Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T17:13:46.033Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Discrimination and Political Engagement: A Cross-national Test

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 October 2024

Randy Besco*
Affiliation:
University of Toronto, Canada

Abstract

What is the effect of personal discrimination on the political engagement of ethnic and racial minorities? Existing research theorizes increased engagement, but evidence is mixed. The discrimination and political engagement link is tested across six countries: Canada, Denmark, France, Germany, the United Kingdom, and the United States. Interest in politics and political actions (e.g. protest and donations) show constant relationships: people who have experienced discrimination have more interest in politics and take more political actions. There is no clear evidence of different effects of political vs social discrimination. However, the link between turnout and discrimination varies systematically across countries: a positive correlation in three separate American datasets, but mixed and null in other countries. This may be the result of the distinctive American conflict over voting rights for racial minorities. The conclusion discusses priorities for future research, including a focus on establishing causal relationships and testing mechanisms.

Type
Research Note
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the Race, Ethnicity, and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association

Discrimination against racial and ethnic minorities remains common in Western countries and has changed little in recent years (Lippens, Vermeiren, and Baert Reference Lippens, Vermeiren and Baert2023; Quillian et al. Reference Quillian, Pager, Hexel and Midtbøen2017) despite some evidence that attitudes have become more positive (Besco Reference Besco2021; Dennison and Geddes Reference Dennison and Geddes2019; Hopkins and Washington Reference Hopkins and Washington2020). Moreover, political and social conflict over immigration and racial issues seems to be growing, and norms excluding politicians who are explicitly racist and anti-immigrants are eroding (Koning Reference Koning2023; Tesler Reference Tesler2020; Valentino, Neuner, and Vandenbroek Reference Valentino, Neuner and Vandenbroek2018). While attitudes about race, immigration, and diversity are generally viewed as affecting party politics (Alonso and Fonseca Reference Alonso and da Fonseca2012; Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1982; Golder Reference Golder2016; Reny, Collingwood, and Valenzuela Reference Reny, Collingwood and Valenzuela2019), the reverse might also be true, as electoral politics such as the Brexit Referendum and election of Donald Trump (Forster Reference Forster2016; Lichtblau Reference Lichtblau2016) led to increased attacks and discrimination. While much effort is put in to understand the behavior and motives of racist voters and politicians, the impact and response of minorities is also crucial.

What is the effect of experiencing discrimination on the political engagement of racial minorities? We might hope that the response would be contestation and mobilization, rather than alienation or withdrawal. This is not guaranteed, given that some research suggests discrimination results in a lower sense of efficacy (Broman, Mavaddat, and Hsu Reference Broman, Mavaddat and Hsu2000) and disengagement from public visibility (Hobbs and Lajevardi Reference Hobbs and Lajevardi2019). Nonetheless, a substantial amount of research in political science, including theories of linked fate (Dawson Reference Dawson1994), group consciousness (Miller et al. Reference Miller, Gurin, Gurin and Malanchuk1981), and policy threat (Campbell Reference Campbell2003), suggest that the experience of discrimination will increase political engagement, mobilization, and participation.

Research examining the relationship between discrimination and political participation generally emphasizes that experience of discrimination makes people more likely to vote and be generally politically engaged. However, there have been many conflicting results, depending on the group (Lien Reference Lien1994), source of discrimination (Bilodeau Reference Bilodeau2017; Oskooii Reference Oskooii2016), or type of engagement (Lien Reference Lien1994; Martin Reference Martin2017; Mattila and Papageorgiou Reference Mattila and Papageorgiou2017; Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2005).

This study aims to test the link between discrimination and political participation across multiple contexts and data sources. To do so, it uses survey data from six countries: Canada, Denmark, France, Germany, the United Kingdom, and the United States. The relationship between the experience of discrimination and three political engagement measures is examined: interest in politics, turnout, and political actions (protesting, donations, petitions, etc). The effects of political and social discrimination are also tested separately. While the available measures and cross-sectional data produce some limitations, the strength of the analysis is the breadth of the test across multiple countries.

The results show clear and straightforward results for political interest and political actions. Across all countries, racial minorities who experience discrimination have higher political interest. Moreover, they report taking more political actions, such as protesting, participating in boycotts, donating to parties and politicians, and signing petitions. Conversely, the results for voter turnout vary substantially across countries, with mostly null or even negative correlations. The exception is in the United States, where three separate datasets show positive and statistically significant correlations: respondents with higher reported discrimination also report higher turnout. These turnout results point to distinctive dynamics in American politics and may be related to longstanding political conflict and mobilization around voting rights for racial minorities. Finally, separating political and social discrimination is somewhat inconsistent, but generally shows similar (positive) rather than different relationships with political engagement. A range of alternative specifications and robustness checks produce similar results for all these findings. We conclude with a discussion about future directions for this research area, emphasizing the need for improvement in behavioral measure and causal designs, and testing of theoretical explanations by examining mediators such as perceived responsibility and efficacy.

Discrimination and Political Participation

Research on the effects of discrimination or attacks on minority groups often finds that experiencing discrimination increases political participation. Lien (Reference Lien1994) found that experiencing discrimination was positively correlated with both voting and non-voting political engagement for Mexican Americans, and Wrinkle et al. (Reference Wrinkle, Stewart, Polinard, Meier and Arvizu1996) and Valdez (Reference Valdez2011) show similar results for non-electoral political engagement among Hispanic Americans. Examining the effect of discrimination on voting and attitudinal engagement, Schildkraut (Reference Schildkraut2005) shows that individual-level discrimination is related to higher levels of behavioral participation (voting and voter registration). Heath et al. (Reference Heath, Fisher, Rosenblatt, Sanders and Sobolewska2013) report a similar finding in the U.K.: personally experiencing discrimination is correlated with political action (protest, signing petitions, joining boycotts), and Martin (Reference Martin2017) similarly finds that perceptions of Islamophobia are correlated with protest among Muslims in the UK. Bilodeau (Reference Bilodeau2017) and Bilodeau et al. (Reference Bilodeau, White, Ma, Turgeon and Henderson2023) find that in Canada perceived discrimination is correlated with more protest and other forms of non-electoral participation. Perceived local discrimination increased political participation among Israeli-Arabs (Beeri and Saad Reference Beeri and Saad2014), and among immigrants to Germany, interest in politics is higher for those with perceived discrimination (Fischer-Neumann Reference Fischer-Neumann2014).

Importantly, there is emerging research which demonstrates these effects are causal. White (Reference White2016) uses the staggered implementation of an immigration enforcement policy and a difference-in-difference method to show this increases registration among Hispanic Americans. Weiss, Siegel, and Romney (Reference Weiss, Siegel and Romney2023), also using difference-in-difference analysis, find that threat of exclusionary citizenship rules mobilized participation among Palestinians. Besco et al. (Reference Besco, Sergio Garcia-Rios, Lajevardi, Oskooii and Tolley2022) use a survey experiment to show that racist campaign rhetoric increases turnout intentions among Hispanic Americans. Given the potential for endogeneity in cross-sectional measurements of self-reported discrimination and political engagement, the results from these causal designs provide some reassurance.

The theoretical basis for the effect of discrimination on political engagement supports both generalized and specific instances of discrimination, oppression, or threat. In generalized mechanisms, theories such as linked fate focus on how broadly defined oppression can politicize group identities and create a belief that the success or failure of minority citizens is connected to that of their fellow minority group members, which drives increased participation in politics (Chong and Rogers Reference Chong and Rogers2005; Dawson Reference Dawson1994; McClain et al. Reference McClain, Johnson Carew, Walton and Watts2009; Miller et al. Reference Miller, Gurin, Gurin and Malanchuk1981). Importantly, Lu and Jones (Reference Lu and Jones2019) showed that the link between experience of discrimination and perceptions of linked fate holds for a range of different minority groups, but for White people these relationships might be quite different (Marsh and Ramirez Reference Marsh and Ramrez2019). Grewal and Hamid (Reference Grewal and Hamid2022) show the converse effect: that Muslims facing discrimination are less supportive of democracy and the “system” if they feel they face this discrimination alone—e.g. without group consciousness or group discrimination.

Specific situations of discrimination can also matter: the policy threat literature theorizes that when people are threatened by policy changes their political participation tends to increase (Barreto et al. Reference Barreto, Manzano, Ramirez and Rim2008; Campbell Reference Campbell2003; Gutierrez et al. Reference Gutierrez, Ocampo, Barreto and Segura2019). This includes new immigration enforcement programs mobilizing Latino Americans (White Reference White2016) and the Patriot Act mobilizing (some) Arab Americans and (Cho, Gimpel and Wu Reference Cho, Gimpel and Wu2006) and Hispanic Americans. Weiss, Siegel, and Romney (Reference Weiss, Siegel and Romney2023) and Besco et al. (Reference Besco, Sergio Garcia-Rios, Lajevardi, Oskooii and Tolley2022) also test similar short-run effects of specific threats and find increases in political mobilization and engagement. Naturally, these general and specific effects can also intersect. For example, Pantoja, Ramirez, and Segura (Reference Pantoja, Ramirez and Segura2001) found that Latino immigrants socialized in a threatening anti-immigration context had high rates of voter registration and turnout, and Latinos with higher linked fate have stronger responses to threats like Donald Trump (Gutierrez et al. Reference Gutierrez, Ocampo, Barreto and Segura2019).

Examining the circumstances by which discrimination might, or might not, affect political engagement, Page (Reference Page2018) leverages the enlargement of the EU and argues that discrimination increases the participation of sexual minorities only if there is social and political support. Oskooii (Reference Oskooii2020, Reference Oskooii2016) theorizes that the type and source of discrimination: political discrimination (e.g. laws or by government officials) tends to increase political participation. However, societal discrimination (e.g. by individuals on the street or private businesses) does not.

However, dispite the above findings, there are multiple studies find conflicting and mixed results. Lien (Reference Lien1994) found that experiencing discrimination was positively correlated with both voting and non-voting political engagement for Mexican-Americans, but not Asian-Americans. Martin (Reference Martin2017) found that perceived Islamophobia increased non-electoral participation, yet decreased rates of voting. Bilodeau (Reference Bilodeau2017) also shows that discrimination is correlated with lower turnout among racial minorities in Canada, but increase in other forms of political activities. Besco et al. (Reference Besco, Sergio Garcia-Rios, Lajevardi, Oskooii and Tolley2022) find effects on political interest and turnout intention, but not on other political actions. Other types of groups also produce similarly mixed results: Mattila and Papageorgiou (Reference Mattila and Papageorgiou2017) show that perceived discrimination due to disability decreases voting but increases other political actions (protesting and contacting politicians). While Oskooii (Reference Oskooii2020, Reference Oskooii2016) argues that political discrimination increased participation and social discrimination does not, Bilodeau et al. (Reference Bilodeau, White, Ma, Turgeon and Henderson2023) find political and social discrimination have quite similar relationship with mobilization. Given these mixed results, this study aims to test these relationships across a range of datasets and countries.

Analysis and Results

This analysis is a multi-country test of the relationship between discrimination against racial and ethnic minorities and the political engagement of members of these groups. The purpose is to see if the same relationship holds in multiple contexts, replicating the analysis in countries that are established democracies but which have substantially different political systems, ethnic groups, and economic contexts. The major constraints were data availability: sufficient sample size of racial minorities, and questions about discrimination. While many surveys ask about discriminatory attitudes, surprisingly few ask about the experience of discrimination. Nonetheless, suitable surveys were found in Canada, Denmark, France, Germany, the United Kingdom, and the United States. These surveys all include multiple different ethnic and racial groups. Table 1 provides an overview, with additional details in the appendix in the Supplementary material.Footnote 1

Table 1. Surveys

The population of interest is racial and ethnic minorities. This category is sometimes defined in different ways, but in this research the definition used is non-White, of non-European ancestry. This is because the effects of experiencing discrimination for White people might be quite different (Marsh and Ramirez Reference Marsh and Ramrez2019). It might be reasonable to include other (White) European national groups since they plausibly experience discrimination in a similar way to ethnic minorities, but this causes significant difficulties for comparisons to the United States and Canada, so these respondents are not included here. In the Canadian, Danish, American, and British surveys, there are questions specifically about the race or ethnicity of the respondents. In the French and German surveys, a variable is constructed using questions about the country of origin of the respondent and their parents.

The key independent variable is personal experience of discrimination. Most of the surveys ask a version of the question “have you personally experienced discrimination due to your race or culture” (see Appendix in the Supplementary material for question-wording details). Some of the surveys ask a series of question about discrimination in various places: on the street, at the office, visiting the doctor, visiting authorities, by the police, in applying for apartments, etc. These are combined into an additive scale for a general discrimination measure so that respondents reporting discrimination in multiple places score higher. These questions also enable the construction of separate political and social discrimination variables (see Appendix in the Supplementary material for coding details).

For the dependant variables, three measures of political engagement are used: interest in politics, turnout, and political actions (protests, donations to parties, etc.). The research discussed above suggests that all three measures will be impacted in a similar way by personal experience of discrimination. While interest in politics is not precisely a measure of political participation, it can be viewed as a measure of psychological engagement with politics, and therefore another useful measure of our key concept. It is also a dependant variable in many of the studies cited above. Notably, interest in politics is not subject to regulations which vary across countries in the way that voting or some other political actions are, and so in that way, it is especially useful measure for cross-national tests. More information on this measure is provided in the sections below, and details of the wording and scales are available in the Appendix, Supplementary material.

Not all surveys include all questions, and there is some variation in the measures available, such as questions about experiencing discrimination in general vs in specific locations, or in the exact political actions asked about. To address this there is a series of tests, including comparing analysis with the same and different questions. These produce very similar results, suggesting that differences in question-wording are not a significant issue, and they capture the same latent variables. This is discussed at greater length in the section on robustness checks below. The demographic correlates are mostly consistent across countries and measures, with education positively correlated and immigrant status and age (except in the USA) negatively correlated with the discrimination measures. Other demographics (gender and income) don’t have a consistent relationship with discrimination. Crucially, there are no systematic differences in correlates related to the type of question. In the Canadian and UK datasets, which have both the general and specific measures of discrimination, the correlates of the two measures are the same, again suggesting they are tapping the same factor. Nonetheless, the specific features of different types of variables are likely to be different, such as range and precision. For example, a scale of questions asking about discrimination is likely to be more precise, but possibly an underestimate relative to a general question, since it can’t ask about all situations. For that reason, the analysis here is on the direction and statistical significance of the estimates, rather than on comparing the precise size of the coefficients.

Control variables include age, education, income, gender, and immigrant status. Note that the main models do not use controls for variables such as partisanship or other political attitudes, for several reasons: they are post-treatment, plausible mediators, alternative measure of the DV (interest), and tend to vary in availability across countries and surveys. Nonetheless, alternative models with additional controls are estimated and discussed in the section on robustness checks below. To make interpretation easier, all variables are scaled to a 0-1 interval except age and political actions, the latter being a count of actions.

Interest in Politics

The first set of models examines the relationship between discrimination and political interest. The dependant variable is interest in politics and the independent variable is personal experience of discrimination. Models are estimated both with and without controls: age, income, gender, and immigrant status, which should contribute to avoiding results differing due to variation in composition of the across countries.

The results in Figure 1 show quite consistent results: in all countries, discrimination is positively correlated with interest in politics. That is, racial minority citizens who say they have personally experienced discrimination tend to be more interested in politics. The magnitude of the relationship for Canada, France, and the United States is very similar. The coefficients for Germany and Denmark are substantially larger. The reason for this isn’t clear—but notably these have the smallest sample size and the largest confidence intervals. The addition of controls makes little difference in the size or statistical significance of the effects.

Figure 1. Discrimination and interest in politics.

Note: Figure shows OLS coefficients from separate models.

Turnout

The second set of models examines the relationship between discrimination and voter turnout. The turnout variables are a scale composed of questions about voting at different elections as available: (national, local, first and second round presidential, etc.—see Appendix in the Supplementary material for specific questions). This provides a more precise measure than a single question, and also helps increase variation—an issue in that self-reported turnout is sometimes very high (see p.12 for analysis of separate items). Only citizens are included. Unfortunately, the German survey does not include a question about voting. Otherwise, the models are similar to the previous set.

In contrast to the political interest models, results for the turnout models in Figure 2 are quite inconsistent. While the discrimination variable is statistically significant in many of the models, in others it is not. In Denmark, discrimination is positively correlated with turnout, while in Canada, France, and the UK it is negatively correlated or non-significant, depending on the model. The clear exception is the United States, where three separate datasets, with and without controls, all produce positive and statistically significant relationship between discrimination and turnout.

Figure 2. Discrimination and turnout.

Note: Figure shows OLS coefficients from separate models.

Next, we turn to the relationship between discrimination and other political actions. The political action variables are scales composed of questions about substantive political actions, excluding voting. This includes participating in protests, petitions, boycotts, donating money to political causes, volunteering in politics or community affairs, and contacting officials, with some variations between surveys (see Appendix in the Supplementary material for details). Many of the surveys do not include detailed questions about political actions, and so models are only estimated for the Canadian, U.K., and U.S. surveys in 2016 and 2004. Since the political action variables are over-dispersed count variables (most people do not take any political actions, a few take a lot), the models use negative binomial regression, with predicted values of the number of actions generated for the figure.

As Figure 3 shows, the correlation of discrimination and political action is statistically significant and positive for all countries: Canada, the U.K., and the United States. The size of the effects ranges from an increase of roughly .5 actions in Canada and the U.K., and about .75 and 1 in the United States. ‘This means, for example, that in the 2016 U.S. survey, the respondents in the highest category of discrimination on average take about 1 more action than those in the lowest category. Given that very few citizens take any political actions at all (as measured by these questions), this is a very substantial difference with important political implications.

Figure 3. Discrimination and political actions.

Note: Figure shows predicted values generated from negative binomial models, showing the difference in number of actions for respondents at the top vs bottom of the discrimination scale.

Government and Social Discrimination

Finally, we examine the distinct role of social and political discrimination. Previous research suggested that discrimination by the government leads to increased political engagement, since this is a way to hold those responsible to account, and leads to change. Conversely, social discrimination by ordinary people does not lead to increased engagement because it is not clearly linked to politics. Political discrimination is usually defined broadly as state actions, including discrimination by state actors or commonly regulated areas, such as immigration officials, employment, or housing. Conversely, social discrimination includes situations such as discrimination by private businesses or on the street.

To test this, models with the three dependant variables are estimated: interest in politics, turnout, and political actions. Rather than a single general discrimination variable, separate measure of social and political discrimination are both included in each model. Following (Oskooii Reference Oskooii2020), political discrimination is coded as reported discrimination by police, border guards, schools, work, housing, and social discrimination is by private businesses, in the street, or by friends and neighbors. See Appendix in the Supplementary material for question-wording and coding details. Some measure are not available in all datasets, so models are estimated where available. Otherwise, the models are identical to those above. Given the number of estimates, for clarity only models with controls are shown, but bivariate models are available in the Appendix, Supplementary material, and show similar overall results.

The results in Figure 4 show little evidence for effects from political but not social discrimination or for larger effects from political discrimination. The turnout results are mostly non-significant and of different signs, which is consistent with the mixed previous results in Figure 3. The interest results do show Canada and the UK with larger political discrimination than social discrimination for interest, but the other countries and measures do not show a clear pattern.

Figure 4. Political and social discrimination.

Note: Figure shows OLS and negative binomial (for political actions) coefficients from separate models with controls.

Robustness Checks and Additional Analysis

To test the robustness of these results, a number of alternative specifications are estimated. The turnout models were estimated with separate turnout variables from first-order elections and lower-order elections (national Parliament or presidential vs provincial or municipal, etc.) rather than a scale of all available turnout questions. The results are very similar, with no systematic differences between different types of elections, or relative to the scale used in the main analysis (Appendix Figure A1 in the Supplementary material). Turnout models with additional controls are also estimated where available, including interest, trust, efficacy, partisanship, and specific ethnic groups (Appendix Figure A2 in the Supplementary material). These also produce quite similar results as the models in the main text. More generally, the turnout variables in all countries are related as expected with other variables, such as positive correlations with age, education, and interest, suggesting there is nothing particularly unusual about the specific questions, coding, or data.

Separate models were estimated for racial and ethnic groups, where measures were available and sufficient sample sized permitted (Black-British, Asian-Americans, Arab-French, etc). For interest in politics and political actions, the results are positive and significant correlations across the board: essentially the same relationship for all groups, and similar to the main results (see Appendix Table A21, A22, and A25 in the Supplementary material). For turnout, other countries are mixed and null, as in the main results, including a number of the American groups (see Appendix Table A23 and A24 in the Supplementary material). Interestingly, the discrimination coefficients for Asian-Americans are the largest and most commonly statistically significant. Given modest sample sizes and the many models estimated it isn’t clear how robust this is, but may be worth future investigation.

Models using separate political actions (rather than the scale) were also estimated where data availability permits, including both those directed at conventional electoral-related actions (petitions, donations, contacting officials) and unconventional non-electoral actions (boycotts, protests). Nearly all are positively correlated with discrimination, statistically significant, and with coefficients of roughly comparable size. See Appendix Tables A12, A13, and A14 in the Supplementary material. Similarly, models with separate political actions and separate government and social discrimination measures show that both social and political discrimination variables are positive and statistically significant, with neither being systematically larger (Appendix Tables A15 and A16 in the Supplementary material). These are similar to Bilodeau et al. (Reference Bilodeau, White, Ma, Turgeon and Henderson2023)’s results for protest, but for a range of different political actions. Models are also estimated for social and political discrimination with additional controls (interest, trust, efficacy, partisanship, and specific ethnic groups (Appendix Figure A3 in the Supplementary material). These are also mixed: the political discrimination coefficients are larger for social discrimination coefficients for interest and turnout in the UK, but not in Canada and not for political actions in either country. The measures of social and political discrimination are correlated in general, but VIF scores are quite low (2 or lower), suggesting multicollinearity is not a serious issue. Finally, the political interest analysis above includes non-citizens (since unlike voting citizenship is not a requirement for interest in politics), but excluding non-citizens produces similar results (see Appendix Figure A4 in the Supplementary material).

Discussion and Conclusions

The ongoing discrimination experienced by racial and ethnic minorities across Western countries is both a product of politics and has important political effects. In light of conflicting and mixed results in previous research, this study examines the relationship between the experience of discrimination and political engagement across multiple countries.

The results are clear and consistent for interest in politics and non-voting political actions. Racial minorities who have personal experience of discrimination tend to have higher levels of political interest and take more political actions, and this holds across all countries examined. This evidence suggests that the relationship is broadly generalizable, rather than requiring explanations related to the particularities of immigration source countries, historical experiences, or political systems. This consistent relationship between discrimination and political participation holds despite some variation in measures and question-wording between surveys, which is point in favor of the robustness of the results.

However, the relationship between discrimination and voting (turnout) is quite different. In fact, the results are positive, negative, or null depending on the country and controls. While there is variation in questions across countries, even examining just the questions about voting in national elections does not seem to produce a clear cross-national pattern which suggests these questions are not the primary driver of inconsistent results. Some differences might be related to different modeling strategies, such as coding of scales, different controls, and so on. Since we do not know the true model or causal structure, one way to view these different results is as a form of robustness check, showing that some previous results may not be robust to alternative specifications. However, the clearly distinctive patterns in U.S. datasets suggest it isn’t simply a product of methodological choices.

Strikingly, all three of the American datasets show a positive and statistically significant relationship between turnout and discrimination—in contrast to most other countries. Moreover, the turnout results seem to be generally consistent with previous results, including negative effects in Canada and Europe (Bilodeau Reference Bilodeau2017; Martin Reference Martin2017; Mattila and Papageorgiou Reference Mattila and Papageorgiou2017), and positive effects in the United States (Besco et al. Reference Besco, Sergio Garcia-Rios, Lajevardi, Oskooii and Tolley2022; Lien Reference Lien1994; White Reference White2016). Denmark doesn’t fit that pattern, but it was the only survey to ask about “any election” rather than the most recent election.

The restriction of voting rights in the United States, which generally falls most heavily on racial minorities (e.g. Kuk, Hajnal and Lajevardi Reference Kuk, Hajnal and Lajevardi2022), has produced a robust civil society infrastructure dedicated to both contesting restrictions and mobilizing voters. Indeed, White (Reference White2016) credits the mobilization effects they find precisely to this kind of activism. Conversely, in Europe there is debate over citizenship regulations and non-citizen voting rights (e.g. Kayran and Erdilmen Reference Kayran and Erdilmen2021), but there are rarely serious attempts to make it difficult for citizens to vote. Where voting rights are a major focus of demands for racial equality, a strong connection between voting and experience of discrimination is more likely to emerge. In places where discrimination is experienced in ways not directly related to voting, the relationship might be different. Other kinds of political engagement, such as protesting, signing petitions, or being interested in politics are not subject to significant government regulations, and therefore differences in these regulations between countries are not relevant. This might lead to the kind of between-country variation (and lack of variation) seen here.

Nonetheless, a puzzle remains. Even if the politics of voting rights in the United States produced a stronger relationship between discrimination and turnout than seen elsewhere, it seems intuitively strange that discrimination would make people interested in politics, more likely to donate or sign petitions, but not to vote. The analysis here seems to convincingly demonstrate a series of relationships, but there is clearly more to be done in explaining why these emerge.

The limitations of the methods used here point toward opportunities in future research—three are discussed here. This study, like many others in the area, uses self-reports of both discrimination and engagement. While these are useful, we need more work using validated vote measures or other administrative data. For costly behavior like protesting or donating the risk of self-reports being “cheap talk” are especially high. These issues make understanding which sub-groups are mobilizing particularly difficult, since for rare actions social desirability effects might be larger than real shifts. Although is already some work which uses administrative measures for political mobilization, such as White (Reference White2016) and Kuhn (Reference Kuhn2022), or uses threatening external events and media data as an IV rather than self-reports (Cho, Gimpel and Wu Reference Cho, Gimpel and Wu2006; Weiss, Siegel and Romney Reference Weiss, Siegel and Romney2023), more is needed. In particular, there is little such work outside the United States.

The use of cross-sectional observational data also raises issues of endogeneity. For example, perhaps people who are interested in politics perceive more discrimination—or perhaps it causes them to actually experience more discrimination as a backlash to their activism. Thankfully, emerging causal evidence suggests this is not entirely the case (Besco et al. Reference Besco, Sergio Garcia-Rios, Lajevardi, Oskooii and Tolley2022;Weiss, Siegel and Romney Reference Weiss, Siegel and Romney2023; White Reference White2016). However, it might be that certain types of engagement are affected more than others, or the size of effects is incorrectly estimated. Or, it could be that causal routes are different for different kinds of political engagement. Studying discrimination causally is difficult, given that random natural variation is uncommon, and experimentally inducing it requires careful ethical considerations. Nonetheless, causal evidence is crucial for understanding these important issues.

Another approach is to more carefully theorize and test the mechanisms and moderators. One useful set of concepts might be perceived responsibility and efficacy. The failure of the social vs political discrimination analysis to replicate is surprising, given evidence from two countries and groups, in addition to a highly plausible theoretical account (e.g. Oskooii Reference Oskooii2020). One explanation might be that the different expected effects of these sources of discrimination rely on expectations about responsibility: that political participation is a good way to contest discrimination by government actors. However, it isn’t clear that people in all contexts will have similar perceptions of responsibility. Are governments responsible for discrimination in schools, workplaces, or housing? Perhaps, but these might also be different than more “typical” state actors like politicians, bureaucrats, or police officers. It isn’t obvious what people on average would think, or how much variation there would be across individuals, groups, or contexts. In addition, would people who experience discrimination view political mobilization as an effective way to contest discrimination? Even if the state isn’t the source of discrimination, it might well be the solution, and thus political engagement is an effective method. Measuring these factors might help explain why, and when, different forms of discrimination lead to political engagement.

Finally, the role of civil society and other types of organizations needs more attention. If mobilization by civic organizations plays a key role in creating the link between threat or discrimination and voting in the United States, as White (Reference White2016) and others suggest, does this not occur in other countries, and why not? There is research on immigrant organizations and political participation in Europe (e.g. Berger, Galonska and Koopmans Reference Berger, Galonska and Koopmans2004; Pilati and Morales Reference Pilati and Morales2016), but this is not well incorporated into the research on discrimination. In part, this is due to methodological difficulties, since work on individual-level behavior tends to focus on individual-level factors, and standard surveys can be difficult to use for studying organizations. Nonetheless, this may be an avenue of research for understanding these quite different results for turnout across countries.

In conclusion, the results here provide an important cross-national test of the relationship of personal discrimination on political participation. We might hope that experiencing discrimination causes people to take political action to resist, rather than becoming discouraged and alienated. This is partially borne out by the results presented here—racial minorities who experience discrimination are more interested in politics and take more political actions, but may not vote at higher rates—at least not everywhere. In a global political environment where discrimination is increasingly prevalent, this is at least partially encouraging news.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/rep.2024.17.

Funding statement

Thank you to Natasha Goel and Mohy-Dean Tabbara for excellent research assistance. Funded by SSHRC Grant 430-2016-00650.

Footnotes

1 Data and code are available at https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/WMOLZL.

References

Alonso, S and da Fonseca, SC (2012) Immigration, left and right. Party Politics 18(6), 865884.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barreto, M, Manzano, S, Ramirez, R and Rim, K (2008) Mobilization, participation, and solidaridad: Latino participation in the 2006 immigration protest rallies. Urban Affairs Review 44(5), 736764.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beeri, I and Saad, M (2014) Political participation unconditioned by inequality and discrimination: The case of minorities-within-minorities in Israeli-Arab mixed municipalities. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 40(10), 15261549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, M, Galonska, C and Koopmans, R (2004) Political integration by a detour? Ethnic communities and social capital of migrants in Berlin. Journal of ethnic and Migration Studies 30(3), 491507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besco, R (2021) From stability to polarization: The transformation of Canadian public opinion on immigration, 1975-2019. American Review of Canadian Studies 51(1), 143165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besco, R, Sergio Garcia-Rios, JL, Lajevardi, N, Oskooii, K and Tolley, E (2022) Fight not flight: The effects of explicit racism on minority political engagement. Electoral Studies 80, 102515.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilodeau, A (2017) Mobilisation or demobilisation? Perceived discrimination and political engagement among visible minorities in Quebec. Political Science 69(2), 122138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bilodeau, A, White, SE, Ma, C, Turgeon, Luc and Henderson, A (2023) Marginalized, but not demobilized: Ethnic minority protest activity when facing discrimination. International Political Science Review 44(5), 627644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broman, CL, Mavaddat, R and Hsu, S (2000) The experience and consequences of perceived racial discrimination: A study of African Americans. Journal of Black Psychology 26(2), 165180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, AL (2003) Participatory reactions to policy threats: Senior citizens and the defense of social security and medicare. Political Behavior 25(1), 2949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carmines, EG and Stimson, JA (1982) Racial issues and the structure of mass belief systems. The Journal of Politics 44 (1), 220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cho, WKT, Gimpel, JG and Wu, T (2006) Clarifying the role of SES in political participation: Policy threat and Arab American mobilization. The Journal of Politics 68(4), 977991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chong, D and Rogers, R (2005) Racial solidarity and political participation. Political Behavior 27(4), 347374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, MC (1994) Behind the Mule: Race and Class in African American Politics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Dennison, J and Geddes, A (2019) A rising tide? The salience of immigration and the rise of anti-immigration political parties in Western Europe. The Political Quarterly 90(1), 107116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer-Neumann, M (2014) Immigrants’ ethnic identification and political involvement in the face of discrimination: A longitudinal study of the German case. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 40(3), 339362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, K (2016) Hate Crimes Soared by 41% after Brexit Vote, Official Figures Reveal. The Independent.Google Scholar
Golder, M (2016) Far right parties in Europe. Annual Review of Political Science 19, 477497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grewal, S and Hamid, S (2022) Discrimination, inclusion, and anti-system attitudes among Muslims in Germany. American Journal of Political Science 68 (2), 511528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutierrez, A, Ocampo, AX, Barreto, MA and Segura, G (2019) Somos Más: How racial threat and anger mobilized Latino voters in the Trump era. Political Research Quarterly 72(4), 960975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, AF, Fisher, SD, Rosenblatt, G, Sanders, D and Sobolewska, M (2013) The Political Integration of Ethnic Minorities in Britain. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobbs, W and Lajevardi, N (2019) Effects of divisive political campaigns on the day-to-day segregation of Arab and Muslim Americans. American Political Science Review 113(1), 270276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, DJ and Washington, S (2020) The rise of Trump, the fall of prejudice? Tracking white Americans’ racial attitudes via a panel survey, 2008–2018. Public Opinion Quarterly 84(1), 119140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kayran, EN and Erdilmen, M (2021) When do states give voting rights to non-citizens? The role of population, policy, and politics on the timing of enfranchisement reforms in liberal democracies. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 47(13), 28552876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koning, EA (2023) Breaking through: how anti-immigrant parties establish themselves and the implications for their study. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion and Parties 33(4), 519537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, E (2022) Does policy threat mobilise? 287 (g) and Latino voter registration in North Carolina and Florida. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies 48(11), 27992818.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuk, J, Hajnal, Z and Lajevardi, N (2022) A disproportionate burden: Strict voter identification laws and minority turnout. Politics, Groups, and Identities 10(1), 126134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lichtblau, E (2016) U.S. Hate Crimes Surge 6%, Fueled by Attacks on Muslims. New York Times.Google Scholar
Lien, P (1994) Ethnicity and political participation: A comparison between Asian and Mexican Americans. Political Behavior 16, 237264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lippens, L, Vermeiren, S and Baert, S (2023) The state of hiring discrimination: A meta-analysis of (almost) all recent correspondence experiments. European Economic Review 151, 104315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lu, F and Jones, B (2019) Effects of belief versus experiential discrimination on race-based linked fate. Politics, Groups, and Identities 7(3), 615624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsh, WZ and Ramrez, R (2019) Unlinking fate? Discrimination, group-consciousness, and political participation among Latinos and whites. Politics, Groups, and Identities 7(3), 625641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, N (2017) Are British Muslims alienated from mainstream politics by Islamophobia and British foreign policy? Ethnicities 17(3), 350370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattila, M and Papageorgiou, A (2017) Disability, perceived discrimination and political participation. International Political Science Review 38(5), 505519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClain, PD, Johnson Carew, JD, Walton, E and Watts, CS (2009) Group membership, group identity, and group consciousness: Measures of racial identity in American Politics? Annual Review of Political Science 12, 471485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, AH, Gurin, P, Gurin, G and Malanchuk, O (1981) Group consciousness and political participation. American Journal of Political Science 25(3), 494511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oskooii, K (2020) Perceived discrimination and political behavior. British Journal of Political Science 50(3), 867892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oskooii, KAR (2016) How discrimination impacts sociopolitical behavior: A multidimensional perspective. Political Psychology 37(5), 613640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Page, D (2018) When does sexuality-based discrimination motivate political participation? Political Psychology 39(5), 10131030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pantoja, AD, Ramirez, R and Segura, GM (2001) Citizens by choice, voters by necessity: Patterns in political mobilization by naturalized Latinos. Political Research Quarterly 54(4), 729750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilati, K and Morales, L (2016) Ethnic and immigrant politics vs. mainstream politics: The role of ethnic organizations in shaping the political participation of immigrant-origin individuals in Europe. Ethnic and Racial Studies 39(15), 27962817.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quillian, L, Pager, D, Hexel, O and Midtbøen, AH (2017) Meta-analysis of field experiments shows no change in racial discrimination in hiring over time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114 (41), 1087010875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reny, TT, Collingwood, L and Valenzuela, AA (2019) Vote switching in the 2016 election: How racial and immigration attitudes, not economics, explain shifts in white voting. Public Opinion Quarterly 83(1), 91113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schildkraut, DJ (2005) The rise and fall of political engagement among Latinos: The role of identity and perceptions of discrimination. Political Behavior 27(3), 285312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tesler, M (2020) Post-racial or Most-racial? Race and Politics in the Obama era. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Valdez, Z (2011) Political participation among Latinos in the United States: The effect of group identity and consciousness. Social Science Quarterly 92(2), 466482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valentino, NA, Neuner, FG and Vandenbroek, ML (2018) The changing norms of racial political rhetoric and the end of racial priming. The Journal of Politics 80(3), 757771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, CM, Siegel, A and Romney, D (2023) How threats of exclusion mobilize palestinian political participation. American Journal of Political Science 67(4), 10801095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, A (2016) When threat mobilizes: Immigration enforcement and Latino voter turnout. Political Behavior 38, 355382.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wrinkle, RD, Stewart, J Jr., Polinard, JL Meier, KJ and Arvizu, JR (1996) Ethnicity and nonelectoral political participation. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences 18(2), 142153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Surveys

Figure 1

Figure 1. Discrimination and interest in politics.Note: Figure shows OLS coefficients from separate models.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Discrimination and turnout.Note: Figure shows OLS coefficients from separate models.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Discrimination and political actions.Note: Figure shows predicted values generated from negative binomial models, showing the difference in number of actions for respondents at the top vs bottom of the discrimination scale.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Political and social discrimination.Note: Figure shows OLS and negative binomial (for political actions) coefficients from separate models with controls.

Supplementary material: File

Besco supplementary material 1

Besco supplementary material
Download Besco supplementary material 1(File)
File 644.4 KB