses on a self-report measure of physical LA.

Another aspect that may account for inconsistencies across studies is the heterogeneity of cognitive outcomes adopted by the studies analyzing LA–cognition associations. Some studies have investigated this question utilizing a variety of cognitive domains such as executive functions, language, speed, memory, and global cognition (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Jin, Hendrie, Liang, Yang, Cheng, Unverzagt, Ma, Hall, Murrell, Li, Bian, Pei and Gao2013; Yates et al., Reference Yates, Ziser, Spector and Orrell2016). Other studies have focused on specific cognitive domains such as speed processing, attention (Ghisletta et al., Reference Ghisletta, Bickel and Lovden2006; Ihle et al., Reference Ihle, Fagot, Vallet, Ballhausen, Mella, Baeriswyl, Sauter, Oris, Maurer and Kliegel2019; Lovden et al., Reference Lovden, Ghisletta and Lindenberger2005), or general cognitive ability based on a few tasks (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Cherbuin, Bunce and Anstey2014; Gow et al., Reference Gow, Avlund and Mortensen2014; Gow et al., Reference Gow, Pattie and Deary2017). In addition, some studies have utilized only one test to assess a specific domain (Ihle et al., Reference Ihle, Fagot, Vallet, Ballhausen, Mella, Baeriswyl, Sauter, Oris, Maurer and Kliegel2019; Lifshitz-Vahav et al., Reference Lifshitz-Vahav, Shrira and Bodner2017), which does not reflect robust cognitive constructs and may depend on task specific characteristics (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Anstey, Christensen and Windsor2012)

Moreover, a source of variability in the studies are the covariates included in the models. Socioeconomic status has not always been included as a covariate (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Anstey, Christensen and Windsor2012; Casaletto et al., Reference Casaletto, Renteria, Pa, Tom, Harrati, Armstrong, Rajan, Mungas, Walters, Kramer and Zahodne2020), despite being a relevant factor that may influence the accessibility and time available to participate in LA. Reverse causality may be a critical aspect for conflicting evidence (Fratiglioni et al., Reference Fratiglioni, Marseglia and Dekhtyar2020; Gow, Corley, et al., Reference Gow, Corley, Starr and Deary2012; Gow, Mortensen, et al., Reference Gow, Mortensen and Avlund2012); for instance, premorbid intelligence and education may influence LA opportunities, choices, engagement, or maintenance/withdrawal from activities. It is also possible that when older adults perceive any cognitive change or difficulty this may affect LA participation.

To further understand the potential protective role of LA on cognitive functioning, it is optimal to account for brain measures, which would differentiate between cognitive reserve and brain maintenance mechanisms. According to the recent Reserve & Resilience framework definitions (https://reserveandresilience.com/definitions/), cognitive reserve is a brain property that allows for sustained cognitive performance in the face of age-related changes, brain insult, or disease. If LA functions as a cognitive reserve mechanism, LA would be associated with reduced cognitive decline given similar structural brain measurements. Considering the same framework, brain maintenance refers to the relative preservation of the brain over time as a determinant of preserved cognition in older age. If LA functions as a brain maintenance mechanism, LA would be associated with more preserved brain measures (Stern et al., Reference Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartres-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat, Ewers, Franzmeier, Kempermann, Kremen, Okonkwo, Scarmeas, Soldan, Udeh-Momoh, Valenzuela, Vemuri, Vuoksimaa, Reserve, Protective Factors and Conceptual Frameworks2018). While there are several brain measures sufficient to answer this theoretical question, cortical thickness is considered a good measure since it is less influenced by brain/head size, unlike others (e.g., brain volume). Therefore, to advance LA research, it is critical to analyze longitudinal data in a well characterized sample of cognitively healthy individuals, with robust measurement of LA and cognition, critical demographic covariates, and brain health measures.

Another research path, and potential source of inconsistencies across studies, is the moderation role of gender and age in activity-cognition associations. It is possible that the LA–cognition associations are influenced by demographics such as age and gender, which are known to influence cognitive functioning and can influence the way people are exposed to LA. A better understanding of moderators may be more precisely informative of when it is more critical to engage in LA, or if there is any age or gender specificity, an aspect that may inform recommendations and interventions.

Gender differences have not gained much attention in LA literature (Bielak, Reference Bielak2010; Hassing, Reference Hassing2020), although patterns of LA may differ between men and women given cultural differences in roles and occupational/career opportunities. Evidence shows gender differences in LA engagement and its association with cognitive functioning. In women, higher involvement in intellectual-cultural activities was associated with better cognitive ability, and higher engagement in domestic activities predicted steeper cognitive decline. In men, higher involvement in self-improvement activities (e.g., participation in clubs, studies and sports) was associated with better cognitive ability across different domains (Hassing, Reference Hassing2020). In addition, there is evidence that cognitively and socially engaging LA in midlife among men (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Helms, Steffens, Burke, Potter and Plassman2008), and intellectual-cultural activities among women (Crowe et al., Reference Crowe, Andel, Pedersen, Johansson and Gatz2003) reduces the risk of dementia 20–40 years later.

It remains unclear whether the association between LA engagement and cognitive decline varies as a function of age. It is hypothesized that engaging in cognitively stimulating activities might have the greatest impact at older ages (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Anstey, Christensen and Windsor2012; Bielak, Reference Bielak2010; Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Cherbuin, Bunce and Anstey2014), when cognitive decline becomes more marked, and the general effects of lifestyle have had the greatest opportunity to accumulate. This hypothesis has found empirical support, but there is limited evidence across the range of adulthood. For instance, in a comparison of cohorts in their early twenties, forties, and sixties, a positive association between LAs and cognitive performance was observed only among the older population (Parslow et al., Reference Parslow, Jorm, Christensen and Mackinnon2006). Similarly, a greater effect of physical activity was observed among older versus younger adults (Hillman et al., Reference Hillman, Motl, Pontifex, Posthuma, Stubbe, Boomsma and de Geus2006; Ogino et al., Reference Ogino, Manly, Schupf, Mayeux and Gu2019). Nevertheless, other groups did not find a greater effect of cognitive activity on cognition in older adulthood (Newson & Kemps, Reference Newson and Kemps2006; Salthouse et al., Reference Salthouse, Berish and Miles2002).

Taking into account the aforementioned literature and methodological considerations, the aims of the current study were to: (1) examine the protective role of LA engagement on cognitive change, considering a 5-year span in cognitively healthy adults aged 21 through 80; (2) explore if specific components of LA (i.e., cognitive-intellectual, social, or physical) are the driver of the relationship between LA engagement and cognitive change; (3) observe whether the association between baseline LA and cognitive change is independent of cortical thickness; and (4) assess the moderating role of gender and age in the LA–cognition associations. Our main hypothesis is that LA is associated with slower cognitive decline across two time points regardless of demographics, and we expect this effect to be independent from brain thickness, supporting the cognitive reserve framework. In our exploratory analysis, we also expect to observe that LA components will predict cognitive change across two time points, but we do not anticipate a specific cognitive benefit to be associated with one activity modality. Regarding the role of moderators, we hypothesized that gender is a moderator of LA–cognition associations in the sense that LA engagement benefits cognitive functioning differently in men and women, however we do not anticipate specific cognitive benefit by gender when considering past research. In relation to age, we hypothesized that LA would be strongly associated with cognitive change at older ages, when cognitive decline becomes more pronounced, and effects of lifestyle have had the greatest opportunity to accumulate.

Our approach adds to existing knowledge by implementing methodological strengths, such as: an aggregate LA frequency score comprising of a range of common activities that reflect real-life experience; concomitantly addressing specific LA components defined based on previous works (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2006; Scarmeas et al., Reference Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly and Stern2001; Verghese et al., Reference Verghese, Lipton, Katz, Hall, Derby, Kuslansky, Ambrose, Sliwinski and Buschke2003) and tested in a confirmatory analysis; robust measurement of cognitive abilities well established to change with aging (Habeck et al., Reference Habeck, Gazes, Razlighi, Steffener, Brickman, Barulli, Salthouse and Stern2016; Salthouse et al., Reference Salthouse, Habeck, Razlighi, Barulli, Gazes and Stern2015); inclusion of multiple cognitive tasks for each cognitive ability predefined statistically; use of latent change score modeling to calculate cognitive scores at baseline and follow-up; and controlling for critical demographics in addition to age and gender, such as education and SES. Critically, we are the first to test cognitive reserve vs. brain maintenance mechanisms when examining LA–cognitive change associations, and the exploration of gender and age moderation adds to the limited research on the potential moderators of these associations.

Methods

Sample characteristics

Participants derived from two ongoing studies at Columbia University Irving Medical Center: the Cognitive Reserve (CR) and the Reference Ability Neural Network (RANN) studies. The CR was designed to elucidate the neural underpinnings of cognitive and brain reserve (Stern et al., Reference Stern, Arenaza-Urquijo, Bartres-Faz, Belleville, Cantilon, Chetelat, Ewers, Franzmeier, Kempermann, Kremen, Okonkwo, Scarmeas, Soldan, Udeh-Momoh, Valenzuela, Vemuri, Vuoksimaa, Reserve, Protective Factors and Conceptual Frameworks2018), and RANN was designed to identify networks of brain activity uniquely associated with performance across the lifespan for four reference abilities: memory, reasoning, speed, and vocabulary (Habeck et al., Reference Habeck, Gazes, Razlighi, Steffener, Brickman, Barulli, Salthouse and Stern2016). This is a longitudinal study utilizing data from these two cohorts.

Both studies share similar recruitment and data collection procedures. Participants who met initial inclusion criteria (i.e., right-handed, native English speakers, no psychiatric/neurological disorders, and normal or corrected-to-normal vision) were evaluated with structured medical screening and neuropsychological evaluations to ensure there were no cognitive impairments, and neuropsychiatric conditions. In addition, information on lifestyle, such as engagement in LA, were collected. For inclusion, a score equal or greater than 130 was required on the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale (Mattis, Reference Mattis1988), and preserved functionality in the Blessed Activities of Daily Living scale (Blessed et al., Reference Blessed, Tomlinson and Roth1968).

Follow-up evaluation occurred at a 5-year interval, when MRI scanning, cognitive, and functional measures were repeated. Performance on the cognitive battery at baseline or follow-up that was indicative of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia was grounds for exclusion. Diagnoses of MCI or dementia was determined in consensus between a neurologist and neuropsychologist who reviewed the medical records to adjudicate a diagnosis based on standard research criteria. The current analysis included 234 participants (age range 21–80 years), who were assessed at both baseline and follow-up, had cognitive data in both assessments, and completed leisure activity questionnaires at baseline. The studies were approved by the Institutional Review Board of the College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University and was completed in accordance with Helsinki Declaration.

Cognitive tasks

At baseline and follow-up, participants underwent a comprehensive neuropsychological (out-of-scanner) evaluation and performed several computerized cognitive tasks during the MRI exam (Gazes et al., Reference Gazes, Lee, Zhiqian, Mensing, Noofoory, Nazario, Babukutty, Habeck and Stern2021; Habeck et al., Reference Habeck, Gazes, Razlighi, Steffener, Brickman, Barulli, Salthouse and Stern2016; Salthouse et al., Reference Salthouse, Habeck, Razlighi, Barulli, Gazes and Stern2015). As per a previous study from our group (Salthouse et al., Reference Salthouse, Habeck, Razlighi, Barulli, Gazes and Stern2015), 12 measures were selected based on a factor analysis reflecting four domains: reasoning, processing speed, memory, and vocabulary. Previous works indicate that reasoning, speed, and memory decline with cognitive aging, while vocabulary tends to increase or remain stable over time (Gazes et al., Reference Gazes, Lee, Zhiqian, Mensing, Noofoory, Nazario, Babukutty, Habeck and Stern2021; Habeck et al., Reference Habeck, Gazes, Razlighi, Steffener, Brickman, Barulli, Salthouse and Stern2016; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2004; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2009, Reference Salthouse2019; Stern et al., Reference Stern, Habeck, Steffener, Barulli, Gazes, Razlighi, Shaked and Salthouse2014). Each cognitive domain included three measures: reasoning: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) Matrix Reasoning, Letter-number Sequencing, and Block Design test (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1997); speed: WAIS-III Digit-symbol (digit coding test), Stroop Color Naming Test (Golden, Reference Golden1975), and Trail Making Test (TMT)-A (time) (Reitan, Reference Reitan1978); memory: Selective Reminding Test (SRT); last trial, continuous long-term retrieval and last retrieval (Buschke & Fuld, Reference Buschke and Fuld1974); vocabulary: WAIS-III Vocabulary test, the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2001), and the American National Adult Reading Test (AMNART) (Grober et al., Reference Grober, Sliwinski and Korey1991).

As described previously (Gazes et al., Reference Gazes, Lee, Zhiqian, Mensing, Noofoory, Nazario, Babukutty, Habeck and Stern2021; Habeck et al., Reference Habeck, Gazes, Razlighi, Steffener, Brickman, Barulli, Salthouse and Stern2016), the in-scanner computerized tasks were comprised of twelve measures reflecting the same cognitive domains selected from the neuropsychological battery. Details on MRI acquisition are described in Supplementary Material. Each domain included three measures: reasoning: Paper Folding (Ekstrom et al., Reference Ekstrom, French, Harman and Dermen1976), Matrix Reasoning and Letter Sets; speed: Digit Symbol, Letter Comparison (Salthouse & Babcock, Reference Salthouse and Babcock1991), and Pattern Comparison tasks; memory: Logical Memory, Word Order Recognition, and Paired Associates; vocabulary: Synonyms, Antonyms, and Picture Naming.

Leisure activities

The LA questionnaire is an updated version of the 13-item questionnaire previously created and used by our group and collaborators (Armstrong et al., Reference Armstrong, Tom, Harrati, Casaletto, Pa, Arce Renteria, Gu, Rajan, Schupf, Fieo, Weuve, Simonsick, Manly, Stern and Zahodne2021; Helzner et al., Reference Helzner, Scarmeas, Cosentino, Portet and Stern2007; Scarmeas et al., Reference Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly and Stern2001). The questionnaire updated some of the items and added additional activities (gardening, going to lecture or concert, cooking, collecting, art & craft). Participation in the 18 LAs during the previous 6 months was collected at baseline (Figure 1). Participants reported the frequency of their participation in each activity using a 3-point scale: never, sometimes, or often (coded as 0, 1, and 2, respectively). An aggregate summed score (range 0–36) was assigned to each participant reflecting the frequency of leisure engagement. We do not have data on the intensity of each activity.

Figure 1. List of leisure activities included in the questionnaire.

In secondary analyses, we examined the frequency of LA reflecting three types of activities: cognitive-intellectual, social, and physical. Following previous literature (Armstrong et al., Reference Armstrong, Tom, Harrati, Casaletto, Pa, Arce Renteria, Gu, Rajan, Schupf, Fieo, Weuve, Simonsick, Manly, Stern and Zahodne2021; Helzner et al., Reference Helzner, Scarmeas, Cosentino, Portet and Stern2007; Scarmeas et al., Reference Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly and Stern2001; Verghese et al., Reference Verghese, Lipton, Katz, Hall, Derby, Kuslansky, Ambrose, Sliwinski and Buschke2003), the LA domains were defined by three summed scores of similarly grouped LA items (Figure 1). Intellectual LA was defined by six items (range 0–12) that reflect intellectually demanding activities and activities that engage cognition through art or music. Art and music activities have been considered cognitively demanding at a similar level to other typical intellectual LAs (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2006) and have specific effects on cognitive performance (Verghese et al., Reference Verghese, Lipton, Katz, Hall, Derby, Kuslansky, Ambrose, Sliwinski and Buschke2003; Yu et al., Reference Yu, Rawtaer, Goh, Kumar, Feng, Kua and Mahendran2020). Social LA was defined by six items (range 0–12) including activities that typically involve some level of social interaction. Physical LA was defined by three items (range 0–6) that reflect activities with evident physical engagement under different effort levels and energy expenditure, which included both physical activity and exercise as defined previously (e.g., going for a walks and rides, gardening, engaging in sports, dance, or exercise).

A confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to evaluate the factor structure of LA domains specified a priori (i.e., cognitive-intellectual, social, and physical). Goodness-of-fit measures were based on three model-fit statistics: root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA) (Steiger, Reference Steiger1990), Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) (Tucker & Lewis, Reference Tucker and Lewis1973), and comparative fit index (CFI) (Bentler, Reference Bentler1990). General good model-data fit is observed if RMSEA values are lower than 0.06 and if TLI and CFI are higher than 0.95 (Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999), in addition, it is acceptable if higher than 0.90 (Bentler & Bonett, Reference Bentler and Bonett1980).

Covariates

The covariates adopted in the main model were age, gender, years of education, annual family income and cognitive performance at baseline. SES was measured based on self-reported family income in the past 12 months. We treated SES it as a dichotomous variable with ≥$75,000 as reference, based on a median split, since 53% of the sample reported receiving up to $74,999 yearly.

Statistical Analyses

Data description

Demographics, cognitive performance, and LA engagement profile of the participants were described using means and standard deviation. Frequency and percentage were utilized for categorical variables. Characteristics of participants by LA frequency tertiles were compared using analyses of variance (ANOVA) for continuous variables and Pearson’s chi-square for categorical variables. Pearson correlations and t-tests were conducted to examine bivariate relationships among LA and demographics.

Latent change score model

We used a multiple indicator latent change score model (LCSM) (Kievit et al., Reference Kievit, Brandmaier, Ziegler, van Harmelen, de Mooij, Moutoussis, Goodyer, Bullmore, Jones, Fonagy, Consortium, Lindenberger and Dolan2018) to generate cognitive scores at baseline and follow-up, as shown in Figure 2 (Gazes et al., Reference Gazes, Lee, Zhiqian, Mensing, Noofoory, Nazario, Babukutty, Habeck and Stern2021), but without covariates. For additional details on the coefficients associated with each of the latent variables, see Table 1 on Supplementary material. The LCSM aims to model change in the latent score rather than observed scores. We modeled the changes of cognitive measures representing the four cognitive domains detailed above, each based on three out-of-scanner and three in-scanner tests, using a traditional confirmatory factor analysis as described in our previous studies (Salthouse et al., Reference Salthouse, Habeck, Razlighi, Barulli, Gazes and Stern2015). Factor loadings were constrained so that baseline and follow-up loadings were the same. Cognitive change values were calculated as follow-up scores minus baseline scores resulted from LCSM. Positive values indicate increase of cognitive performance and negative values indicate decline in performance over time. We also established the measurement invariance across two time points. The goodness of the model fit was assessed using the CFI, TLI, and RMSEA, with evidence of adequate fit indicated by CFI/TLI ≥ 0.90 and RMSEA ≤ 0.08 (Marsh et al., Reference Marsh, Hau, Grayson, Maydeu-Olivares and McArdle2005). The overall fit statistics were close to acceptable range: CFI = 0.85, TLI = 0.84, and RMSEA = 0.069 (95% CI = 0.065–0.072, p < 0.001). The full result of the LCSM and its R code is found in Supplementary material.

Figure 2. Diagram for the latent change score model.  Legend: Coefficients linking indicators and the latent variables at both time points are constrained to be same.  Acronyms of the figures are listed by reference abilities: Fluid Reasoning: In scanner-Paper Folding (PF), Matrix Reasoning (MR), Letter Sets (LSets); Out-scanner- Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) Block design task (BD), WAIS III Letter–Number Sequencing test (LNSD), and WAIS III Matrix Reasoning test (W3MR); Processing Speed: In scanner-Digit Symbol (DS), Letter Comparison (LC), Pattern Comparison (PC); Out-scanner- Digit Symbol subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (DSWAIS), Part A of the Trail making test (TMTA), and Color naming component of the Stroop (CNS); Memory: In scanner-Logical Memory (LM), Word Order recognition (WOR), Paired Associates (PA); Out-scanner- Selective Reminding Task - long-term storage sub-score (SRTlts), Selective Reminding Task - continuous long-term retrieval (SRTctrl), and Selective Reminding Task - the number of words recalled on the last trial (SRTlast); Vocabulary: In scanner-Synonyms (SYN), Antonyms (ANT), Picture Naming (PN); Out-scanner- vocabulary subtest from the WAIS III  (VOC), the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR), and American National Adult Reading Test (NART).

Association between baseline LA and cognitive change

To examine the relationship between LA and cognitive change, hierarchical multiple regression models were computed using the cognitive change score in each of the cognitive domains as dependent variables and LA as the independent variable, both for total LA frequency in the primary analyses and for each of the LA categories (i.e., cognitive-intellectual, social, and physical) in the secondary analyses. First, the models were adjusted for baseline age only (Model 1), and subsequently adjusted for gender, years of education, SES, and baseline cognitive performance (Model 2). The role of gender as a potential moderator was investigated in Model 3, which included the same covariates as Model 2, as well as the interaction between baseline LA and gender. Similarly, age as a potential moderator was investigated in Model 4, which included the same covariates as Model 2, but also included the interaction between baseline LA and age. Linear moderation effect was investigated using age as a continuous variable (Model 4A), and nonlinear moderation effect was examined using age as categorical variable (Model 4B) considering the three age groups (i.e., young = 21–39 years, middle aged = 40–59 years, and older adults = 60–80 years).

Given that LA participation may influence not only cognitive functioning but brain health measures (Casaletto et al., Reference Casaletto, Renteria, Pa, Tom, Harrati, Armstrong, Rajan, Mungas, Walters, Kramer and Zahodne2020), in a subset of participants that had available brain measurements (N = 140), we added change in thickness to the model, considering all covariates included in our main model (Model 2). By controlling for cortical thickness, we may observe the effect of LA on cognitive change beyond differential brain change (i.e., brain maintenance), which would suggest a cognitive reserve mechanism.

It is relevant to note that we performed a regression with the estimated factor scores as the primary analysis for two reasons. First, the factors were estimated in the larger sample size (n = 254) and second, LCSM results in different factor loadings across different models. However, since the LCSM possibly has better power, we also performed a separate LCSM analysis as a supplementary.

Analyses were performed using R package and SPSS 26, and the significance level was set at 0.05. We also provided effect sizes (Cohen`s f2) for each regression model, which are interpreted according to Cohen’s guidelines: f2 ≥ 0.02 (small), f2 ≥ 0.15 (medium), and f2 ≥ 0.35 (large) (Cohen, Reference Cohen1988).

