Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-19T00:17:19.788Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of cataract surgery on neuropsychological test performance: A randomized controlled trial

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 September 2006

KAARIN J. ANSTEY
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health Research, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia
STEPHEN R. LORD
Affiliation:
Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute, Sydney, Australia
MICHAEL HENNESSY
Affiliation:
School of Ophthalmology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
PAUL MITCHELL
Affiliation:
School of Ophthalmology, University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia
KATHERINE MILL
Affiliation:
Prince of Wales Medical Research Institute, Sydney, Australia
CHWEE VON SANDEN
Affiliation:
Centre for Mental Health Research, Australian National University, Canberra, Australia

Abstract

Recent cross-sectional studies have reported strong associations between visual and cognitive function, and longitudinal studies have shown relationships between visual and cognitive decline in late life. Improvement in cognitive performance after cataract surgery has been reported in patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment. We investigated whether improving visual function with cataract surgery would improve neuropsychological performance in healthy older adults. A randomized clinical trial of cataract surgery performed at acute hospitals was conducted on 56 patients (mean age 73) with bilateral cataract, after excluding a total of 54 patients at the screening stage, of whom 53 did not meet visual acuity criteria and one did not have cataract. In-home assessments included visual and neuropsychological function, computerized cognitive testing and health questionnaires. Results showed no cognitive benefits of cataract surgery in cognitively normal adults. We conclude that visual improvement following cataract surgery is not strongly associated with an improvement in neuropsychological test performance in otherwise healthy adults. Joint associations between visual and cognitive function in late life are likely to be due to central factors, and unlikely to be strongly related to eye disease. Short-term increased neural stimulation from improved visual function does not appear to affect cognitive performance. (JINS, 2006, 12, 632–639.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2006 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

