Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T14:50:23.355Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Differential impairment in recognition of emotion across different media in people with severe traumatic brain injury

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 July 2005

SKYE MCDONALD
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia
JENNIFER CLARE SAUNDERS
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of New South Wales, Sydney, Australia

Abstract

Recent evidence suggests that there may be dissociable systems for recognizing emotional expressions from different media including audio and visual channels, and still versus moving displays. In this study, 34 adults with severe traumatic brain injuries (TBI) and 28 adults without brain injuries were assessed for their capacity to recognize emotional expressions from dynamic audiovisual displays, conversational tone alone, moving facial displays, and still photographs. The TBI group were significantly impaired in their interpretation of both audio and audiovisual displays. In addition, eight of the 34 were significantly impaired in their capacity to recognize still facial expressions. In contrast, only one individual was impaired in the recognition of moving visual displays. Information processing speed was not found to play a significant role in producing problems with dynamic emotional expression. Instead the results suggest that visual moving displays may enlist different brain systems to those engaged with still displays, for example, the parietal cortices. Problems with the processing of affective prosody, while present, were not clearly related to other emotion processing problems. While this may attest to the independence of the auditory affective system, it may also reflect problems with the dual demands of listening to conversational meaning and affective tone. (JINS, 2005, 11, 392–399.)

Type
Research Article
Copyright
© 2005 The International Neuropsychological Society

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adams, J.H., Doyle, D., Ford, I., Graham, D.I., McGee, M., & McLellan, D.R. (1989). Brain damage in fatal non-missile head injury in relation to age and type of injury. Scottish Medical Journal, 34(1), 399401.Google Scholar
Adams, J.H., Doyle, D., Graham, D.I., Lawrence, A.E., McLellan, D.R., Gennarelli, T.A., Pastuszko, M., & Sakamoto, T. (1985). The contusion index: A reappraisal in human and experimental non-missile head injury. Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology, 11(4), 299308.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R. (2002). Neural systems for recognizing emotion. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 12(2), 169177.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., & Tranel, D. (2002). Neural systems for recognition of emotional prosody: A 3-D lesion study. Emotion, 2(1), 2351.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Cooper, G., & Damasio, A.R. (2000). A role for the somatosensory cortices in the visual recognision of emotions as revealed by three dimensional lesion mapping. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 26832690.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A.R. (1996). Cortical systems for the recognition of emotion in facial expressions. Journal of Neuroscience, 16(23), 76787687.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R., Russell, J.A., & Tranel, D. (1999). A role for the human amygdala in recognizing emotional arousal from unpleasant stimuli. Psychological Science, 10(2), 167171.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R. & Tranel, D. (2004). Impaired judgments of sadness but not happiness following bilateral amygdala damage. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(3), 453462.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, A.R. (2003). Dissociable neural systems for recognizing emotions. Brain and Cognition, 52(1), 6169.Google Scholar
Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2001). Emotion recognition from faces and prosody following temporal lobectomy. Neuropsychology, 15(3), 396404.Google Scholar
Angrilli, A., Palomba, D., Cantagallo, A., Maietti, A., & Stegagno, L. (1999). Emotional impairment after right orbitofrontal lesion in a patient without cognitive deficits. Neuroreport, 10(8), 17411746.Google Scholar
Bassili, J.N. (1978). Facial motion in the perception of faces and of emotional expression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 373379.Google Scholar
Borod, J.C. (1993). Cerebral mechanisms underlying facial, prosodic and lexical emotional expression: A review of neuropsychological studies and methodological issues. Neuropsychology, 7(4), 445463.Google Scholar
Borod, J.C., Koff, E., Lorch, M.P., & Nicholas, M. (1986). The expression and perception of facial emotion in brain-damaged patients. Neuropsychologia, 24(2), 169180.Google Scholar
Breitenstein, C., Daum, I., & Ackermann, H. (1998). Emotional processing following cortical and subcortical brain damage: Contribution of the fronto-striatal circuitry. Behavioural Neurology, 11(1), 2942.Google Scholar
Broks, P., Young, A.W., Maratos, E., Coffey, P.J., Calder, A.J., Isaac, C.L., Mayes, A.R., Hodges, J.R., Montaldi, D., Cezaylirli, E., Roberts, N., & Hadley, D. (1998). Face processing impairments after encephalitis: Amygdala damage and recognition of fear. Neuropsychologia, 36(1), 5970.Google Scholar
Brooks, N., Campsie, L., Symington, C., Beattie, A., & Campsie, L. (1987). The effects of severe head injury on patient and relative within seven years of injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 2(3), 113.Google Scholar
Buchanan, T.W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2004). Anteromedial temporal lobe damage blocks startle modulation by fear and disgust. Behavioral Neuroscience, 118(2), 429437.Google Scholar
Cicone, M., Wapner, W., & Gardener, H. (1980). Sensitivity to emotional expressions and situations in organic patients. Cortex, 16, 145158.Google Scholar
Courville, C.B. (1945). Pathology of the nervous system (2nd ed.). Mountain View, California: California Pacific Press.
Croker, V. & McDonald, S. (in press). Recognition of emotion from facial expression following traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury.
Eckman, P. & Freisen, W.V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect. Palo Alto, California: Consulting Psychological Press.
Green, R.E.A., Turner, G.R., & Thompson, W.F. (2004). Deficits in facial emotion perception in adults with recent traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychologia, 42, 133141.Google Scholar
Hornak, J., Rolls, E.T., & Wade, D. (1996). Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage. Neuropsychologia, 34(4), 247261.Google Scholar
Humphrey, G.W., Donnelly, N., & Riddoch, M.J. (1993). Expression is computed separately from facial identity and is computed separately for moving and static faces: Neuropsychological evidence. Neuropsychologia, 31, 173181.Google Scholar
Jackson, H.F. & Moffat, N.J. (1987). Impaired emotional recognition following severe head injury. Cortex, 23, 293300.Google Scholar
Levin, H.S., High, W.M., Goethe, K.E., Sisson, R.A., Overall, J.E., Rhoades, H.M., Eisenberg, H.M., Kalisky, Z., & Gary, H.E. (1987). The neuro-behavioural rating scale: assessment of the behavioural sequalae of head injury by the clinician. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 50, 183193.Google Scholar
Lezak, M.D. (1978). Living with the characterologically altered brain-injured patient. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 592598.Google Scholar
Marquardt, T.P., Rios-Brown, M., Richburg, T., Seibert, L.K., & Cannito, M.P. (2001). Comprehension and expression of affective sentences in traumatic brain injury. Aphasiology, 15(10–11), 10911101.Google Scholar
McDonald, S. & Flanagan, S. (2004). Social perception deficits after Traumatic Brain Injury: The interaction between emotion recognition, mentalising ability and social communication. Neuropsychology, 18, 572579.Google Scholar
McDonald, S., Flanagan, S., & Rollins, J. (2002). The Awareness of Social Inference Test. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation: Harcourt Assessment.
McDonald, S., Flanagan, S., Rollins, J., Kinch, J. (2003). TASIT: A new clinical tool for assessing social perception after traumatic brain injury. Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation, 18, 219238.Google Scholar
McDonald, S. & Pearce, S. (1996). Clinical insights into pragmatic language theory: The case of sarcasm. Brain and Language, 53, 81104.Google Scholar
McHugo, G.J. & Smith, C.A. (1996). The power of faces: A review of John Lanzetta's research on facial expression and emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 20, 85120.Google Scholar
McMordie, W.R., Barker, S.L., & Paolo, T.M. (1990). Return to work (RTW) after head injury. Brain Injury, 4, 5790.Google Scholar
Milders, M., Fuchs, S., & Crawford, J.R. (2003). Neuropsychological impairments and changes in emotional and social behaviour following severe traumatic brain injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25(2), 157172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearce, S., McDonald, S., & Coltheart, M. (1998). Ability to process ambiguous advertisements after frontal lobe damage. Brain and Cognition, 38, 150164.Google Scholar
Phillips, M.L., Drevets, W.C., Rauch, S.L., & Lane, R. (2003). Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Society of Biological Psychiatry, 54, 504514.Google Scholar
Ponsford, J.L., Olver, J.H., & Curran, C. (1995). A profile of outcome: 2 years after traumatic brain injury. Brain Injury, 9(1), 110.Google Scholar
Prigatano, G.P. & Pribram, K.H. (1982). Perception and memory of facial affect following brain injury. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 54, 859869.Google Scholar
Ross, E.D. & Mesulam, M.-M. (1979). Dominant language functions of the right hemisphere? Prosody and emotional gesturing. Archives of Neurology, 36(3), 144148.Google Scholar
Ross, E.D., Thompson, R.D., & Yenkosky, J. (1997). Lateralization of affective prosody in brain and the callosal integration of hemispheric language functions. Brain and Language, 56(1), 2754.Google Scholar
Spell, L.A. & Frank, E. (2000). Recognition of nonverbal communication of affect following traumatic brain injury. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 24(4), 285300.Google Scholar
Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A.W., Calder, A.J., Karnat, A., Lange, H., Homber, G.V., Perrett, D.I., & Rowland, D. (1996). Loss of disgust: Perception of faces and emotion in Huntingdon's disease. Brain, 119, 16471665.Google Scholar
Tate, R.L., Fenelon, B., Manning, M.L., & Hunter, M. (1991). Patterns of neuropsychological impairment after severe blunt head injury. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 179, 117126.Google Scholar
Tate, R.L., Lulham, J., Broe, T., Strettles, B., & Pfaff, A. (1989). Psychosocial outcome for the survivors of severe blunt head injury: The results from a consecutive series of 100 patients. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 52, 11281134.Google Scholar
Tranel, D., Bechara, A., & Denburg, N.L. (2002). Asymmetric functional roles of right and left ventromedial prefrontal cortices in social conduct, decision making and emotional processing. Cortex, 38(4), 589612.Google Scholar
van Zomeren, A.H. & Brouwer, W.H. (1987). Head injury and concepts of attention. In H.S. Levin, J. Grafman, & H.M. Eisenberg (Eds.), Neurobehavioural recovery from head injury (pp. 398415). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Wertz, R.T., Henschel, C.R., Auther, L.L., Ashford, J.R., & Kirshner, H.S. (1998). Affective prosodic disturbance subsequent to right hemisphere stroke: A clinical application. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 11(1–2), 89102.Google Scholar