Results

Sample characteristics

Sample characteristics are described in Table 1. The mean age was 53.8 years (SD = 16.4, range 21–80 years), 56.4% were female, 62.2% White, 24.9% African American, 3.9% Asian, and 11.9% Hispanic. In addition, mean years of education was 16.3 years (SD = 2.3) and approximately half of the participants (53.4%) reported annual family income below <$75.000. Regarding the LA profile, the baseline number of LA was 12.9 activities (SD = 2.3, range 7–18) and the frequency was 19.7 (SD = 4.4, range 8.4–32). Older individuals reported greater engagement in LA (p = .04), and, at a trend level, individuals with higher education reported higher LA engagement (p = .05). Although there was no gender effect on the overall LA frequency, cooking as a LA was significantly higher in women (p = .007).

Table 1. Demographic and cognitive characteristics of the study participants by tertiles of leisure activity frequency

BL: baseline; CH: change (follow-up minus baseline); LA: leisure activity; SD: standard deviation; YO: years old.

a p values from ANOVA for continuous variables and chi-squared for categorical variables.

b Missing data for 26 participants.

* p-values < .05.

Correlation analysis (Table 2, Supplementary Material) revealed that age was positively associated with overall LA frequency (p = .02) and negatively associated with baseline performance, indicating that reasoning, memory, and speed domains decreased as a function of age, whereas vocabulary increased with aging (ps. < .001). Age was also negatively associated with cognitive change scores, indicating a steeper decline in older individuals (ps. < .001), however the association was not significant for memory. Education was positively associated with LA frequency (p = .01), and baseline performance for reasoning, vocabulary (ps. < .001) and memory (p = .008), but negatively associated with change in vocabulary (p = .04), which did not remain significant after controlling for age (p = .16). T-tests revealed an effect of gender on vocabulary baseline scores, indicating women perform better (p = .01), and SES was associated with baseline performance for vocabulary and reasoning (ps. = .001), indicating poorer performance in those with lower annual income.

Relationship between LA frequency and cognitive change

In Model 1 (adjusted only for age) we observed that higher baseline LA frequency was associated with lower rates of cognitive decline for reasoning (p = .003) and speed (p = .004) (Table 2). In Model 2 (adjusted for age and demographics), we observed that higher LA frequency was associated with improvement on vocabulary (p = .02) and lower rates of cognitive decline for reasoning (p = .003), speed (p = .004), and at trend level, for memory (p = .05) (Table 2, Figure 3). A similar pattern was observed in a model not including SES as a covariate, since there was missing data for 24 participants (Table 3, Supplementary Material). It is worth mentioning that in the main model (Model 2), the effect sizes (f2) ranged from medium (memory and vocabulary) to large (speed and reasoning), and most results survived the Bonferroni correction, except memory, as indicted in Table 2. Our results remained similar when analyzing the data using LSCM approach (Supplementary Material, R code link).

Figure 3. Associations between total leisure activity frequency and cognitive change.

Table 2. Association between leisure activity frequency at baseline and cognitive change

LA: leisure activity; ß: standardized regression coefficient; gender: F = 0, M = 1; family income: <75.000 = 0. >75.000 = 1; didn’t know = 2 (10 participants). Results in each model that survived Bonferroni correction are highlighted in bold.

a p < .0001.

b Non-significant model.

* p-values ≤ .05.

The associations between each LA category and cognitive change (Table 3) followed a similar pattern observed in the main finding (Model 2). For instance, higher social-LA frequency predicted vocabulary improvement (at a trend level) and slower rates of cognitive decline for reasoning and speed. Similarly, higher cognitive-intellectual LA was associated with slower decline of reasoning and memory, and at trend level for speed. No associations were observed between cognitive change and physical LA.

Table 3. Association between categories of leisure activity frequency at baseline and cognitive change

LA: leisure activity; ß: standardized regression coefficient; gender: F = 0, M = 1; family income: <75.000 = 0. >75.000 = 1; didn’t know = 2 (10 participants).

a p < .0001.

b p = .001.

* p-values ≤ .05.

Confirmatory factor analysis

The CFA indicated an acceptable fit for the three-factor model of LA including cognitive-intellectual, social, and physical domains. The RMSEA of 0.058 indicates a good fit (lower than 0.06), and CFI of 0.86 and TLI of 0.83 are marginal but close to the acceptable fit of 0.90. The detailed CFA diagram is presented as Supplementary Material (Figure 1). Since the model fit was marginal, we ran another CFA including only the items with significant loadings, which resulted in a better fit (CFI of 0.90 and TLI of 0.87 – Figure 2, Supplementary Material) and did not significantly alter the LA-cognitive change associations (Table 4, Supplementary Material).

Test for brain maintenance effect

Even though LA was not associated with the thickness change (p > .05), individual differences in structural change might influence cognitive changes. Therefore, in a subset of participants (N = 140), we tested how change in thickness influenced the effects of LA on cognitive change, considering the same covariates included in Model 2. Total-LA predicted lower rates of cognitive decline for reasoning (p = .02) and speed (p = .03), but not for memory and vocabulary (Table 5, Supplementary Material).

Gender moderation

Gender moderated the association between baseline LA frequency and cognitive change for reasoning (p = .02) and speed (p = .03) (Table 4). The analyses stratified by gender revealed that these associations occurred only in men (ps = .001) and was not significant for women (ps > .05).

Table 4. Gender moderation of the association between leisure activity and cognitive change

LA: leisure activity; ß: standardized regression coefficient; gender: F = 0, M = 1; family income: <75.000 = 0. >75.000 = 1; didn’t know = 2 (10 participants).

a p < 0.0001.

* p-values ≤ .05.

Age moderation

Age did not moderate the association between baseline LA engagement and cognitive change across all cognitive domains (Table 6, Supplementary Material).

Discussion

The overall study aim was to examine the relationship between self-reported measures of LA frequency and cognitive change over a span of 5 years across adulthood. We found that greater baseline LA engagement relates to less decline across cognitive domains (i.e., reasoning, speed, and episodic memory), and better vocabulary performance. Both social and cognitive-intellectual LA predicted less cognitive decline in reasoning, however LA components presented specific associations with cognitive change: social-LA was associated with less decline on speed and better vocabulary, and cognitive-intellectual-LA was associated with less memory decline. Physical-LA was not associated with cognitive change. Additionally, the relationships between LA frequency and cognitive change were shown to be stable across adulthood since age did not moderate these associations. Moreover, our data revealed gender as a moderator of LA-cognition associations, indicating stronger association in men.

