Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T10:48:58.890Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developmental disturbance of access to biographical information and people's names: A single-case study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2009

Martial van der Linden
Affiliation:
University of Liège, Neuropsychology Unit, Department of Psychology, Liège, Belgium
Serge Brédart
Affiliation:
University of Liège, Neuropsychology Unit, Department of Psychology, Liège, Belgium
Myriam Schweich
Affiliation:
University of Louvain, Cognitive Neuropsychology Unit, Department of Experimental Psychology, Louvain-la-Neuve, Belgium

Abstract

The paper describes the case of a person (GB) without any clinical evidence of cerebral disease who showed a specific impairment in the retrieval of biographical information, including names, about famous people. This deficit appeared while GB scored normally in different long-term memory tasks, and in object naming tasks. Moreover, he showed no impairment in the structural encoding and the recognition of faces. His specific impairment is interpreted both in terms of Bruce and Young's (1986) functional model of person recognition and in terms of the more recent Burton et al. (1990) interactive activation version of the Bruce and Young model. (JINS, 1995, 7, 589–595.)

Type
Case Study
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bruce, V. & Young, A.W. (1986). Understanding face recognition. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 305327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruyer, R. & Schweich, M. (1991). A clinical test battery of face processing. International Journal of Neuroscience, 61, 1930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buschke, H. & Fuld, P.A. (1974). Evaluating storage, retention, and retrieval in disordered memory and learning. Neurology, 24, 10191025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, A.M. & Bruce, V. (1993). Naming faces and naming names: Exploring an interactive activation model of person recognition. Memory, 1, 457480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, A.M., Bruce, V., & Johnston, R.A. (1990). Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 361380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, R. & De Haan, E. (1993). Developmental prosopagnosia: A functional analysis and implications for remediation. In Riddoch, M.J. & Humphreys, G.W. (Eds.), Cognitive neuropsychology and cognitive rehabilitation (pp. 77101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carney, R. & Temple, C.M. (1993). Prosopanomia? A possible category-specific anomia for faces. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 185195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Haan, E.H.F. & Campbell, R. (1991). A fifteen year follow-up of a case of developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex, 27, 489509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, H.D. (1986). Processing underlying face recognition. In Bruyer, R., (Ed.), The neuropsychology of face perception and facial expression. Hillsdale NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fery, P., Vincent, E., & Brédart, S. (1995). Personal name anomia: A single case study. Cortex, 31, 191198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hittmair-Delazer, M., Denes, G., Semenza, C., & Mantovan, M.C. (1994). Anomia for people's names. Neuropsychologia, 32, 465476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucchelli, F. & De Renzi, E. (1992). Proper name anomia. Cortex, 28, 221230.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConachie, H.R. (1976). Developmental prosopagnosia. A single case report. Cortex, 12, 7682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenna, P. & Warrington, E. (1980). Testing for nominal dysphasia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 43, 781788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obler, L. & Fein, D. (1988). The exceptional brain. Neuropsychology of talent and special abilities. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Semenza, C. & Zettin, M. (1988). Generating proper names: A case of selectice inability. Cognitive Psychology, 5, 711721.Google Scholar
Semenza, C. & Zettin, M. (1989). Evidence from aphasia for the role of proper names as pure referring expressions. Nature, 342, 678679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snoodgras, J.G. & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174215.Google Scholar
Temple, C.R.M. (1992). A case of developmental prosopagnosia. In Campbell, R. (Ed.), Mental lives: Case studies in cognition (pp. 201215). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Valentine, T., Brédart, S., Lavwson, R., & Ward, G. (1991). What's in a name? Access to information from people's names. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 3, 147176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van der Linden, M., Wyns, C.H., Coyette, F., Von Frenckell, R., & Seron, X. (1989). Un questionnaire d’auto-évaluation de la mémoire (Q.A.M.), Brussels: Editest.Google Scholar
Violon, A. & Wyns, Ch. (1984). Manuel du test de perception et d’apprentissage progressif en mémoire visuelle: “La ruche.” Châtelineau: Editions L’application des Techniques Modernes.Google Scholar
Warrington, E.K. (1984). Recognition memory test. Windson: NFER-Nelson.Google Scholar