Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T21:06:49.552Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attentional networks efficiency in preterm children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 October 2009

R. PIZZO*
Affiliation:
Child Clinical Neuropsychology Unit, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
S. URBEN
Affiliation:
Child Clinical Neuropsychology Unit, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
M. VAN DER LINDEN
Affiliation:
Cognitive Psychopathology and Neuropsychology Unit, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
C. BORRADORI-TOLSA
Affiliation:
Division of Child Development and Growth, Department of Pediatrics, University of Geneva Medical School, Geneva, Switzerland
M. FRESCHI
Affiliation:
Division of Child Development and Growth, Department of Pediatrics, University of Geneva Medical School, Geneva, Switzerland
M. FORCADA-GUEX
Affiliation:
Division of Neonatology, Department of Pediatrics, University Hospital, Lausanne, Switzerland
P. HÜPPI
Affiliation:
Division of Child Development and Growth, Department of Pediatrics, University of Geneva Medical School, Geneva, Switzerland
K. BARISNIKOV
Affiliation:
Child Clinical Neuropsychology Unit, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
*
*Correspondence and reprint requests to: Roxane Pizzo, Child Clinical Neuropsychology Unit, FPSE, University of Geneva, 40 bd Pont-d’Arve, 1211 Geneva, Switzerland. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Recent studies have reported specific executive and attentional deficits in preterm children. However, the majority of this research has used multidetermined tasks to assess these abilities, and the interpretation of the results lacks an explicit theoretical backdrop to better understand the origin of the difficulties observed. In the present study, we used the Child Attention Network Task (Child ANT; Rueda et al. 2004) to assess the efficiency of the alerting, orienting and executive control networks. We compared the performance of 25 preterm children (gestational age ≤ 32 weeks) to 25 full-term children, all between 5½ and 6½ years of age. Results showed that, as compared to full-term children, preterm children were slower on all conditions of the Child ANT and had a specific deficit in executive control abilities. We also observed a significantly higher correlation between the orienting and executive control networks in the preterm group, suggesting less differentiation of these two networks in this population. (JINS, 2010, 16, 130–137.)

Type
Research Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The International Neuropsychological Society 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Anderson, P.J. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 7182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, P.J., Doyle, L.W.; Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group. (2003). Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children who were born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight in the 1990s. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 32643272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Assel, M.A., Landry, S.H., Swank, P., Smith, K.E., & Steelman, L.M. (2003). Precursors to mathematical skills: Examining the roles of visual–spatial skills, executive processes, and parenting factors. Journal of Applied Developmental Science, 7, 2738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ayward, J.P. (2002). Cognitive and neuropsychological outcomes: More than IQ scores. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 8, 234240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayless, S., & Stevenson, J. (2007). Executive functions in school-age children born very prematurely. Early Human Development, 83, 247254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berger, A., Jones, L., Rothbart, M.K., & Posner, M.I. (2000). Computerized games to study the development of attention in childhood. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 32, 297303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhutta, A.T., Cleves, P.H., Cradock, M.M., & Anand, K.J. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm. Journal of the American Medical Association, 288, 728737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Böhm, B., Katz-Salamon, M., Lagercrantz, H., & Forssberg, H. (2002). Developmental risks and protective factors for influencing cognitive outcome at 5 1/2 years of age in very-low-birthweight children. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44, 508516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, N., Chilcoat, H., DelDotto, J., Andreski, P., & Brown, G. (1996). Low birth weight and neurocognitive status at six years of age. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 389397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, R., Espy, K.E., & Wiebe, S.A. (2008). Short-term memory, working memory and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years. Developmental Neuropsychology, 33, 205228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Frias, C.M., Dixon, R.A., & Strauss, E. (2006). Structure of four executive functioning tests in healthy older adults. Neuropsychology, 20, 206214.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deforge, H., André, M., Hascoët, J.-M., Toniolo, A.-M., Demange, V., & Fresson, J. (2006). Cognitive development and attention performances at school age of “normal” prematurely born children. Archives de Pédiatrie, 13, 11951201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M., Francis, D.J., Cirino, P.T., Schachar, R., Barnes, M.A., & Fletcher, J.M. (2009). Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 15, 331343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edgin, J., Inder, T.E., Anderson, P.J., Hood, K.M., Clark, C.A., & Woodward, L.J. (2008). Executive functioning in preschool children born very preterm: Relationship with early white matter pathology. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 14, 90101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 665697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I.K., Fabes, R.A., Shepard, S., Losoya, S., Murphy, B.C., et al. (2000). Prediction of elementary school children’s externalizing problem behaviors from attentional and behavioral regulation and negative emotionality. Child Development, 71, 13671382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elgen, I., Lundervold, A.J., & Sommerfeld, K. (2004). Aspects of inattention in low birth weight children. Pediatric Neurology, 30, 9298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espy, K.A., McDiarmid, M.D., Cwik, M.F., Meade Stalets, M., Hamby, A., & Senn, T.E. (2004). The contribution of the executive functions to emergent mathematic skill in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 26, 465486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Espy, K.A., Stalets, M.M., McDiarmid, M.M., Senn, T.E., Cwik, M.F., & Hamby, A. (2002). Executive functions in preschool children born preterm: Application of cognitive neuroscience paradigms. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 8392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Yanghong, W., & Posner, M.I. (2003). Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain activity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences United States of America, 100, 74067411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Fossella, J., Flombaum, J.I., & Posner, M.I. (2005). The activation of attentional networks. NeuroImage, 26, 471479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M.I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340347.Google ScholarPubMed
Fan, J., Wu, J., Fossella, J., & Posner, M.I. (2001). Assessing the heritability of attentional networks. Neuroscience, 2, 1420.Google ScholarPubMed
Fernandez-Duque, D., & Posner, M.I. (2001). Brain imaging of attentional networks in normal and pathological states. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23, 7493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, N.P., Miyake, A., Corley, R.P., Young, S.E., De Fries, J.C., & Hewitt, J.K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17, 172179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, J.M., O’Callaghan, M.J., & Mohay, H. (1999). Executive function of children with extremely low birthweight: A case control study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 292297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hrabok, M., Kerns, K.A., & Müller, U. (2006). The vigilance, orienting, and executive attention networks in 4-year-old children. Child Neuropsychology, 13, 408421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (1993). Batterie pour l’examen psychologique de l’enfant. Paris: ECPA.Google Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (2003). NEPSY: Bilan neuropsychologique de l’enfant-manuel: Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.Google Scholar
Largo, R.H., Pfister, D., Mohnari, L., Kundu, S., Lipp, A., & Due, G. (1989). Significance of prenatal, perinatal and posnatal factors in the development of AGA preterm infant at five to seven years. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 31, 440456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leclercq, V., Jambaqué, I., Picard, A., Bricout, L., & Siéroff, E. (2006). Trouble du contrôle attentionnel et prématuré. Revue de Neuropsychologie, 16, 4164.Google Scholar
Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., & Robertson, I.H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42, 10651081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosarti, C., Giouroukou, E., Micali, N., Rifkin, L., Morris, R.G., & Murray, R.M. (2007). Impaired executive functioning in young adults born very preterm. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 571581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M.I., & Perterson, J. (1990). The attention system of human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 2542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M.I., & Rothbart, M.K. (2000). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 427441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rickard, A.L., Kelly, E.A., Doyle, L.W., & Callanan, C. (2001). Cognition, academic progress, behavior and self-concept at 14 years of very low birth weight. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 22, 1118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridderinkhof, K.R., van der Molen, M.W., Band, G.P., & Bashore, T.R. (1997). Sources of interference from irrelevant information: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 315341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rueda, M.R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B.D., Halparin, J.D., Gruber, D.B., Lercari, L.P., et al. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42, 10291040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shum, D., Neulinger, K., O’Callaghan, M., & Mohay, H. (2008). Attentional problems in children born very preterm or with extremely low birth weight at 7-9 years. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 103112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snyder, E., Davis, D.W., Burns, B., & Robinson, J. (2007). Examining attention networks in preschool children born with very low birth weights. Journal of Early Childhood and Infant Psychology, 26, 611626.Google Scholar
Spieler, D.H., Balota, D.A., & Faust, M.E. (1996). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 22, 461479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamm, L., McCandliss, B.D., Liang, A., Wigal, T.L., Posner, M.I., & Swanson, J.M. (in press). Can attention itself be trained? Attention training of children at-risk for ADHD. In McBurnett, K. (Ed.), Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A 21st Century Perspective. New York: Marcel Dekker.Google Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Hack, M., & Klein, N.K. (1998). Attention deficits in children with <750gr birth weight. Child Neuropsychology, 4, 2134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, H.G., Minich, N.M., Klein, N., & Hack, M. (2004). Longitudinal outcomes of very low birth weight: Neuropsychological findings. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 149163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van de Weijer-Bergsma, E., Wijnroks, L., & Jongmans, M.J. Attention development in infants and preschool children born preterm: A review. Infant Behavior and Development, 31, 333351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimmerman, D.W. (1986). Tests of Significance of Correlation Coefficients in the Absence of Bivariate Normal Populations. Journal of Experimental Education, 54, 223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar