Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T15:14:52.829Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive control of learning and memory in children with bilateral spastic cerebral palsy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2005

DESIRÉE A. WHITE
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri
SHAWN E. CHRIST
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri

Abstract

Executive control of learning and memory was examined in children with bilateral spastic cerebral palsy (SCP). We hypothesized that SCP-related brain damage would disrupt executive but not associative aspects of learning and memory. To test this hypothesis, the California Verbal Learning Test–Children's Version was administered to 16 children with bilateral SCP and 19 control children ranging from 6 to 18 years of age. Controlling for general verbal ability, the groups did not differ in initial learning and retention of information over time, suggesting that associative learning and memory processes subserved by medial temporal brain regions were relatively intact in children with SCP. In contrast, impairments in learning over repeated trials, strategic processing, and inhibition in the SCP group pointed to disruptions in prefrontally-mediated executive aspects of learning and memory. The inhibitory deficit was more pronounced in younger children with SCP, suggesting a developmental delay in this ability. (JINS, 2005, 11, 920–924.)

Type
BRIEF COMMUNICATIONS
Copyright
© 2005 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Allen, M. (2002). Preterm outcomes research: A critical component of neonatal intensive care. Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 8, 221233.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Brandling-Bennett, E., White, D., Armstrong, M., Christ, S., & DeBaun, M. (2003). Patterns of verbal long-term and working memory performance reveal deficits in strategic processing in children with frontal infarcts related to sickle cell disease. Developmental Neuropsychology, 24, 423434.Google Scholar
Brunstrom, J., Bastian, A., Wong, M., & Mink, J. (2000). Motor benefit from levodopa in spastic quadriplegic cerebral palsy. Annals of Neurology, 47, 662665.Google Scholar
Chapman, L. & Chapman, J. (1978). The measurement of differential deficit. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 14, 303311.Google Scholar
Christ, S., White, D., Brunstrom, J., & Abrams, R. (2003). Inhibitory control following prenatal brain injury. Neuropsychology, 17, 171178.Google Scholar
Delis, D., Kramer, J., Kaplan, E., & Ober, B. (1994). California Verbal Learning Test: Children's version manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Diamond, A. (1998). Evidence for the importance of dopamine for prefrontal functions early in life. In A. Roberts & T. Robbins (Eds.), The prefrontal cortex: Executive and cognitive functions (pp. 144164). New York: Oxford University Press.
Isaacs, E., Lucas, A., Chong, W., Wood, S., Johnson, C., Vargha-Khadem, F., & Gadian, D. (2000). Hippocampal volume and everyday memory in children of very low birth weight. Pediatric Research, 47, 713720.Google Scholar
Johnston, M., Trescher, W., Ishida, A., & Nakajima, W. (2001). Neurobiology of hypoxic-ischemic injury in the developing brain. Pediatric Research, 49, 735741.Google Scholar
Levin, H., Culhane, K., Hartmann, J., Evankovich, K., Mattson, A., Harward, H., Ringholz, G., Ewing-Cobbs, L., & Fletcher, J. (1991). Developmental changes in performance on tests of purported frontal lobe functioning. Developmental Neuropsychology, 7, 377395.Google Scholar
Moscovitch, M. & Umilta, C. (1990). Modularity and neuropsychology: Modules and central processes in attention and memory. In M. Schwartz (Ed.), Modular deficits in Alzheimer-type dementia (pp. 159). Cambridge: MIT/Bradford.
Okumura, A., Kato, T., Kuno, K., Hayakawa, F., & Watanabe, K. (1997). MRI findings in patients with spastic cerebral palsy II: Correlation with type of cerebral palsy. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 39, 369372.Google Scholar
Pliszka, S., McCracken, J., & Maas, J. (1996). Catecholamines in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Current perspectives. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 264272.Google Scholar
Schatz, J., Craft, S., Koby, M., & Park, T. (1997). Associative learning in children with perinatal brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 3, 521527.Google Scholar
Shimamura, A. (2002). Memory retrieval and executive control processes. In D. Stuss & R. Knight (Eds.), Principles of frontal lobe function (pp. 210220). London: University Press.
Taylor, H., Klein, N., Minich, N., & Hack, M. (2000). Verbal memory deficits in children with less than 750 g birth weight. Child Neuropsychology, 6, 4963.Google Scholar
Thatcher, R. (1997). Human frontal lobe development: A theory of cyclical cortical reorganization. In N. Krasnegor, G. Lyon, & P. Goldman-Rakic (Eds.), Development of the prefrontal cortex (pp. 85113). Baltimore: Paul Brooks.
White, D., Craft, S., Hale, S., & Park, T. (1994). Working memory and articulation rate in children with spastic diplegic cerebral palsy. Neuropsychology, 8, 180186.Google Scholar
White, D., Nortz, M., Mandernach, T., Huntington, K., & Steiner, R. (2001). Deficits in memory strategy use related to prefrontal dysfunction during early development: Evidence from children with phenylketonuria. Neuropsychology, 15, 221229.Google Scholar
Woodcock, R. & Johnson, M. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery: Revised. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.