Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T05:22:25.841Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Confabulations are emotionally charged, but not always for the best

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 August 2010

ANA BAJO*
Affiliation:
King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry, London, United Kingdom Brain Injury Rehabilitation Unit, Edgware Community hospital, London, United Kingdom
SIMON FLEMINGER
Affiliation:
King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry, London, United Kingdom Lishman Brain Injury Unit, Maudsley Hospital, London, United Kingdom
MICHAEL KOPELMAN
Affiliation:
King’s College London, Institute of Psychiatry, London, United Kingdom
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Ana Bajo, Brain Injury Rehabilitation Unit (BIRU), Edgware Community Hospital, Edgware, MIDDX HA8 0AD United Kingdom. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

There is disagreement regarding the underlying basis of confabulation and, in particular, whether emotional mechanisms influence the presence or the content of confabulations. In this study, we have examined the emotional content of confabulations and “true” memories given by 24 memory-disordered patients on two autobiographical memory tasks. Two judges made pleasant/neutral/unpleasant ratings. Although many of the “memories” were evaluated as “neutral”, there was an enhanced level of statements rated as having affective content (either pleasant or unpleasant) amongst these patients’ confabulations, compared with their “true” memories. This affective bias was present irrespective of whether patients had suffered focal pathology extending to the ventro-medial frontal cortex (VMFC) or other pathology. There was also a correlation between participants’ self-evaluated mood-states and both true and false memories’ affective content, suggestive of a mood congruency effect in both types of memory. In summary, there was an enhanced tendency to produce memories with affective content (pleasant and unpleasant) amongst confabulations (whether or not there was VMFC pathology). The affective content of both confabulations and true memories produced may relate, in part, to an individual’s current mood-state. (JINS, 2010, 16, 975–983.)