Our results agree with previous longitudinal studies examining the association between baseline LA and cognitive change in healthy middle-aged individuals and older adults (Ghisletta et al., Reference Ghisletta, Bickel and Lovden2006; Lovden et al., Reference Lovden, Ghisletta and Lindenberger2005; Richards et al., Reference Richards, Hardy and Wadsworth2003; Small et al., Reference Small, Dixon, McArdle and Grimm2012; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Jin, Hendrie, Liang, Yang, Cheng, Unverzagt, Ma, Hall, Murrell, Li, Bian, Pei and Gao2013; Yates et al., Reference Yates, Ziser, Spector and Orrell2016). Critically, LA protective benefits have been observed in several cognitive domains, such as global cognition, language, executive functions (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Jin, Hendrie, Liang, Yang, Cheng, Unverzagt, Ma, Hall, Murrell, Li, Bian, Pei and Gao2013; Yates et al., Reference Yates, Ziser, Spector and Orrell2016), speed processing (Ghisletta et al., Reference Ghisletta, Bickel and Lovden2006; Lovden et al., Reference Lovden, Ghisletta and Lindenberger2005; Small et al., Reference Small, Dixon, McArdle and Grimm2012; Yates et al., Reference Yates, Ziser, Spector and Orrell2016) and episodic memory (Richards et al., Reference Richards, Hardy and Wadsworth2003; Small et al., Reference Small, Dixon, McArdle and Grimm2012; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Jin, Hendrie, Liang, Yang, Cheng, Unverzagt, Ma, Hall, Murrell, Li, Bian, Pei and Gao2013; Yates et al., Reference Yates, Ziser, Spector and Orrell2016). A novel finding from our study is that LA was not only associated with less cognitive decline but also with better performance on vocabulary or crystallized intelligence, as this domain has been shown to improve over time (Gazes et al., Reference Gazes, Lee, Zhiqian, Mensing, Noofoory, Nazario, Babukutty, Habeck and Stern2021; Salthouse, Reference Salthouse1998, Reference Salthouse2010). Interestingly, our finding was consistent across these cognitive domains with effect sizes ranging from medium (vocabulary) to large (reasoning and speed). Our findings support the cognitive reserve framework, in line with the differential preservation hypothesis (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2006), stating that above and beyond several demographic characteristics and differential brain change, an active lifestyle confers protective effects on cognitive functioning and helps individuals to cope better with aging.

Our results contrast with previous findings that LA was associated with cognitive ability but not change over time (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Anstey, Christensen and Windsor2012; Gow et al., Reference Gow, Avlund and Mortensen2014; Gow et al., Reference Gow, Pattie and Deary2017; Mitchell et al., Reference Mitchell, Cimino, Benitez, Brown, Gibbons, Kennison, Shirk, Atri, Robitaille, Macdonald, Lindwall, Zelinski, Willis, Schaie, Johansson, Dixon, Mungas, Hofer and Piccinin2012). These studies support the preserved differentiation hypothesis, which states that people who are more mentally active tend to have high levels of cognitive functioning throughout their lives, and thus differences in performance are preserved across adulthood (Salthouse, Reference Salthouse2006). While it remains inconclusive if LA level affects subsequent cognitive change, and/or whether cognitive ability level at baseline predicts LA participation (Bielak, Reference Bielak2010; Gow et al., Reference Gow, Avlund and Mortensen2014), our results support the former owing to the fact that we controlled for baseline cognitive ability, and intervention studies suggest the same (Kivipelto et al., Reference Kivipelto, Mangialasche and Ngandu2018; Leanos et al., Reference Leanos, Kurum, Strickland-Hughes, Ditta, Nguyen, Felix, Yum, Rebok and Wu2020; Park et al., Reference Park, Lodi-Smith, Drew, Haber, Hebrank, Bischof and Aamodt2014). Nevertheless, we cannot rule out that premorbid intelligence or any subtle cognitive difficulty may have influenced LA engagement.

We observed a consistent cognitive benefit of total LA frequency, a variable reflecting a variety of activities. This is in line with the reported benefits of “activity diversity” (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Charles and Almeida2020) and learning multiple skills simultaneously (Leanos et al., Reference Leanos, Kurum, Strickland-Hughes, Ditta, Nguyen, Felix, Yum, Rebok and Wu2020). When examining the effects of different components separately, social and cognitive-intellectual LAs were those that drove the main findings: both LA components benefit reasoning trajectory, however social LA seemed more relevant for speed and vocabulary trajectory, and cognitive-intellectual LA for memory trajectory. It is possible that these differences occurred by chance or may have been influenced by dose or level of effort of the LAs. In addition, the division between social and cognitive domains might be questionable, since these modalities may be stimulated simultaneously (e.g., playing cards/games, playing musical instruments, going to classes) and therefore provide synergetic effects.

Although the specific mechanism through which LA influences cognitive functioning remains unclear, intellectual enrichment has been associated with increased synaptic density in mice (Cracchiolo et al., Reference Cracchiolo, Mori, Nazian, Tan, Potter and Arendash2007), while in humans, cognitive activity/training is associated with brain structural and functional benefits, supporting its role in maintaining better brain health. For instance, cognitive training has been associated with increased white matter (Lovden et al., Reference Lovden, Bodammer, Kuhn, Kaufmann, Schutze, Tempelmann, Heinze, Duzel, Schmiedek and Lindenberger2010), grey matter and cortical volume (Nguyen et al., Reference Nguyen, Murphy and Andrews2019) and better network efficiency and plasticity of neural circuits (Cheng, Reference Cheng2016; Miotto et al., Reference Miotto, Batista, Simon and Hampstead2018). Similarly, social activity engagement can also provide mental stimulation through complex communication and interaction with others (Bennett et al., Reference Bennett, Schneider, Tang, Arnold and Wilson2006; Evans et al., Reference Evans, Llewellyn, Matthews, Woods, Brayne and Clare2019; Fratiglioni et al., Reference Fratiglioni, Marseglia and Dekhtyar2020), but also benefit through other mechanisms such as stress management and emotional support.

Surprisingly, we observed no association between physical LA and cognitive change, in contrast with the robust literature on the benefits of physical activity on cognitive/brain functioning and reduced cardiovascular disease risk (Fratiglioni et al., Reference Fratiglioni, Marseglia and Dekhtyar2020; Ogino et al., Reference Ogino, Manly, Schupf, Mayeux and Gu2019). There are several reasons why we were unable to find a relationship between physical LA and cognitive change: (1) the reduced number of questions addressing physical activity in our questionnaire (three items); (2) the self-report nature of the data; and (3) the lack of specificity on the activity, such as duration, intensity, and attendance.

It is relevant to highlight that the LA domains were chosen based on similar structures observed in previous works (Armstrong et al., Reference Armstrong, Tom, Harrati, Casaletto, Pa, Arce Renteria, Gu, Rajan, Schupf, Fieo, Weuve, Simonsick, Manly, Stern and Zahodne2021; Helzner et al., Reference Helzner, Scarmeas, Cosentino, Portet and Stern2007; Scarmeas et al., Reference Scarmeas, Levy, Tang, Manly and Stern2001). Critically, our CFA model presented an acceptable fit for the 3-factor model. For instance, the RMSEA value observed in the model is in line with the recommendation of a good model fit of RMSEA < 0.06. The CFI value and TLI were a little below the criteria of 0.95, but slightly close to the acceptable fit of 0.09 (Bentler & Bonett, Reference Bentler and Bonett1980). Based on these CFA indices, we consider this sample as an acceptable fit to the 3-factor model. In addition, when we optimize our model fit selecting only the items with significant loadings, the results remained similar.

Another novel contribution of our study is the evidence of gender moderation on reasoning and speed, indicating that the protective role of LA on cognition was stronger in men (for both domains). In our sample, we did not find gender differences in overall LA frequency, so the explanation of our findings remains unclear. Our findings suggest that men and women may engage differently in LA, which may also modify its potential protective role to age-related cognitive decline. This is in line with a few studies that observed gender differences in LA engagement and its association with cognitive performance and trajectories (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Helms, Steffens, Burke, Potter and Plassman2008; Crowe et al., Reference Crowe, Andel, Pedersen, Johansson and Gatz2003; Hassing, Reference Hassing2020). Future research should better address the gender differences and develop interventions to address potential gaps. We cannot rule out the possibility that our finding is just a reflection of our sample characteristics.

Our findings did not support the prediction that the association between LA and cognitive change would be stronger at older age (Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Anstey, Christensen and Windsor2012; Bielak, Reference Bielak2010; Bielak et al., Reference Bielak, Cherbuin, Bunce and Anstey2014; Hillman et al., Reference Hillman, Motl, Pontifex, Posthuma, Stubbe, Boomsma and de Geus2006; Ogino et al., Reference Ogino, Manly, Schupf, Mayeux and Gu2019). In contrast, our results are in line with studies that did not observe a greater effect of cognitive activity on cognition in older adults (Newson & Kemps, Reference Newson and Kemps2006; Salthouse et al., Reference Salthouse, Berish and Miles2002). The absence of age moderation suggests that LA engagement and an active lifestyle are relevant for preserving cognitive functioning in any stage of adult life, and not only at older age. We do not rule out the possibility that sample size or characteristics and cohort effects may have influenced our findings. Our results support the development of prevention-focused intervention to younger and older populations. In addition, it is possible that LA engagement is linked to, reflects or influences other relevant clinical outcomes, such as general health and mood/depression (Sharifian et al., Reference Sharifian, Gu, Manly, Schupf, Mayeux, Brickman and Zahodne2020).

The strengths of our study that enhances LA research include (1) a wide age range, addressing LA across adulthood; (2) an aggregate LA frequency score comprising of a range of common everyday life activities, and concomitantly addressing specific LA components defined statistically and based on previous works; (3) a robust measurement of cognitive abilities well established to change with aging (Habeck et al., Reference Habeck, Gazes, Razlighi, Steffener, Brickman, Barulli, Salthouse and Stern2016; Salthouse et al., Reference Salthouse, Habeck, Razlighi, Barulli, Gazes and Stern2015) comprising of multiple cognitive measures (six within each cognitive domain); (4) the use of latent change score modeling to identify latent cognitive variables at baseline and follow-up under similar parameters, which allowed us to model within and between person variances; (5) models that comprise critical demographics beyond age and gender, such as education and SES; (6) exploratory analysis on the effect of brain measures on the effect of LA on cognitive performance; and (7) moderation analysis of gender and age, both aspects with limited evidence.

The study limitations should also be noted. First, generalizability is limited since the sample is relatively modest in size, highly educated, and majority white (especially the older individuals). Future research needs samples adequately powered to investigate the roles of education, gender, and race/ethnicity in these associations. An obvious limitation inherent in the design is the self-report nature of the data, which may be influenced by recall bias and have reduced accuracy. Another limitation is the lack of data on retirement/work status and general health, which likely influences available time to engage in LA. Although we adopted a longitudinal design, we did not address the potential bidirectional association between LA and cognition, and therefore, reverse causality still is a confounder.

In conclusion, our results expand on previous literature by showing a positive association between active lifestyle and positive cognitive trajectories in a well characterized, cognitively heathy sample across adulthood. Initial LA frequency benefits cognitive trajectories over 5 years, above and beyond demographics and level of cognitive ability. Its potential protective effects seem to occur similarly across adulthood, supporting a life-course approach. Gender differences were shown to be a relevant factor that modifies the LA-cognitive change association. Our results suggest that LA is a proxy for cognitive reserve and serves as a potential buffer against age-related cognitive decline across adulthood and genders, with implications for future reserve-enhancing interventions and prevention trials, which may be needed to begin before old age.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617722000510

Acknowledgements

We thank Dr Suhang Song for the editorial assistance.

Funding statement

This work was supported by the National Institute of Aging (R01AG026158, R01AG061008, and RF1AG038465).

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Armstrong, N. M., Tom, S. E., Harrati, A., Casaletto, K., Pa, J., Arce Renteria, M., Gu, Y., Rajan, K. B., Schupf, N., Fieo, R., Weuve, J., Simonsick, E. M., Manly, J. J., Stern, Y., Zahodne, L. B. (2021, March 30). Longitudinal relationship of leisure activity engagement with cognitive performance among non-demented, community-dwelling older adults. Gerontologist, 62(3), 352363, https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnab046 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belleville, S., Moussard, A., Ansaldo, A. I., Belchior, P., Bherer, L., Bier, N., Bohbot, V. D., Bruneau, M. A., Cuddy, L. L., Gilbert, B., Jokel, R., Mahalingam, K., McGilton, K., Murphy, K. J., Naglie, G., Rochon, E., Troyer, A. K., Anderson, N. D. (2019, May 22). Rationale and protocol of the ENGAGE study: A double-blind randomized controlled preference trial using a comprehensive cohort design to measure the effect of a cognitive and leisure-based intervention in older adults with a memory complaint. Trials, 20(1), 282. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-019-3250-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Tang, Y., Arnold, S. E., & Wilson, R. S. (2006, May). The effect of social networks on the relation between Alzheimer’s disease pathology and level of cognitive function in old people: A longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Neurology, 5(5), 406412. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70417-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentler, P. M. (1990, March). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238246. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentler, P. M., & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588606. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bielak, A. A. (2010). How can we not ‘lose it’ if we still don’t understand how to ‘use it’? Unanswered questions about the influence of activity participation on cognitive performance in older age–a mini-review. Gerontologia, 56(5), 507519. https://doi.org/10.1159/000264918 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bielak, A. A., Anstey, K. J., Christensen, H., & Windsor, T. D. (2012, March). Activity engagement is related to level, but not change in cognitive ability across adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 219228. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024667 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bielak, A. A., Cherbuin, N., Bunce, D., & Anstey, K. J. (2014, July). Preserved differentiation between physical activity and cognitive performance across young, middle, and older adulthood over 8 years. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69(4), 523532. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbu016 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blessed, G., Tomlinson, B. E., & Roth, M. (1968, July). The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 114(512), 797811. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.114.512.797 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buschke, H., & Fuld, P. A. (1974). Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology, 24(11), 10191025. https://doi.org/doi.org/10.1212/WNL.24.11.1019 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, M. C., Helms, M. J., Steffens, D. C., Burke, J. R., Potter, G. G., & Plassman, B. L. (2008, September). Midlife activity predicts risk of dementia in older male twin pairs. Alzheimers & Dementia, 4(5), 324331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2008.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casaletto, K. B., Renteria, M. A., Pa, J., Tom, S. E., Harrati, A., Armstrong, N. M., Rajan, K. B., Mungas, D., Walters, S., Kramer, J., Zahodne, L. B. (2020). Late-life physical and cognitive activities independently contribute to brain and cognitive resilience. Journal of Alzheimers Disease, 74(1), 363376. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-191114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspersen, C. J., Powell, K. E., & Christenson, G. M. (1985, March-April). Physical activity, exercise, and physical fitness: Definitions and distinctions for health-related research. Public Health Reports, 100(2), 126131. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/3920711 Google ScholarPubMed
Cheng, S. T. (2016, September). Cognitive reserve and the prevention of dementia: The role of physical and cognitive activities. Current Psychiatry Reports, 18(9), 85. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-016-0721-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. E. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Cracchiolo, J. R., Mori, T., Nazian, S. J., Tan, J., Potter, H., & Arendash, G. W. (2007, October). Enhanced cognitive activity–over and above social or physical activity–is required to protect Alzheimer’s mice against cognitive impairment, reduce Abeta deposition, and increase synaptic immunoreactivity. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 88(3), 277294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nlm.2007.07.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowe, M., Andel, R., Pedersen, N. L., Johansson, B., & Gatz, M. (2003, September). Does participation in leisure activities lead to reduced risk of Alzheimer’s disease? A prospective study of Swedish twins. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(5), P249P255. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/58.5.p249 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Dermen, D. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Education Testing Service.Google Scholar
Evans, I. E. M., Llewellyn, D. J., Matthews, F. E., Woods, R. T., Brayne, C., & Clare, L. (2019, December). Social isolation, cognitive reserve, and cognition in older people with depression and anxiety. Aging & Mental Health, 23(12), 16911700. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2018.1506742 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fratiglioni, L., Marseglia, A., & Dekhtyar, S. (2020, June). Ageing without dementia: Can stimulating psychosocial and lifestyle experiences make a difference? Lancet Neurology, 19(6), 533543. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(20)30039-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazes, Y., Lee, S., Zhiqian, F., Mensing, A., Noofoory, D., Nazario, G. H., Babukutty, R., Habeck, C., & Stern, Y. (2021). IQ moderation of cognitive decline supports cognitive reserve and not brain maintenance. medRxiv. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1101/2021.02.19.21251920 Google Scholar
Ghisletta, P., Bickel, J. F., & Lovden, M. (2006, September). Does activity engagement protect against cognitive decline in old age? Methodological and analytical considerations. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61(5), P253P261. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/61.5.p253 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golden, C. J. (1975). A group version of the stroop color and word test. Journal of Personality Assessment, 39(4), 386388. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa3904_10 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gow, A. J., Avlund, K., & Mortensen, E. L. (2014). Leisure activity associated with cognitive ability level, but not cognitive change. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1176. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01176 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gow, A. J., Corley, J., Starr, J. M., & Deary, I. J. (2012, March). Reverse causation in activity-cognitive ability associations: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 250255. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024144 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gow, A. J., Mortensen, E. L., & Avlund, K. (2012, October). Activity participation and cognitive aging from age 50 to 80 in the glostrup 1914 cohort. Journal of The American Geriatrics Society, 60(10), 18311838. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04168.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gow, A. J., Pattie, A., & Deary, I. J. (2017, January). Lifecourse activity participation from early, mid, and later adulthood as determinants of cognitive aging: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1921. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 72(1), 2537. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbw124 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grober, E., Sliwinski, M., & Korey, S. R. (1991). Development and validation of a model for estimating premorbid verbal intelligence in the elderly. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13(6), 933949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habeck, C., Gazes, Y., Razlighi, Q., Steffener, J., Brickman, A., Barulli, D., Salthouse, T., & Stern, Y. (2016, January 15). The Reference Ability Neural Network Study: Life-time stability of reference-ability neural networks derived from task maps of young adults. Neuroimage, 125, 693704. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.077 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassing, L. B. (2020, January 1). Gender differences in the association between leisure activity in adulthood and cognitive function in old age: A prospective longitudinal population-based study. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(1), 1120. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbx170 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helzner, E. P., Scarmeas, N., Cosentino, S., Portet, F., & Stern, Y. (2007, December). Leisure activity and cognitive decline in incident Alzheimer disease. Archives of Neurology, 64(12), 17491754. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.64.12.1749 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillman, C. H., Motl, R. W., Pontifex, M. B., Posthuma, D., Stubbe, J. H., Boomsma, D. I., & de Geus, E. J. C. (2006, November). Physical activity and cognitive function in a cross-section of younger and older community-dwelling individuals. Health Psychology, 25(6), 678687. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.678 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 155. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ihle, A., Fagot, D., Vallet, F., Ballhausen, N., Mella, N., Baeriswyl, M., Sauter, J., Oris, M., Maurer, J., Kliegel, M. (2019, February). Cross-lagged relation of leisure activity participation to Trail Making Test performance 6 years later: Differential patterns in old age and very old age. Neuropsychology, 33(2), 234244. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000497 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kievit, R. A., Brandmaier, A. M., Ziegler, G., van Harmelen, A. L., de Mooij, S. M. M., Moutoussis, M., Goodyer, I. M., Bullmore, E., Jones, P. B., Fonagy, P., Consortium, N., Lindenberger, U., & Dolan, R. J. (2018, October). Developmental cognitive neuroscience using latent change score models: A tutorial and applications. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 33, 99117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.11.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kivipelto, M., Mangialasche, F., & Ngandu, T. (2018, November). Lifestyle interventions to prevent cognitive impairment, dementia and Alzheimer disease. Nature Reviews Neurology, 14(11), 653666. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41582-018-0070-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leanos, S., Kurum, E., Strickland-Hughes, C. M., Ditta, A. S., Nguyen, G., Felix, M., Yum, H., Rebok, G. W., & Wu, R. (2020, June 2). The impact of learning multiple real-world skills on cognitive abilities and functional independence in healthy older adults. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(6), 11551169. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbz084 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, S., Charles, S. T., & Almeida, D. M. (2020, February 6). Change is good for the brain: Activity diversity and cognitive functioning across adulthood. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa020 Google Scholar
Lifshitz-Vahav, H., Shrira, A., & Bodner, E. (2017, May). The reciprocal relationship between participation in leisure activities and cognitive functioning: The moderating effect of self-rated literacy level. Aging & Mental Health, 21(5), 524531. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2015.1124838 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Livingston, G., Huntley, J., Sommerlad, A., Ames, D., Ballard, C., Banerjee, S., Brayne, C., Burns, A., Cohen-Mansfield, J., Cooper, C., Costafreda, S. G., Dias, A., Fox, N., Gitlin, L. N., Howard, R., Kales, H. C., Kivimaki, M., Larson, E. B., Ogunniyi, A., …Mukadam, N. (2020, August 8). Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet, 396(10248), 413446. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovden, M., Bodammer, N. C., Kuhn, S., Kaufmann, J., Schutze, H., Tempelmann, C., Heinze, H. J., Duzel, E., Schmiedek, F., Lindenberger, U. (2010, November). Experience-dependent plasticity of white-matter microstructure extends into old age. Neuropsychologia, 48(13), 38783883. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.026 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovden, M., Ghisletta, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2005, September). Social participation attenuates decline in perceptual speed in old and very old age. Psychology and Aging, 20(3), 423434. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.423 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K.-T., & Grayson, D. (2005). Goodness of fit in structural equation models. In Maydeu-Olivares, A., & McArdle, J. J. (Eds.), Contemporary psychometrics: A festschrift for Roderick P. McDonald (pp. 275340). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Mattis, S. (1988). Dementia Rating Scale (DRS). Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Miotto, E. C., Batista, A. X., Simon, S. S., & Hampstead, B. M. (2018). Neurophysiologic and cognitive changes arising from cognitive training interventions in persons with mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review. Neural Plasticity, 2018, 7301530. https://doi.org/10.1155/2018/7301530 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, M. B., Cimino, C. R., Benitez, A., Brown, C. L., Gibbons, L. E., Kennison, R. F., Shirk, S. D., Atri, A., Robitaille, A., Macdonald, S. W., Lindwall, M., Zelinski, E. M., Willis, S. L., Schaie, K. W., Johansson, B., Dixon, R. A., Mungas, D. M., Hofer, S. M., Piccinin, A. M. (2012). Cognitively stimulating activities: Effects on cognition across four studies with up to 21 years of longitudinal data. Journal of Aging Research, 2012(5), 461592. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/461592 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newson, R. S., & Kemps, E. B. (2006, July–September). The influence of physical and cognitive activities on simple and complex cognitive tasks in older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 32(3), 341362. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610730600699134 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, L., Murphy, K., & Andrews, G. (2019, August). Cognitive and neural plasticity in old age: A systematic review of evidence from executive functions cognitive training. Ageing Research Reviews, 53, 100912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2019.100912 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogino, E., Manly, J. J., Schupf, N., Mayeux, R., & Gu, Y. (2019, December). Current and past leisure time physical activity in relation to risk of Alzheimer’s disease in older adults. Alzheimers & Dementia, 15(12), 16031611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2019.07.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, D. C., Lodi-Smith, J., Drew, L., Haber, S., Hebrank, A., Bischof, G. N., & Aamodt, W. (2014, January). The impact of sustained engagement on cognitive function in older adults: The Synapse Project. Psychological Science, 25(1), 103112. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613499592 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parslow, R. A., Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., & Mackinnon, A. (2006). An instrument to measure engagement in life: Factor analysis and associations with sociodemographic, health and cognition measures. Gerontologia, 52(3), 188198. https://doi.org/10.1159/000091829 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reitan, R. M. (1978). Manual for administration of neuropsycho- logical test batteries for adults and children. Sant Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Richards, M., Hardy, R., & Wadsworth, M. E. (2003, February). Does active leisure protect cognition? Evidence from a national birth cohort. Social Science & Medicine, 56(4), 785792. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0277-9536(02)00075-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sala, G., Jopp, D., Gobet, F., Ogawa, M., Ishioka, Y., Masui, Y., Inagaki, H., Nakagawa, T., Yasumoto, S., Ishizaki, T., Arai, Y., Ikebe, K., Kamide, K., Gondo, Y. (2019). The impact of leisure activities on older adults’ cognitive function, physical function, and mental health. PLoS One, 14(11), e0225006. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (1998, September). Independence of age-related influences on cognitive abilities across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 851864. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.34.5.851 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (2004). What and when of cognitive aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(4), 140144. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00293.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (2006, March). Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the validity of the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1(1), 6887. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00005.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (2009, September). Decomposing age correlations on neuropsychological and cognitive variables. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15(5), 650661. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617709990385 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (2010, September). Selective review of cognitive aging. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(5), 754760. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617710000706 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (2019, February). Trajectories of normal cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 34(1), 1724. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000288 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A., & Babcock, R. L. (1991). Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Developmental Psychology, 27(5), 763776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., Berish, D. E., & Miles, J. D. (2002, December). The role of cognitive stimulation on the relations between age and cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging, 17(4), 548557. https://doi.org/10.1037//0882-7974.17.4.548 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A., Habeck, C., Razlighi, Q., Barulli, D., Gazes, Y., & Stern, Y. (2015, November). Breadth and age-dependency of relations between cortical thickness and cognition. Neurobiology of Aging, 36(11), 30203028. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.08.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarmeas, N., Levy, G., Tang, M. X., Manly, J., & Stern, Y. (2001, December 26). Influence of leisure activity on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology, 57(12), 22362242. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.57.12.2236 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharifian, N., Gu, Y., Manly, J. J., Schupf, N., Mayeux, R., Brickman, A. M., & Zahodne, L. B. (2020, January). Linking depressive symptoms and cognitive functioning: The mediating role of leisure activity. Neuropsychology, 34(1), 107115. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000595 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Small, B. J., Dixon, R. A., McArdle, J. J., & Grimm, K. J. (2012, March). Do changes in lifestyle engagement moderate cognitive decline in normal aging? Evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Neuropsychology, 26(2), 144155. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026579 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steiger, J. H. (1990, April 1). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25(2), 173180. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y. (2012, November). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurology, 11(11), 10061012. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stern, Y., Arenaza-Urquijo, E. M., Bartres-Faz, D., Belleville, S., Cantilon, M., Chetelat, G., Ewers, M., Franzmeier, N., Kempermann, G., Kremen, W. S., Okonkwo, O., Scarmeas, N., Soldan, A., Udeh-Momoh, C., Valenzuela, M., Vemuri, P., Vuoksimaa, E., Reserve, R., Protective Factors, P. I. A. E. D., & Conceptual Frameworks, W. (2018, September 14). Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers & Dementia, 16(9), 13051311. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, Y., Habeck, C., Steffener, J., Barulli, D., Gazes, Y., Razlighi, Q., Shaked, D., & Salthouse, T. (2014, December). The Reference Ability Neural Network Study: Motivation, design, and initial feasibility analyses. Neuroimage, 103, 139151. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.09.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38(1), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verghese, J., LeValley, A., Derby, C., Kuslansky, G., Katz, M., Hall, C., Buschke, H., & Lipton, R. B. (2006, March 28). Leisure activities and the risk of amnestic mild cognitive impairment in the elderly. Neurology, 66(6), 821827. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000202520.68987.48 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verghese, J., Lipton, R. B., Katz, M. J., Hall, C. B., Derby, C. A., Kuslansky, G., Ambrose, A. F., Sliwinski, M., & Buschke, H. (2003, June 19). Leisure activities and the risk of dementia in the elderly. The New England Journal of Medicine, 348(25), 25082516. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa022252 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, H. X., Jin, Y., Hendrie, H. C., Liang, C., Yang, L., Cheng, Y., Unverzagt, F. W., Ma, F., Hall, K. S., Murrell, J. R., Li, P., Bian, J., Pei, J. J., Gao, S. (2013, February). Late life leisure activities and risk of cognitive decline. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences, 68(2), 205213. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/gls153 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler adult intelligence scale (3rd ed., pp. 684–690). Sant Anotni, TX: Harcourt Assessment.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2001). The Wechsler test of adult reading (WTAR). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Yates, L. A., Ziser, S., Spector, A., & Orrell, M. (2016, November). Cognitive leisure activities and future risk of cognitive impairment and dementia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. International Psychogeriatrics, 28(11), 17911806. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1041610216001137 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, J., Rawtaer, I., Goh, L. G., Kumar, A. P., Feng, L., Kua, E. H., & Mahendran, R. (2020, August 7). The art of remediating age-related cognitive decline: Art therapy enhances cognition and increases cortical thickness in mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 27(1), 7988. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617720000697 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. List of leisure activities included in the questionnaire.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Diagram for the latent change score model.  Legend: Coefficients linking indicators and the latent variables at both time points are constrained to be same.  Acronyms of the figures are listed by reference abilities: Fluid Reasoning: In scanner-Paper Folding (PF), Matrix Reasoning (MR), Letter Sets (LSets); Out-scanner- Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III) Block design task (BD), WAIS III Letter–Number Sequencing test (LNSD), and WAIS III Matrix Reasoning test (W3MR); Processing Speed: In scanner-Digit Symbol (DS), Letter Comparison (LC), Pattern Comparison (PC); Out-scanner- Digit Symbol subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (DSWAIS), Part A of the Trail making test (TMTA), and Color naming component of the Stroop (CNS); Memory: In scanner-Logical Memory (LM), Word Order recognition (WOR), Paired Associates (PA); Out-scanner- Selective Reminding Task - long-term storage sub-score (SRTlts), Selective Reminding Task - continuous long-term retrieval (SRTctrl), and Selective Reminding Task - the number of words recalled on the last trial (SRTlast); Vocabulary: In scanner-Synonyms (SYN), Antonyms (ANT), Picture Naming (PN); Out-scanner- vocabulary subtest from the WAIS III  (VOC), the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR), and American National Adult Reading Test (NART).

Figure 2

Table 1. Demographic and cognitive characteristics of the study participants by tertiles of leisure activity frequency

Figure 3

Figure 3. Associations between total leisure activity frequency and cognitive change.

Figure 4

Table 2. Association between leisure activity frequency at baseline and cognitive change

Figure 5

Table 3. Association between categories of leisure activity frequency at baseline and cognitive change

Figure 6

Table 4. Gender moderation of the association between leisure activity and cognitive change

Supplementary material: File

Sanz Simon et al. supplementary material

Sanz Simon et al. supplementary material

Download Sanz Simon et al. supplementary material(File)
File 671 KB