AIHW. (2005). Vision problems among older Australians. Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare.
Anstey, K.J., Butterworth, P., Borzycki, M., & Andrews, S. (2006). Between and within individual effects of visual contrast sensitivity on perceptual matching, processing speed and associative memory in older adults. Gerontology, 52, 124130.Google Scholar
Anstey, K.J., Dain, S., Andrews, S., & Drobny, J. (2002). Visual abilities in older adults explain age-differences in Stroop and fluid intelligence but not face recognition: Implications for the vision-cognition connection. Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition, 9, 253265.Google Scholar
Anstey, K.J., Dear, K., Christensen, H., & Jorm, A.F. (2005). Biomarkers, health, lifestyle and demographic variables as correlates of reaction time performance in early, middle and late adulthood. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58A, 521.Google Scholar
Anstey, K.J., Hofer, S.M., & Luszcz, M.A. (2003). A latent growth curve analysis of late-life sensory and cognitive function over 8 years: Evidence for specific and common factors underlying change. Psychology and Aging, 18, 714726.Google Scholar
Anstey, K.J. & Luszcz, M.A. (2002). Selective non-response to clinical assessment in the longitudinal study of aging: Implications for estimating population levels of cognitive function and dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 704709.Google Scholar
Anstey, K.J., Luszcz, M.A., & Sanchez, L. (2001). Two-year decline in vision but not hearing is associated with memory decline in very old adults in a population-based sample. Gerontology, 47, 289293.Google Scholar
Anstey, K.J. & Smith, G.A. (1999). Interrelationships among biological markers of aging, health, activity, acculturation, and cognitive performance in late adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 14, 605618.Google Scholar
Bäckman, L. & Forsell, Y. (1994). Episodic memory functioning in a community-based sample of old adults with major depression: Utilization of cognitive support. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 361370.Google Scholar
Ball, K., Berch, D.B., Helmers, K.F., Jobe, J.B., Leveck, M.D., Marsiske, M., Morris, J.N., Rebok, G.W., Smith, D.M., Tennstedt, S.L., Unverzagt, F.W., & Willis, S.L. (2002). Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 288, 22712281.Google Scholar
Benton-Sivan, A. (1992). The Benton Visual Retention Test (5th ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation.
Christensen, H., Mackinnon, A.J., Korten, A., & Jorm, A.F. (2001). The “common cause hypothesis” of cognitive aging: Evidence for not only a common factor but also specific associations of age with vision and grip strength in a cross-sectional analysis. Psychology and Aging, 16, 588599.Google Scholar
Congdon, N., Vingerling, J.R., Klein, B.E., West, S., Friedman, D.S., Kempen, J., O'Colmain, B., Wu, S.Y., Taylor, H.R., & Eye Diseases Prevalence Research Group. (2004). Prevalence of cataract and pseudophakia/aphakia among adults in the United States. Archives of Ophthalmology, 122, 487494.Google Scholar
Dickinson, C.M. & Rabbitt, P.M.A. (1991). Simulated visual impairment: Effects on text comprehension and reading speed. Clinical Vision Science, 6, 301308.Google Scholar
Drobny, J., Anstey, K.J., & Andrews, S. (2005). Visual memory testing in older adults with age-related visual decline: A measure of memory or visual functioning? Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27, 425435.Google Scholar
Elliott, D.B., Patla, A.E., Furniss, M., & Adkin, A. (2000). Improvements in clinical and functional vision and quality of life after second eye cataract surgery. Optometry and Vision Science, 77, 1324.Google Scholar
Erixon-Lindroth, N., Farde, L., Wahlin, T.-B.R., Sovago, J., Halldin, C., & Bäckman, L. (2005). The role of the striatal dopamine transporter in cognitive aging. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 138, 112.Google Scholar
Ferraro, F.R., Bang, B.J., & Scheuler, K. (2002a). Visual degradation in Boston Naming Test performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95, 11151118.Google Scholar
Ferraro, F.R., Grossman, J., Bren, A., & Hoverson, A. (2002b). Effects of orientation on Rey Complex Figure performance. Brain and Cognition, 50, 139144.Google Scholar
Harwood, R.H., Foss, A.J., Osborn, F., Gregson, R.M., Zaman, A., & Masud, T. (2005). Falls and health status in elderly women following first eye cataract surgery: A randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Ophthalmology, 89, 5359.Google Scholar
Hofer, S.M. & Sliwinski, M.J. (2001). Understanding ageing. An evaluation of research designs for assessing the interdependence of ageing-related changes. Gerontology, 47, 341352.Google Scholar
Horn, J.L. (1982). The aging of human abilities. In B.B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of developmental psychology (pp. 847869). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Hultsch, D.F., Hertzog, C., Small, B.J., & Dixon, R.A. (1999). Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging, 14, 245263.Google Scholar
Li, S.C., Lindenberger, U., & Sikstrom, S. (2001). Aging cognition: From neuromodulation to representation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 479486.Google Scholar
Lindenberger, U. & Baltes, P.B. (1994). Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: A strong connection. Psychology and Aging, 9, 339355.Google Scholar
Lindenberger, U. & Potter, U. (1998). The complex nature of unique and shared effects in hierarchical linear regression: Implications for developmental psychology. Psychological Methods, 3, 218230.Google Scholar
Lindenberger, U., Scherer, H., & Baltes, P.B. (2001). The strong connection between sensory and cognitive performance in old age: Not due to sensory acuity reductions operating during cognitive assessment. Psychology and Aging, 16, 196205.Google Scholar
Lindenberger, U., Singer, T., & Baltes, P.B. (2002). Longitudinal selectivity in aging populations: Separating mortality-associated versus experimental components in the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Journals of Gerontology, Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, P474482.Google Scholar
Lundstrom, M., Stenevi, U., & Thorburn, W. (2001). Quality of life after first- and second-eye cataract surgery: Five-year data collected by the Swedish national cataract register. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 27, 15531559.Google Scholar
Raven, J.C. (1940). Matrix tests. Mental Health, 1, 1018.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A. (1994). Aging associations: Influence of speed on adult age differences in associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 14861503.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T.A., Hancock, H.E., Meinz, E.J., & Hambrick, D.Z. (1996). Interrelations of age, visual acuity, and cognitive functioning. Journals of Gerontology, Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 51, P317330.Google Scholar
Schneider, B. & Pichora-Fuller, M. (2000). Implications of perceptual deterioration for cognitive ageing research. In The handbook of aging and cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tamura, H., Tsukamoto, H., Mukai, S., Kato, T., Minamoto, A., Ohno, Y., Yamashita, H., & Mishima, H.K. (2004). Improvement in cognitive impairment after cataract surgery in elderly patients. Journal of Cataract and Refractive Surgery, 30, 598602.Google Scholar
Valentijn, S.A., van Boxtel, M.P., van Hooren, S.A., Bosma, H., Beckers, H.J., Ponds, R.W., & Jolles, J. (2005). Change in sensory functioning predicts change in cognitive functioning: Results from a 6-year follow-up in the Maastricht aging study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 374380.Google Scholar
van Boxtel, M.P., ten Tusscher, M.P., Metsemakers, J.F., Willems, B., & Jolles, J. (2001). Visual determinants of reduced performance on the Stroop color-word test in normal aging individuals. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 620627.Google Scholar
von Gunten, A., Giannakopoulos, P., Bouras, C., & Hof, P.R. (2004). Neuropathological changes in visuospatial systems in Alzheimer's disease. In Vision in Alzheimer's disease (pp. 3061). Basel: Karger.
Warrington, E.K. (1984). Recognition Memory Test. Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.
Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised manual. New York: Psychological Corporation.
Witkovsky, P. (2004). Dopamine and retinal function. Documenta Ophthalmologica, 108, 1740.Google Scholar