Type
Symposia
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Burgess, P.W., & McNeil, J.E. (1999). Content-specific confabulation. Cortex, 35, 163182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, P.W., & Shallice, T. (1996). Confabulation and the control of recollection. Memory, 4, 359411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, P.W., & Shallice, T. (1997). The Hayling and Brixton Tests. Test manual. Bury St Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company Ltd.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., & Turner, M. (2009). Confabulation and delusion. In Hirstein, W. (Ed.), Confabulation. Views from neuroscience, psychiatry, psychology, and philosophy. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Conway, M.A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C.W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, M.A., & Tacchi, P.C. (1996). Motivated confabulation. Neurocase, 2, 325338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalla Barba, G. (1993). Confabulation: Knowledge and recollective experience. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 10, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalla Barba, G., & Boissé, M.F. (2010). Temporal consciousness and confabulation: Is the medial temporal lobe “temporal”? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15, 95117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalla Barba, G., Cappelletti, J.Y., Signorini, M., & Denes, G. (1997). Confabulation: Remembering ’another’ past, planning ’another’ future. Neurocase, 3, 425435.Google Scholar
DeLuca, J. (2009). Confabulation in anterior communicating artery syndrome. In Hirstein, W. (Ed.), Confabulation. Views from neuroscience, psychiatry, psychology, and philosophy. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fotopoulou, A. (2008). False selves in neuropsychological rehabilitation: The challenge of confabulation. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 18, 541565.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fotopoulou, A. (2010). The affective neuropsychology of confabulation and delusion. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15, 3863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fotopoulou, A., Conway, M., Griffiths, P., Birchall, D., & Tyrer, S. (2007a). Self-enhancing confabulation: Revisiting the motivational hypothesis. Neurocase, 13, 615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fotopoulou, A., Conway, M.A., & Solms, M. (2007b). Confabulation: Motivated reality monitoring. Neuropsychologia, 45, 21802190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fotopoulou, A., Conway, M.A., Solms, M., Tyrer, S., & Kopelman, M. (2008a). Self-serving confabulation in prose recall. Neuropsychologia, 46, 14291441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fotopoulou, A., Conway, M.A., Tyrer, S., Birchall, D., Griffiths, P., & Solms, M. (2008b). Is the content of confabulation positive? An experimental study. Cortex, 44, 764772.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fotopoulou, A., Solms, M., & Turnbull, O. (2004). Wishful reality distortions in confabulation: A case report. Neuropsychologia, 42, 727744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilboa, A., Alain, C., Stuss, D.T., Melo, B., Miller, S., & Moscovitch, M. (2006). Mechanisms of spontaneous confabulations: A strategic retrieval account. Brain, 129, 13991414.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilboa, A., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of confabulation: A review and a model. In Baddeley, A., Kopelman, M.D., & Wilson, B.A. (Eds.), The handbook of memory disorders. Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Johnson, M.K., & Raye, C.L. (2000). Cognitive and brain mechanisms of false memories and beliefs. In Schacter, D.L. & Scarry, E. (Eds.), Memory, brain, and belief. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kopelman, M.D. (1987). Two types of confabulation. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 50, 14821487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopelman, M.D. (1999). Varieties of false memory. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 16, 197214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopelman, M.D. (2010). Varieties of confabulation and delusion. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15, 1437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kopelman, M.D., Ng, N., & Van Den Brouke, O. (1997). Confabulation extending across episodic, personal, and general semantic memory. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 683712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopelman, M.D., Wilson, B.A., & Baddeley, A. (1990). The autobiographical memory interview. Bury St Edmunds, UK: Thames Valley Test Company Ltd.Google Scholar
Korsakoff, S.S. (1891). Erinnerungstäuschungen (Pseudoreminiscenzen) bei polyneuritischer Psychose. Allegemeine Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie und psychisch-gerichtliche Medicin, 47, 390410.Google Scholar
Landis, J.R., & Koch, G.G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metcalf, K. (2006). The origin and nature of confabulation: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective. PhD Thesis. Macquarie University (Australia).Google Scholar
Metcalf, K., Langdon, R., & Coltheart, M. (2010). The role of personal biases in the explanation of confabulation. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 15, 6494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, H., & Willison, J. (1991). National Adult Reading Test (NART). Test Manual. (2nd ed.). Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON Publishing Company Ltd.Google Scholar
Reitan, R.M. (1958). Validity of the Trail Making Test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 8, 271276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnider, A. (2003). Spontaneous confabulation and the adaptation of thought to ongoing reality. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 662671.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnider, A. (2008). The confabulating mind. How the brain creates reality. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnider, A., von Daniken, C., & Gutbrod, K. (1996). The mechanisms of spontaneous and provoked confabulations. Brain, 119, 13651375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T., & Evans, M.E. (1978). The involvement of the frontal lobes in cognitive estimation. Cortex, 14, 294303.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toosy, A.T., Burbridge, S.E., Pitkanen, M., Loyal, A.S., Akanuma, N., Laing, H., et al. . (2008). Functional imaging correlates of fronto–temporal dysfunction in Morvan’s syndrome. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 79, 734735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turnbull, O., Jenkins, S., & Rowley, M.L. (2004). The pleasantness of false beliefs: An emotion-based account of confabulation. Neuro-Psychoanalysis, 6, 516.Google Scholar
Turner, M.S., Cipolotti, L., Yousry, T.A., & Shallice, T. (2008). Confabulation: Damage to a specific inferior medial prefrontal system. Cortex, 44, 637648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, M.S., Simons, J.S., Gilbert, S.J., Frith, C.D., & Burgess, P.W. (2008). Distinct roles for lateral and medial rostral prefrontal cortex in source monitoring of perceived and imagined events. Neuropsychologia, 46, 14421453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, W.R., Skowronski, J.J., & Thompson, C.P. (2003). Life is pleasant-and memory helps to keep it that way! Review of General Psychology, 7, 203210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1999). WASI. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Manual. London: The Psychological Corporation Ltd.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D., Wycherley, R.J., Benjamin, L., Crawford, J., & Mockler, D. (1997). WMS-III UK. Wechsler Memory Scale - Third Edition. Administration and scoring manual. London: The Psychological Corporation Ltd.Google Scholar
Zigmond, A.S., & Snaith, R.P. (1983). The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 361370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed