Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T03:04:03.404Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Racism and the Pinkerton syndrome in Singapore: effects of race on hiring decisions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 April 2019

Peter K. H. Chew*
Affiliation:
College of Healthcare Sciences, James Cook University, Singapore
Jessica L. Young
Affiliation:
College of Healthcare Sciences, James Cook University, Singapore
Gerald P. K. Tan
Affiliation:
College of Healthcare Sciences, James Cook University, Singapore
*
Author for correspondence: Peter K. H. Chew, Email: [email protected]

Abstract

The aim of the study was to examine racism and the Pinkerton syndrome in Singapore. Specifically, the study examined the effects of race on hiring decisions in a simulated hiring decision task. Participants were 171 (61% males) Singaporean Chinese undergraduates from a private university in Singapore. They were randomly assigned into one of nine groups and asked to review a resume of a job applicant. The study used a 3 (Academic qualifications: strong, moderate, or weak) × 3 (Race: White, Chinese, or Malay) between-subjects design with perceived warmth, competence, applicant suitability and recommended salary as the dependent variables. The results showed that while Chinese participants discriminated against Malay applicants (racism), they discriminated in favor of White applicants (the Pinkerton syndrome). The results provided a potential explanation to the economic disparities between Malays and the other races, and first experimental evidence for racism and the Pinkerton syndrome in Singapore.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2019

In a recent survey of 2,000 Singaporeans, 53% indicated that racism is not an important problem in contemporary Singapore (Mathews, Reference Mathews2016). It is unclear whether this optimism is justified. Although racism has been extensively investigated in other countries since the 1900s (Allport, Reference Allport1954), there are little to no experimental investigations of racism in Singapore (Chew, Reference Chew2018). In addition, it has been observed that some Singaporean women tended to exclusively date and marry White men (Hudson, Reference Hudson, Koh and Balasingamchow2015; Kuah, Reference Kuah1997; Lim, Reference Lim1995). This phenomenon is referred to as the Pinkerton syndrome,Footnote 1 and the derogatory term Sarong Party Girls is often used to describe these women. In this article, we argue that the observation is a narrow, overt manifestation of a general tendency for Asians to be prejudiced and to discriminate in favor of Whites. While a similar phenomenon has been investigated in other countries (David & Okazaki, Reference David and Okazaki2006b; Utsey, Abrams, Opare-Henaku, Bolden, & Williams, Reference Utsey, Abrams, Opare-Henaku, Bolden and Williams2015), there are little to no experimental investigations of the Pinkerton syndrome in Singapore.

Racism

Racism refers to “the differential treatment enacted by an individual, group, or organization on individuals based on assumptions of a group’s phenotypic, linguistic, or cultural differences” (Gamst, Liang, & Der-Karabetian, Reference Gamst, Liang and Der-Karabetian2011, p. 251). There are two forms of racism: (a) overt racism, characterized by open hostility, aggression, and discrimination; and (b) covert racism, characterized by expressing prejudice or discriminating others in subtle, socially justifiable manners. It has been argued that both forms of racism have always been expressed concurrently throughout history and that a distinction between the two is redundant (Leach, Reference Leach2005). In contrast, others have argued that covert racism has replaced overt racism due to the Civil Rights Movement in the 1960s (Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Dovidio and Gaertner1986; Katz, Reference Katz1981; McConahay, Reference McConahay, Dovidio and Gaertner1986; McConahay & Hough, Reference McConahay and Hough1976).

Aversive racism is a form of covert racism where individuals “endorse egalitarian values and regard themselves as nonprejudiced but who nonetheless discriminate in subtle, rationalizable ways” (Hodson, Dovidio, & Gaertner, Reference Hodson, Dovidio and Gaertner2002, p. 460). In one study, 78 White undergraduates were asked to review applications to the university with either strong, moderate, or weak academic qualifications (Hodson et al., Reference Hodson, Dovidio and Gaertner2002). There was no discrimination against Black applicants with either strong or weak academic qualifications. However, highly prejudiced White participants discriminated against Black applicants with moderate qualifications. Evidence for aversive racism has also been found in a simulated hiring decision task. In this study, 194 White undergraduates were asked to review the credentials of a potential job applicant with either strong, moderate, or weak qualifications (Dovidio & Gaertner, Reference Dovidio and Gaertner2000). A similar pattern of results was found. White participants discriminated against Black applicants with moderate qualifications but not those with either strong or weak qualifications. Both studies showed that the ambiguous nature of moderate qualifications allowed White participants to rationalize their discriminatory behavior.

The negative effects of racism have been well documented. For example, the relationships between self-reported perceived racism and a range of negative physical and mental health outcomes have been documented in several literature reviews (Brondolo, Gallo, & Myers, Reference Brondolo, Gallo and Myers2009; Paradies, Reference Paradies2006; Williams & Mohammed, Reference Williams and Mohammed2009) and at least two meta-analyses (Paradies et al., Reference Paradies, Ben, Denson, Elias, Priest, Pieterse and Gee2015; Pieterse, Todd, Neville, & Carter, Reference Pieterse, Todd, Neville and Carter2012). Racism has also been linked to reduced employment opportunities. Field experiments have been conducted where fictitious resumes, containing either a White name or a Black name, were sent in response to job advertisements (Bertrand & Mullainathan, Reference Bertrand and Mullainathan2004; Pager, Western, & Bonikowski, Reference Pager, Western and Bonikowski2009). The results indicated that resumes with a Black name are less likely to receive a positive response (e.g., a call for an interview) from employers. Overall, a review of field experiments concluded that a Black job seeker is “50 to 500 percent less likely to be considered by employers as an equally qualified White job applicant” (Pager, Reference Pager2007, p. 114).

Racism Research in Singapore

Singapore is a multiracial society consisting of four races: 74.1% of the population are Chinese, 13.4% Malays, 9.2% Indians, and 3.3% Others (Singapore Department of Statistics, 2010). Since the 1980s, Malays have been underperforming on educational achievement and economic indicators compared to Chinese and Indians (Mutalib, Reference Mutalib2011). These racial disparities have not been resolved. According to the latest population census, only 5.1% of Malays had obtained a university qualification compared to 22.6% of Chinese and 35% of Indians (Singapore Department of Statistics, 2010). In addition, the median monthly household income for Malays was $3,844 compared to $5,100 for Chinese and $5,370 for Indians. Lastly, only 2.8% of Malays live in condominiums and landed properties compared to 18% of Chinese and 16.3% of Indians. Such disparities are often explained using the concept of meritocracy – Malays are not talented or hardworking enough to achieve educational or financial success. This explanation is unsatisfactory given the problems inherent with meritocracy. For example, meritocracy assumes that everyone is provided with equal opportunities to succeed (i.e. regardless of race, socioeconomic status); this assumption is not supported by the literature (Barr & Low, Reference Barr and Low2005; Koh, Reference Koh2014; Moore, Reference Moore2000; Tan, Reference Tan2008). Instead, these disparities seem to suggest the presence of racism against Malays in Singapore (Lee, Reference Lee2004).

Racism is prohibited by two laws in Singapore: The Sedition Act of Singapore (The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore, 1948) and Penal Code Chapter 224 (The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore, 1871). In particular, the Sedition Act makes it an offence for anyone to “to promote feelings of ill-will and hostility between different races or classes of the population of Singapore” (The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore, 1948, p. 2). Given that the Sedition Act has been used to arrest individuals for racist comments (AsiaOne News, 2012; The New Paper, 2008), it is likely that racism in Singapore would be covert, instead of overt, in nature. However, the limited research on racism in Singapore has not taken covert racism into consideration.

Racism research in Singapore has been dominated by narrative reviews (Barr, Reference Barr2006; Barr & Low, Reference Barr and Low2005; Chua, Reference Chua2003; Moore, Reference Moore2000; Mutalib, Reference Mutalib2011; Teo, Reference Teo2005; Walsh, Reference Walsh2007). These reviews tended to adopt a historical narrative, tracking policies from the 1960s to explain how Malays are marginalized in Singapore. Some of these policies include the Speak Mandarin Campaign for the Chinese but an absence of similar campaigns for the respective languages of the other races (Teo, Reference Teo2005). Also, primary school English textbooks in the 1980s tended to depict Malays in menial jobs (e.g. a street sweeper) but Chinese in skilled jobs (e.g. a doctor; Barr, Reference Barr2006). Lastly, the recurring pattern of Malays requesting equal treatment and the government’s resistance to such requests has been documented (Mutalib, Reference Mutalib2011). Taken together, these reviews provided a plausible account of institutionalized racism in Singapore.

In contrast, the small number of quantitative researches suggests that there is little racism in Singapore (Chin & Vasu, Reference Chin and Vasu2012; Mathews, Reference Mathews2013, Reference Mathews2016; Ooi, Reference Ooi2005). For example, two large-scale studies administered questionnaires using a set of survey questions at the homes of Singaporeans to examine racial harmony (Mathews, Reference Mathews2013, Reference Mathews2016). Overall, although there was some evidence of racism, the results from both studies provided support for racial harmony in Singapore. However, these studies focused on self-reported racial attitudes and perceived racial discrimination. The validity of the results is in question because the items used in the studies (e.g. “People from all races should be treated equally”) promote a socially desirable response. This limitation and the lack of experimental evidence precluded a conclusion on whether racism exists in contemporary Singapore.

The Pinkerton Syndrome

Since the 1990s, it has been observed that some Singaporean women tended to exclusively date and marry White men (Hudson, Reference Hudson, Koh and Balasingamchow2015; Kuah, Reference Kuah1997). Lim (Reference Lim1995) speculated that this could be due to the perception that White men are novel, interesting, socially refined, and intelligent. In other words, Singaporean women might discriminate in favor of White men because of the positive stereotypes associated with Whites. This phenomenon is referred to as the Pinkerton syndrome, and the derogatory term Sarong Party Girls is often used to describe these women. However, it seems that the preference for Whites is not restricted to interpersonal attraction. A cursory search of Singapore’s online media showed that the Hokkien dialect term Ang Mor Tua Kee, literally translated as “Westerners Big Shot”, is often used when there is a perception that Whites receive preferential treatment in a wide variety of situations. Accordingly, we redefine the Pinkerton syndrome broadly as the tendency for Asians to be prejudiced and to discriminate in favor of Whites; it is an unusual form of racism perpetuated by the majority against themselves for a specific minority. The Pinkerton syndrome is probably caused by two factors: colonial mentality and colorism.

Colonial mentality is conceptualized as “a form of internalized oppression, characterized by a perception of ethnic or cultural inferiority”, due to a history of colonization by Western powers (David & Okazaki, Reference David and Okazaki2006b, p. 241). Colonial mentality is characterized by within-group discrimination (preference for more westernized Asians), a desire for the physical characteristics of Whites, colonial debt (belief that Asians should be grateful to their colonizers), cultural shame and embarrassment, and internalized cultural inferiority. Evidence for colonial mentality has been found among Filipino Americans (David & Okazaki, Reference David and Okazaki2006b) and Ghanaians (Utsey et al., Reference Utsey, Abrams, Opare-Henaku, Bolden and Williams2015); both the Philippines and Ghana have a history of colonization by Western powers. The negative effects of colonial mentality have been well documented. For example, colonial mentality is positively correlated with depression and anxiety, and negatively correlated with self-esteem and collective self-esteem (David & Okazaki, Reference David and Okazaki2006b; Utsey et al., Reference Utsey, Abrams, Opare-Henaku, Bolden and Williams2015). In addition to colonial mentality, a history of colonization could also lead to colorism.

Colorism is defined as “the system that privileges the lighter skinned over the darker skinned people within a community of color” (Hunter, Reference Hunter2002, p. 176). While some researchers have argued that colorism is a direct result of colonization (Hunter, Reference Hunter2002, Reference Hunter2007), others have maintained that colorism predates colonization (Ashikari, Reference Ashikari2005; Li, Min, Belk, & Shalini Bahl, Reference Li, Min, Belk, Shalini Bahl, Lee and Duluth2008; Wagatsuma, Reference Wagatsuma1967). Either way, colorism predicts that there will be a preference for lighter skinned individuals, resulting in both between- and within-group discrimination. Indeed, a literature review showed that darker skinned individuals are disadvantaged in education and housing, and for darker skinned women, in dating and marriage (Hunter, Reference Hunter2007). Unsurprisingly, darker skinned individuals are also disadvantaged in employment. For example, darker skinned Latinos, Mexican Americans, and African Americans had a lower annual income than their lighter skinned counterparts (Hunter, Reference Hunter2007).

Singapore was a colony of the United Kingdom from 1819 to 1942 before gaining independence in 1965 (Lepoer, Reference Lepoer1989). The long history of colonization could have resulted in colonial mentality and colorism. Consequently, the Pinkerton syndrome should be prevalent among Singaporeans. Yet, despite the research on colonial mentality and colorism in other countries, and the observations of some Singaporean women preferring White men, there has been little to no experimental investigations of the Pinkerton syndrome in Singapore.

The Current Study

Overall, there are two research gaps in the literature. First, there are little to no experimental investigations of racism in Singapore (Chew, Reference Chew2018). Most studies on racism were conducted predominantly in the United States and it is currently unclear whether the results are generalizable to samples from other countries. Furthermore, the generalizability of those results may be limited since racism is a highly contextualized phenomenon. For example, the strict laws in Singapore might serve to minimize or eliminate racism (e.g. The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore, 1871, 1948). While there is racism research in Singapore, these studies tended to be either narrative or descriptive, both of which are susceptible to biases (e.g. researcher bias). Second, there are little to no experimental investigations of the Pinkerton syndrome in Singapore. The preference for an out-group is not a new phenomenon. While most individuals would exhibit in-group favoritism and out-group derogation, minorities tend to exhibit in-group derogation and out-group favoritism (Tajfel & Turner, Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Austin1986). However, the Pinkerton syndrome predicts a new phenomenon where Asians, a majority group, would discriminate against themselves for Whites (i.e. a minority group in Asia). In addition, the Pinkerton syndrome is distinct from colonial mentality and colorism. Those two constructs tended to focus on within-group discrimination (David & Okazaki, Reference David and Okazaki2006a, Reference David and Okazaki2006b; Hunter, Reference Hunter2002, Reference Hunter2007) whereas the Pinkerton syndrome focuses on between-group discrimination.

Accordingly, the current study aims to address the research gaps by examining racism and the Pinkerton syndrome in an experiment. Given that racism in Singapore is probably covert in nature, the current study adopts the aversive racism framework (Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Dovidio and Gaertner1986) to examine the effects of race on hiring decisions in a simulated hiring decision task (Dovidio & Gaertner, Reference Dovidio and Gaertner2000). Hiring decisions were examined because of the potential economic implications of the results. Consistent with the framework, we expect Chinese participants to discriminate against Malay applicants with moderate qualifications but not those with either strong or weak qualifications. Specifically, we hypothesized that Chinese participants will rate Malay applicants with moderate qualifications to be less competent (Hypothesis 1a), less suitable for the job (Hypothesis 1b), and will recommend a lower salary (Hypothesis 1c) than equally qualified Chinese applicants.

In contrast, the Pinkerton syndrome is generally not considered as racism in Singapore. Hence, due to the perceived acceptability of the Pinkerton syndrome, we expect Chinese participants to discriminate in favor of White applicants, regardless of the strength of academic qualifications. Specifically, we hypothesized that Chinese participants will rate White applicants to be more competent (Hypothesis 2a), more suitable for the job (Hypothesis 2b), and will recommend a higher salary (Hypothesis 2c) than equally qualified Chinese applicants.

Method

Construction and Evaluation of Resumes and Names

Three resumes were constructed in consultation with the Career Counsellor of the university. Elements of the resumes were varied to manipulate the strength of the academic qualifications. Some of these elements are listed in Table 1. In addition, we generated four names for each of the three races, resulting in a total of 12 names.

Table 1. Variation of elements in the resume to manipulate the strength of academic qualifications

Eighteen Singaporean Chinese undergraduates were randomly assigned to receive one of the three resumes without names (i.e. either strong, moderate, or weak in academic qualifications). The participants reviewed the resume and completed the Perception of Warmth and Competence Scale (Bloodhart, Reference Bloodhart2009; Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu, Reference Fiske, Cuddy, Glick and Xu2002) and the Hiring Decision Scale (Barrick, Swider, & Stewart, Reference Barrick, Swider and Stewart2010). Subsequently, they recommended a monthly salary for the job applicant given the starting salary range of $2,800 to $3,562 for a psychology graduate in Singapore (Ministry of Education, Singapore, 2015). Lastly, participants were randomly presented the 12 names, one at a time, and identified the race of the individual using five options: Chinese, Malay, Indian, White, and Others. Preliminary analyses suggested that participants were able to discriminate between strong, moderate, and weak academic qualifications in terms of perceived competence and applicant suitability, and recommended salary. Also, they were able to correctly identify the race of the individuals based on the names presented.

Participants

Participants were a convenience sample of 175 Singaporean Chinese undergraduates from a private university in Singapore. Data from four participants were removed because they were multivariate outliers. The final sample consisted of 171 participants (61% males). Their ages ranged from 17 to 43 years (M = 24.08, SD = 3.02).

Instruments

Perception of Warmth and Competence Scale

This scale is an eight-item instrument designed to assess two factors: (a) Perception of Warmth (e.g. “How friendly do you think the candidate is?”) and (b) Perception of Competence (e.g. “How skilled do you think the candidate is?”; Fiske et al., Reference Fiske, Cuddy, Glick and Xu2002). Responses are made on a 9-point Likert-type scale that ranges from 0 = not at all to 8 = extremely. Higher scores indicate higher perception of warmth and competence. A study omitted the confidence item (i.e. “How confident do you think the candidate is?”) because it loaded equally on both Warmth and Competence, and reported internal consistencies of .93 and .91 for the two factors respectively (n = 144; Bloodhart, Reference Bloodhart2009).

Hiring Decision Scale

This scale is a four-item instrument designed to assess perceived applicant suitability (e.g. “On the basis of his/her qualifications, I feel this applicant would be a good hire for this job”; Barrick et al., Reference Barrick, Swider and Stewart2010). Responses are made on a 5-point Likert-type scale that ranges from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree. Higher scores indicate higher perception of applicant suitability. The study reported an internal consistency of .93 for the instrument (n = 189).

Procedure

Upon arrival at the research lab, participants were asked to play the role of a human resource consultant assisting a company in hiring decisions. In order to conceal the true purpose of the study, participants were told that the study aims to examine the effects of warmth perception on hiring decisions. First, participants were provided with a job description containing both the responsibilities and requirements of a research assistant. Second, participants were randomly assigned into one of nine groups – Academic qualifications (strong, moderate, or weak) × Race (White, Chinese, or Malay) – and reviewed the relevant resume. We manipulated race by assigning the following names to the resumes: David Allan Smith (White), Wong Junjie (Chinese), and Muhammad Ashraf Bin Abdullah (Malay). Finally, participants completed the Perception of Warmth and Competence Scale (Bloodhart, Reference Bloodhart2009) and the Hiring Decision Scale (Barrick et al., Reference Barrick, Swider and Stewart2010), recommended a salary for the job applicant using the salary range of $2,800 to $3,562 (Ministry of Education, Singapore, 2015), and completed a demographics form. At the end of the experiment, participants were debriefed about the true purpose of the study and received course credit for their time. This procedure was approved by James Cook University’s Human Research Ethics Committee.

Results

All results were analyzed using SPSS version 25. Exploratory factor analysis was conducted using principal axis factoring extraction and Promax rotation (K = 4) to examine the underlying structure of the Perception of Warmth and Competence Scale and the Hiring Decision Scale. Two factors were extracted for the Perception of Warmth and Competence Scale and a simple structure was obtained, with all items loading substantially on only one factor. However, the confidence item (i.e. “How confident do you think the candidate is?”) loaded on the Warmth factor instead of the Competence factor. Accordingly, the item was omitted for subsequent analyses. In addition, only the Competence factor was used in subsequent analysis due to its relevance to the aims of the current study. The internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the factor was .89. One factor was extracted for the Hiring Decision Scale and a simple structure was obtained, with all items loading substantially on the factor. The internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the instrument was .91.

The means and standard deviations of perceived competence and applicant suitability, and recommended salary are presented in Table 2. A 3 (Academic qualifications: strong, moderate, or weak) × 3 (Race: White, Chinese, or Malay) between-subjects multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted with perceived competence and applicant suitability, and recommended salary as the dependent variables. Pillai’s trace was used to interpret the results due to violations of the homogeneity of covariance assumption and the homogeneity of variance assumption for perceived applicant suitability.

Table 2. Means and standard deviations of perceived competence and applicant suitability, and recommended salary by academic qualifications (strong, moderate, or weak) and race (White, Chinese, or Malay) of the candidate

Note: Mean scores that are significantly different after post hoc tests are bolded.

There was no statistically significant interaction effect between academic qualifications and race on the combined dependent variables, F(12, 486) = 1.12, p = .34, Pillai’s trace = .08. There was a statistically significant main effect of academic qualifications, F(6, 322) = 10.63, p = .000, Pillai’s trace = .33, partial eta squared = .17; and a statistically significant main effect of race, F(6, 322) = 8.99, p = .000, Pillai’s trace = .29, partial eta squared = .14, on the combined dependent variables. Given the aims of the current study, follow-up analyses only focused on race as the independent variable. The dependent variables were considered separately and the results are presented in Table 3.

Table 3. Effects of race on perceived competence and applicant suitability, and recommended salary

Note: The alpha level was corrected using a Bonferroni adjustment (i.e. alpha = .05/3 analyses = .017). All tests are significant, p = .000.

Post hoc tests were conducted using Tukey HSD (honest significant difference) test to examine the effects of race on each of the dependent variables. For perceived competence, Whites (M = 17.78, SD = 2.69) and Chinese (M = 16.53, SD = 3.16) had higher perceived competence than Malays (M = 15.08, SD = 3.48), p = .000, 95% CI [1.48, 3.92] and p = .02, 95% CI [.23, 2.66] respectively. For perceived applicant suitability, Whites (M = 15.89, SD = 1.98) had higher perceived applicant suitability than Chinese (M = 14.45, SD = 3.01) and Malays (M = 13.21, SD = 3.40), p = .01, 95% CI [.29, 2.58] and p = .000, 95% CI [1.57, 3.79] respectively. Also, Chinese had higher perceived applicant suitability than Malays, p =.02, 95% CI [.14, 2.35]. Lastly for recommended salary, Whites (M = 3112.74, SD = 213.75) had a higher recommended salary than Chinese (M = 2,992.73, SD = 164.55) and Malays (M = 2,890.94, SD = 157.21), p = .000, 95% CI [47.22, 192.81] and p = .000, 95% CI [151.07, 292.54] respectively. Also, Chinese had a higher recommended salary than Malays, p =.002, 95% CI [31.40, 172.18]. The remaining pairwise comparisons were not statistically significant.

Discussion

There was no significant interaction effect between academic qualifications and race. The results showed that Chinese participants rated Malay applicants as less competent, less suitable for the job, and recommended a lower salary than equally qualified Chinese applicants. However, this effect was found regardless of the strength of academic qualifications, providing partial support for Hypotheses 1a, 1b and 1c. The results also showed that Chinese participants rated White applicants as equally competent, more suitable for the job, and recommended a higher salary than equally qualified Chinese applicants, providing no support for Hypothesis 2a, but support for Hypotheses 2b and 2c.

The finding that Chinese participants discriminated against Malay applicants regardless of the strength of academic qualifications is inconsistent with the aversive racism framework (Gaertner & Dovidio, Reference Gaertner, Dovidio, Dovidio and Gaertner1986) and previous studies that found discrimination only against applicants with moderate qualifications (Dovidio & Gaertner, Reference Dovidio and Gaertner2000; Hodson et al., Reference Hodson, Dovidio and Gaertner2002). The inconsistency suggests that either the aversive racism framework is not applicable to the Singaporean context or the participants were displaying overt racism. Either way, the discrimination observed in the current study could be explained by Chinese participants acting on their negative stereotypes of Malays (i.e. less competent). Consequently, the negative stereotype resulted in lower perceived applicant suitability and a lower recommended salary for Malay applicants.

The results provided a potential explanation to the economic disparities between Malays and the other races (Singapore Department of Statistics, 2010). First, given their lower perceived competence and applicant suitability, Malays will be less likely to be hired and more likely to be retrenched by organizations. Second, Malay applicants were recommended a monthly salary that is $101.79 less than equally qualified Chinese applicants. This income gap will probably widen over time. Lastly, while admittedly speculative, such discrimination might also occur when making promotion decisions. For example, a Chinese manager might choose to promote and increase the salary of a Chinese employee over an equally qualified Malay employee. Taken together, these discriminatory behaviors would maintain or even exacerbate the current economic disparities.

The finding that Chinese participants discriminated in favor of White applicants is consistent with the predictions made by colonial mentality (David & Okazaki, Reference David and Okazaki2006b) and colorism (Hunter, Reference Hunter2002), and provides the first evidence of the Pinkerton syndrome in Singapore. The mechanisms underlying the Pinkerton syndrome seem to be different from that of racism. Specifically, it appears that Chinese participants were not acting on the positive stereotypes of Whites since they rated White applicants as equally competent. While it is possible that other positive stereotypes could be activated and acted upon, this explanation is unlikely given that perceived competence was assessed broadly (Fiske et al., Reference Fiske, Cuddy, Glick and Xu2002). However, despite the perception of equal competence, Chinese participants rated White applicants as more suitable for the job and recommended them a higher salary than equally qualified Chinese applicants. Such results demonstrate the strong negative effects of colonial mentality and colorism.

In an increasingly globalized world, companies in Singapore have access to a wider racial diversity of job applicants. Consequently, Malays face a dual disadvantage of racism (Chinese against Malays) and the Pinkerton syndrome (Chinese for Whites). For example, Malay applicants were recommended a monthly salary that is $221.80 less than equally qualified White applicants. However, the extent of this disadvantage is revealed by an inspection of the mean scores across academic qualifications in Table 2. In particular, Malay applicants with strong academic qualifications had similar scores on perceived competence and applicant suitability, and a similar recommended salary than White applicants with weak academic qualifications. In other words, a Malay applicant has to be outstanding in order to be given the same consideration as a lesser qualified White applicant. Taken together, these findings illustrate the negative effects of the Pinkerton syndrome.

Limitations of the current study should be noted. The study recruited as participants a convenience sample of Singaporean Chinese university students. Also, participants in the study reviewed and rated only one resume. This procedure is inconsistent with hiring decision procedures in the real world, where managers review and compare multiple resumes. The use of a non-representative sample and the low external validity imposes a limit on the generalizability of the results. In future, the procedure might be improved by conducting a field experiment where these resumes, containing either a White name, a Chinese name or a Malay name, are sent in response to job advertisements (Pager, Reference Pager2007). Subsequently, the responses (or the lack thereof) of the organizations could be used as an indicator of racism.

Those limitations notwithstanding, the current study has high internal validity. The differential perception and treatment can only be attributed to differences in the race of the applicant. Also, the study documented the differences in recommended salaries that provided a potential explanation for economic disparities. Given that salary packages in Singapore are often confidential and only offered to successful applicants, it is challenging for field experiments to estimate the economic implications of racism. Lastly, and more importantly, the study provided the first experimental evidence of racism and the Pinkerton syndrome in Singapore. Accordingly, the study initiated a resolution of the mixed findings between narrative reviews and quantitative research of racism in Singapore. Also, the study established the Pinkerton syndrome as an important social problem that needs to be resolved.

Future research directions include developing a measure to assess both overt and covert racism in Singapore. Such a measure has the potential to clarify the results found in the current study (i.e. non-applicability of the aversive racism framework vs. displays of overt racism). Second, future research could examine which of the two factors, colonial mentality or colorism, is primarily responsible for the Pinkerton syndrome. For example, researchers could manipulate the skin tone of the photographs of White and Chinese models and examine participants’ ratings of those photographs. The results have the potential to clarify whether the Pinkerton syndrome is caused by a preference for Whites or a preference for lighter skinned individuals. Lastly, given the negative effects of racism and the Pinkerton syndrome, there is pressing need for well-evaluated interventions to be documented and shared with researchers and policy makers. The goal of resolving the protracted economic disparities between Malays and the other races (Mutalib, Reference Mutalib2011) might come to fruition when the negative effects of those constructs are attenuated from the process of hiring decisions.

Author ORCIDs

Peter K. H. Chew 0000-0002-5243-1481

Financial Support

None.

Footnotes

1. The term is used in reference to the character B.F. Pinkerton, who married a Japanese girl for convenience but eventually left her for an American wife, in the opera Madame Butterfly.

References

Allport, G.W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Ashikari, M. (2005). Cultivating Japanese whiteness: The “whitening” cosmetics boom and the Japanese identity. Journal of Material Culture, 10, 7391. doi: 10.1177/1359183505050095 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
AsiaOne News. (2012, July 1). 2 arrested for making racist remarks online. Retrieved from http://news.asiaone.com/News/AsiaOne+News/Crime/Story/A1Story20120701-356542.html Google Scholar
Barr, M.D. (2006). Racialised education in Singapore. Educational Research for Policy and Practice, 5, 1531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, M.D. and Low, J. (2005). Assimilation as multiracialism: The case of Singapore’s Malays. Asian Ethnicity, 6, 161182. doi: 10.1080/14631360500226606 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrick, M.R., Swider, B.W. and Stewart, G.L. (2010). Initial evaluations in the interview: Relationships with subsequent interviewer evaluations and employment offers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 11631172. doi: 10.1037/a0019918 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertrand, M. and Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. American Economic Review, 94, 9911013. doi: 10.1257/0002828042002561 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloodhart, B.P. (2009). The subtle exploitation and patronization of women in masculine domains (Unpublished master’s dissertation). The Pennsylvania State University, USA.Google Scholar
Brondolo, E., Gallo, L.C. and Myers, H.F. (2009). Race, racism and health: Disparities, mechanisms, and interventions. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chew, P.K.H. (2018). Racism in Singapore: A review and recommendations for future research. Journal of Pacific Rim Psychology, 12, 19. doi: 10.1017/prp.2018.3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chin, Y. and Vasu, N. (2012). The ties that bind & blind: A report on inter-racial and inter-religious relations in Singapore. Singapore: Centre of Excellence for National Security, S. Rajaratnam School of International Studies, Nanyang Technological University. Retrieved from https://www.rsis.edu.sg/wp-content/uploads/2014/10/PB121231_The_Ties_that_Bind__Blind.pdf Google Scholar
Chua, B.H. (2003). Multiculturalism in Singapore: An instrument of social control. Race & Class, 44, 5877. doi: 10.1177/0306396803044003025 Google Scholar
David, E.J.R. and Okazaki, S. (2006a). Colonial mentality: A review and recommendation for Filipino American psychology. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 12, 116. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.12.1.1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, E.J.R. and Okazaki, S. (2006b). The Colonial Mentality Scale (CMS) for Filipino Americans: Scale construction and psychological implications. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53, 241252. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.2.241 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J.F. and Gaertner, S.L. (2000). Aversive racism and selection decisions: 1989 and 1999. Psychological Science, 11, 315319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, S.T., Cuddy, A.J., Glick, P. and Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878902.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaertner, S.L. and Dovidio, J.F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In Dovidio, J.F. and Gaertner, S.L. (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 6189). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gamst, G.C., Liang, C.T.H. and Der-Karabetian, A. (2011). Handbook of multicultural measures. USA: SAGE.Google Scholar
Hodson, G., Dovidio, J.F. and Gaertner, S.L. (2002). Processes in racial discrimination: Differential weighting of conflicting information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 460471. doi: 10.1177/0146167202287004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hudson, C. (2015). Dangerous sexuality in Singapore: The sarong party girl. In Koh, A. and Balasingamchow, Y.-M. (Eds.), Women and the politics of representation in southeast Asia (pp. 1632). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hunter, M. (2002). “If you’re light you’re alright”: Light skin color as social capital for women of color. Gender & Society, 16, 175193. doi: 10.1177/08912430222104895 Google Scholar
Hunter, M. (2007). The persistent problem of colorism: Skin tone, status, and inequality. Sociology Compass, 1, 237254. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9020.2007.00006.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katz, I. (1981). Stigma: A social psychological analysis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Koh, A. (2014). Doing class analysis in Singapore’s elite education: Unravelling the smokescreen of “meritocratic talk”. Globalisation, Societies and Education, 12, 196210. doi: 10.1080/14767724.2014.888308 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuah, K.E. (1997). Inventing a moral crisis and the Singapore state. Asian Journal of Women’s Studies, 3, 3670. doi: 10.1080/12259276.1997.11665786 Google Scholar
Leach, C.W. (2005). Against the notion of a “new racism”. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 15, 432445. doi: 10.1002/casp.841 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, W.K.M. (2004). The economic marginality of ethnic minorities: an analysis of ethnic income inequality in Singapore. Asian Ethnicity, 5, 2741. doi: 10.1080/1463136032000168880 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lepoer, B.L. (1989). Singapore: A country study. Washington: GPO for the Library of Congress. Retrieved from http://countrystudies.us/singapore/ Google Scholar
Li, E.P., Min, H.J., Belk, R.W. and Shalini Bahl, J.K. (2008). Skin lightening and beauty in four Asian cultures. In Lee, A.Y. and Duluth, D.S. (Eds.), NA-advances in consumer research (vol. 35, pp. 444449). Duluth, MN: Association for Consumer Research.Google Scholar
Lim, C. (1995). The Pinkerton syndrome. Asia Magazine, 33, 1012.Google Scholar
Mathews, M. (2013). Indicators of racial and religious harmony. Singapore: Institute of Policy Studies, National University of Singapore.Google Scholar
Mathews, M. (2016). Channel NewsAsia-Institute of Policy Studies (CNA-IPS) survey on race relations. Singapore: Institute of Policy Studies, National University of Singapore.Google Scholar
McConahay, J.B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the Modern Racism Scale. In Dovidio, J.F. and Gaertner, S.L. (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination, and racism (pp. 91125). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
McConahay, J.B. and Hough, J.C. (1976). Symbolic racism. Journal of Social Issues, 32, 2345. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1976.tb02493.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ministry of Education, Singapore. (2015). Graduate Employment Survey. Singapore: Ministry of Education. Retrieved from https://www.moe.gov.sg/docs/default-source/document/education/post-secondary/files/ntu.pdf Google Scholar
Moore, R.Q. (2000). Multiracialism and meritocracy: Singapore’s approach to race and inequality. Review of Social Economy, 58, 339360. doi: 10.1080/00346760050132364 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutalib, H. (2011). The Singapore minority dilemma. Asian Survey, 51, 11561171. doi: 10.1525/as.2011.51.6.1156 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ooi, G.L. (2005). The role of the developmental state and interethnic relations in Singapore. Asian Ethnicity, 6, 109120. doi: 10.1080/14631360500135336 Google Scholar
Pager, D. (2007). The use of field experiments for studies of employment discrimination: Contributions, critiques, and directions for the future. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 609, 104133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pager, D., Western, B. and Bonikowski, B. (2009). Discrimination in a low-wage labor market a field experiment. American Sociological Review, 74, 777799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradies, Y. (2006). A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. International Journal of Epidemiology, 35, 888901. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl056 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradies, Y., Ben, J., Denson, N., Elias, A., Priest, N., Pieterse, A.Gee, G. (2015). Racism as a determinant of health: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLOS ONE, 10, e0138511. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138511 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pieterse, A.L., Todd, N.R., Neville, H.A. and Carter, R.T. (2012). Perceived racism and mental health among Black American adults: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 59, 19. doi: 10.1037/a0026208 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singapore Department of Statistics. (2010). Census of Population 2010 Statistical Release 2: Households and Housing. Singapore: Author.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H. and Turner, J.C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Austin, S. and Austin, W.G. (Eds.), Psychology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 724). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.Google Scholar
Tan, K.P. (2008). Meritocracy and elitism in a global city: Ideological shifts in Singapore. International Political Science Review / Revue Internationale de Science Politique, 29, 727.Google Scholar
Teo, P. (2005). Mandarinising Singapore: A critical analysis of slogans in Singapore’s “Speak Mandarin” campaign. Critical Discourse Studies, 2, 121142. doi: 10.1080/17405900500283565 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The New Paper. (2008, May 21). Blogger arrested for posting racist online content. Retrieved from http://news.asiaone.com/News/AsiaOne+News/Singapore/Story/A1Story20080521-66273.html Google Scholar
The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore. Penal Code, 298 Chapter 224 § (1871).Google Scholar
The Statutes of the Republic of Singapore. Sedition Act, Chapter 290 § (1948).Google Scholar
Utsey, S.O., Abrams, J.A., Opare-Henaku, A., Bolden, M.A. and Williams, O. (2015). Assessing the psychological consequences of internalized colonialism on the psychological well-being of young adults in Ghana. Journal of Black Psychology, 41, 195220. doi: 10.1177/0095798414537935 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagatsuma, H. (1967). The social perception of skin color in Japan. Daedalus, 96, 407443.Google Scholar
Walsh, S.P. (2007). The roar of the lion city ethnicity, gender, and culture in the Singapore armed forces. Armed Forces & Society, 33, 265285. doi: 10.1177/0095327X06291854 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, D.R. and Mohammed, S.A. (2009). Discrimination and racial disparities in health: Evidence and needed research. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32, 2047. doi: 10.1007/s10865-008-9185-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Variation of elements in the resume to manipulate the strength of academic qualifications

Figure 1

Table 2. Means and standard deviations of perceived competence and applicant suitability, and recommended salary by academic qualifications (strong, moderate, or weak) and race (White, Chinese, or Malay) of the candidate

Figure 2

Table 3. Effects of race on perceived competence and applicant suitability, and recommended salary