Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T07:17:44.934Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Studying factual versus social cues as triggers of change in food behaviour

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 December 2024

Carolin V. Zorell*
Affiliation:
School of Humanities, Education and Social Sciences, Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden
Ansung Kim
Affiliation:
School of Hospitality, Culinary Arts and Meal Science, Örebro University, Grythyttan, Sweden
Nicklas Neuman
Affiliation:
Department of Food Studies, Nutrition and Dietetics, Uppsala University, Uppsala, Sweden
*
Corresponding author: Carolin V. Zorell; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Numerous public initiatives aim to influence individual food choices by informing about what is considered ‘healthy’, ‘climate-friendly’, and generally ‘sustainable’ food. However, research suggests that rather than public authorities, social influence is more likely to affect people’s behaviour. Using a randomised controlled trial, this study investigated if and how the two kinds of influences (factual versus social) could affect the real-life, self-reported intake of plant- and animal-based foods. In a four-month randomised controlled trial, a self-selected sample of adults living in Sweden (N = 237) tracked their daily food consumption several times per week using a tailored mobile phone app. Participants were randomised into one of three groups: two treatment groups receiving factual or social information about plant- and animal-based food consumption, or a control group receiving no information. Pre- and post-questionnaires provided additional background information about the participants. Participants’ food habits varied from week to week, and an explorative analysis pointed to a slight decrease in the consumption of animal-based food in the group that received social information. However, the longer-term patterns remained relatively constant in all groups, showing no substantial shift regardless of the kind of cues that the participants received. By investigating the roles of two common types of information about food and dietary change, the results contribute to discussions about how and by whom effective and efficient measures can be implemented to transform food habits. The results suggest there is limited potential for sustained and substantial behavioural changes through both social and factual information campaigns.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Introduction

Food habits are remarkably ingrained and resistant to change. Many public and private actors use information campaigns, public guidelines, and recommendations that rely on (scientific) facts as go-to strategies aiming to change people’s eating behaviours. They are relatively inexpensive and have the advantage of being non-intrusive to individual liberty. However, the effects are in many ways disappointing. For example, while the intake of (red) meat and other animal-based products, particularly beef, has been identified and publicly broadcast as a relevant factor for both negative health and environmental impacts,(Reference Clark, Springmann and Hill1,Reference Clark, Domingo and Colgan2) global consumption is increasing.(Reference Watts, Amann and Arnell3)

Given the strongly routinised and habitual nature of eating(Reference Warde4) and its centrality in tradition, social relations, and identity,(Reference Jönsson, Michaud and Neuman5) this is not surprising. Yet, it means that other strategies, rather than mere guidelines and recommendations, are needed if change is to be realised. One such solution could be to move from fact-based guidelines to exploiting human sociality. A broad range of research demonstrates that behavioural change, including eating patterns, is greatly affected by different forms of social influence.(Reference Smith and Christakis6,Reference Anderson Steeves, Johnson and Pollard7) Social norms, social facilitation, shared identities (i.e. social homophily, in-group identification, etc.), role models and ‘central persons’ are factors that could affect human behaviour,(Reference Banjeree, Chandrasekhar and Duflo8Reference Centola10) including whether, what, when and how much individuals eat.(Reference Higgs11Reference Zorell14) Furthermore, several studies suggest that social information, that is, cues about what others are doing, is more successful at making people adopt or change behaviours than ‘neutral’ facts and factual information. Examples include consumer goods such as music streaming,(Reference Salganik, Dodds and Watts15) energy consumption,(Reference Nolan, Schultz and Cialdini16) towel use in hotels,(Reference Reese, Loew and Steffgen17) and voter turnout.(Reference Bond, Fariss and Jones18)

Most of the studies about eating behaviour are short-term interventions that do a good job at isolating different explanatory variables, yet they say little about longer-term habits. Nor do they systematically explore the potential effects of multiple co-occurring social and informational influences — which is what tends to happen in day-to-day life. In this study, we present a randomised controlled trial (RCT) in which we incorporate such co-occurring influences and explore the comparative effects of factual versus social information on eating behaviour over a period of four months. We are guided by two research questions: (1) Do social cues (social information) affect the self-reported intake of food and, if so, under what conditions? (2) Do these effects differ from the (potential) effects of factual information (e.g. dietary guidelines)?

Our pre-established main hypothesis is that social information has a larger effect on change in individual eating behaviour than factual information.(Reference Zorell, Kim and Neuman19) To test this hypothesis, we study whether either of the respective kinds of information could make individuals change their self-reported intake of plant foods (increased) and animal-based foods (decreased). We are mainly inspired by the work of Damon Centola(Reference Centola10,Reference Centola20) who used artificial social network experiments in online communities and demonstrated that social influences can cause social change, i.e., change on aggregate, not just for individuals. More precisely, he showed that repeated social reinforcement is the most effective way of driving diffusion of health behaviour in social groups and networks.

Building on this, we compare the effect of social cues with cues based on health and dietary facts. We choose to focus on plant- versus animal-based foods as an example of a dietary change that is widely discussed and emphasised as necessary to tackling both contemporary health and ecological challenges.(Reference Watts, Amann and Arnell3) With this in mind, we aim to mimic a real-world everyday life setting and contribute original insights into the ongoing discussions of how and by whom effective and efficient measures could be implemented for achieving large-scale dietary change.

Methods

Study design and population

The study is based on a four-month RCT using a mixed experimental design (within- and between-subjects design) in Sweden from mid-October 2022 to early February 2023. We used a mobile phone app called ‘PAN Sweden’ (henceforth referred to as ‘the app’). The app was co-developed by Metabite (previously Wellness Foundry) and Wellmo, and tailored to our needs so that it served as both an assessment tool for dietary intake and an instrument of exposure. A four-month study period was chosen based on an assessment of academic discussions about the optimal balance between studying habit change and reasonable levels of risk for dropouts and declining adherence by study participants.(Reference Harray, Boushey and Pollard21Reference Wood and Rünger26)

We tested the method and app functionalities in a pilot study with a small group of people (n=23) during two weeks in the summer of 2022. Our primary aim was to try out the registration and randomisation procedures and the app functionalities, which permitted us to test the various measurement instruments and forms of treatment. Given that this pilot study was about functionalities, we had no intention of publishing our results.

Our study population comprises adults (≥18 years old) living in Sweden, a country with a wide supply of plant-based foods, public recommendations and guidelines to consume more fruits, vegetables and whole grains while limiting the intake of (red) meat, and a political rhetoric claiming to be a country at the forefront of environmental sustainability.(Reference Kristersson27) Nonetheless, meat consumption continues to be the standard and is high.(28Reference Strid, Hallström and Lindroos31) The participants were recruited on different websites and physical premises (e.g. university campuses), social media channels, and through interviews with the research team in a newspaper, a podcast and on public radio. We presented the study as being about the role of information and social interaction in affecting food habits. Anyone interested could then proceed to do a screening survey. The target sample size was determined based on an extensive literature review of similar kinds of studies, suggesting a target of around 100 people per group.(Reference Zorell, Kim and Neuman19)

In the screening survey, the respondents had to confirm their age and that they were neither following a specific diet for medical reasons nor suffering from disordered eating (self-diagnosed or diagnosed by a physician). If they fulfilled the requirements, after having provided their written informed consent, they were redirected to a pre-study questionnaire that collected sociodemographic information and information about their food consumption, focusing on animal- and plant-based protein sources. Upon completion, they were redirected to a separate website that gave them contact information for receiving an anonymised login to the app. The participants did not receive any monetary compensation. However, in week 11 we announced that we would be offering a gift as token of our gratitude, and those participants who remained actively involved until the end of the study received a voucher for two meat-substitute products available at supermarkets in Sweden. The Swedish Ethical Review Authority deemed the study exempt from ethical review (dnr: 2022-02646-01).

Exposure and assessment

The recruited participants were randomised to one of three groups using a digital die toss (www.random.org): (1) one treatment group in which the participants were exposed to factual information; (2) a second treatment group in which the participants were exposed to social information; and (3) a control group that received no information. In the app, the participants could post pictures and texts about the meals they ate and view one another’s posts within the group (but not across groups). Moreover, in all three groups network interaction was possible, meaning they could not only make posts themselves but also comment and ‘like’ each other’s activities. The structure of the design is outlined in Table 1.

Table 1. Outline of the study design

The factual information group received messages about the connection between diet and health or environmental impact based on the official Swedish dietary guidelines,(32) country-specific estimates of food-related greenhouse gas emissions,(Reference Röös33) and information about organic farming.(Reference Andersson, Jörgensen and Sidemo-Holm34) The social information group received descriptive messages about what the others in the group had eaten during the past week, for example:

Over the past few weeks, your group has reported reduced consumption of dairy products. The average participant reported around seven servings in week 4, around 6 in week 5, and around 5 in week 6.

For the social group, in the first two weeks data about past consumption in the group was not available yet (week 1) or slightly skewed (week 2). Therefore, the participants received pre-established messages about food consumption in the Swedish population based on data from the Swedish Food Agency and the Swedish Board of Agriculture (see Fig. 1). From week 3 onwards, we used the true consumption data of the group (e.g. the message on the right in Fig. 1). We did not focus exclusively on decreased consumption trends, but also on both increased consumption (e.g. week 5) and neutral trends (e.g. week 8). Several messages also considered consumption patterns over multiple weeks rather than only focusing on the past week. All messages were primarily about encouraging the consumption of plant foods, but not exclusively; for example, we included information about salt intake and healthy fats (see Table S1 in the Supplementary Material).

Fig. 1. Social messages from weeks 1 (left), 2 (middle), and 3 (right) (translated from Swedish). Source: The sources of information for weeks 1 and 2 were the Swedish Food Agency (2012) and the Swedish Board of Agriculture (2022).

Our intention was to mimic a real-world environment with varied information, yet plenty of (implicit) suggestions to increase plant- and decrease animal-based food intake (especially red meat). We thereby aimed to reduce the artificiality of the information flow, while also somewhat concealing the aim of the research. The data collection and subsequent analyses were not blinded to the conditions of the experiments.

The participants reported what they ate in the app by selecting portions of pre-established food categories. The food categories included in our analyses were dairy products, (red) meat, poultry, eggs, fish/shellfish, grains, legumes, fruits, starchy vegetables (e.g. potatoes), other vegetables, and nuts and seeds. For each of them, the size (i.e. gram) per portion was pre-established in the app. The participants could select as many portions per meal and food category as they had to reflect the actual amount of the food in their meal. They were asked to report a complete day’s intake at least two days per week (yet they could report as many as they wished). This was meant to balance regular reporting without overburdening them. To ensure continued participation, we sent two reminders to record food intake per week on a ‘rolling schedule’ (Tuesday/Friday, Wednesday/Saturday, etc.), so the reminders were not limited to potentially busy days. We thereby aimed to decrease the risk that the participants adjusted their food intake (e.g. eating ‘healthier’) on the days that the reminders were expected. Extra reminders were sent by SMS at mid-study time, right before New Year, and at study end (i.e. weeks 8, 11, and 16).

The study ended with a post-study questionnaire asking for the participants’ food consumption (same as the pre-study, plus questions about eating out and who usually decided what meals were eaten), as well as study involvement (e.g. perception of weekly reminders, following others in the group). This was collected for pre- and post-study comparisons, as well as to help assess interaction with other people and their posts in the app (see Table S2 in the Supplementary Material), degree of study compliance, and hidden variable biases. We also asked the participants to guess what the study was about. We did not reveal the purpose of the study in the questionnaire, but the participants had the option to contact us and find out about it after the study had ended.

Measurements and statistical analysis

Based on the daily meal reports, we calculate our main outcome in terms of the proportion of plant- to animal-based foods at baseline compared to the end of the trial. For reasons of analytic transparency, the main outcome has been presented in advance,(Reference Zorell, Kim and Neuman19) and is measured as the proportion of animal-based food consumption portions reported (dairy products, eggs, meat, poultry, and seafood) relative to plant food consumption portions (fruits, grains, legumes, starchy vegetables, non-starchy vegetables, and nuts and seeds) collected during the last 15 weeks of the 16-week trial. Data from week 1 is excluded due to the ongoing recruitment of participants and minor technical difficulties in the app that needed fixing.

We focus on proportions rather than absolute quantities since this outcome is less sensitive to increased/decreased amounts of food reported in total. For example, if a participant ate decreasing amounts of food in general over the study period, a decline in the total amount of meat reported could be wrongly interpreted as an exposure effect if we do not consider the general reported intakes. In addition, for both the main outcome variable and our secondary outcome (the number of portions reported), we use the sum of weekly food portions for each food category reported by a person, divided by the number of days recorded by the same person in each week.

The main results are initially assessed through a mixed-model repeated-measures ANOVA with unbalanced data. To explore the treatment effects on food intake, we compute the interaction effects between time (15 weeks) and treatment status (control and treatments), while the participants are applied as a random effect. Further analyses are conducted using profile analyses to determine the presence of patterns and group effects based on complete data (factual group: n = 10, social group: n = 14, control group: n = 12). Profile analysis uses techniques to identify, quantify and interpret the differences in score profiles among individuals or groups, thereby helping to examine and interpret levels or patterns of performance across multiple variables.(Reference Bulut and Desjardins35)

We also perform explorative analyses of intakes reported in the pre- and post-questionnaires using paired sample t-tests for each group. The mixed-model for repeated-measures ANOVA and paired sample t-tests are performed using XLstat (Addinsoft, New York, USA), whereas the profile analyses are conducted in R version 4.3.1 using the profileR package (version 0.3-5). The final selection of statistical techniques was developed in consultation with a professional statistician and differs from the original plan outlined in the study protocol.(Reference Zorell, Kim and Neuman19)

Results

Table 2 summarises the figures of the recruitment process: 328 received an invitation to download the app, 259 successfully registered, and 237 reported at least one meal. The distribution of dropouts at the beginning was even across the three groups, suggesting that the randomisation worked well. While lower than the targeted number of participants per group (i.e. 100), we reached about 80 individuals per group who reported two to three meals on two or more days per week. 16 participants actively dropped out, i.e., stating they wanted to withdraw from the study.

Table 2. Recruitment and participation in the study groups

Note: The ‘Invited’ column refers to participants who received log-in details for the app, while the ‘Accessed’ column refers to participants who successfully downloaded and logged in to the app.

The mean age of the participants who reported at least one meal was 42 (range: 20–74 years), and the average monthly income report is in line with the Swedish average (Table 3; see also Table S3 in the Supplementary Material). The sample is skewed in terms of gender and level of education with 76 per cent being female and 78 per cent reporting a college/university degree or higher. Nevertheless, the consumption of meat, poultry and other animal-based protein sources was surprisingly high.

Table 3. Characteristics of participants who reported a meal at least once

Note: All data are from the pre-questionnaire.

a Animal-based protein sources include beef, pork, lamb, poultry, seafood; plant-based protein sources include beans, peas, quinoa, etc; meat alternatives include vegetarian meat alternatives such as mycoprotein products and soya mince.

Seventy-three per cent of the participants categorised themselves as omnivores, 14 per cent as flexitarian, and only 5 per cent claimed to follow a vegetarian diet. The rate of a vegetarian diet is slightly higher than suggested for the Swedish population.(28) Vegan and pescatarian diets are followed by 4 per cent each, also higher than national estimates. At the same time, 51 per cent reported that they consumed animal-based protein sources five or more times per week, 21 per cent 3 or 4 times per week, and 13 per cent once or twice per week. More than half of the participants reported substituting meat with plant protein sources such as legumes and pulses on a weekly basis or more often, while 10 per cent never did this. In contrast, 22 per cent reported that they never used meat substitutes, while around 40 per cent reported using them on a weekly basis (most of them — 23 per cent — once or twice a week).

During the study, an average of 310 meals were logged per week. This includes a total of 6,039 meals reported in treatment group 1 (factual group), 4,487 meals in group 2 (social group), and 4,353 in the control group over 16 weeks. In the first weeks, reporting and posting of meals was strong across all groups. As the study progressed, activity decreased substantially over time and ensued slightly differently across the groups, as can be seen in Fig. 2 (see also Table S4 in Supplementary Material). At the study end, around 21 participants continued to regularly report their meals in each group.

Fig. 2. The number of participants who reported meals per week over time.

Main outcomes

The proportions of food consumption (in portions) of animal-based foods compared to plant-based foods over time are shown in Fig. 3. All three groups experienced some fluctuations over time, but there is no statistically significant effect at the group level (F(2, 159.4) = 2.694, P = 0.071) or at the week level (F(14, 1044.3) = 1.190, P = 0.277). No interaction effect between group and week can be observed (F(28, 1044.1) = 0.966, P = 0.516). Although the P-value does not reach conventional levels of significance, the F-statistic suggests some variability between groups. Therefore, an additional profile analysis with complete data is performed. But the results do not change (see Table S5 in the Supplementary Material).

Fig. 3. Proportion of animal-based foods (vs. plant-based foods) consumption over time (± standard error, SE).

Secondary and explorative outcomes

Despite the absence of significant effects on the proportions of food consumption, further analyses are performed on the absolute intake of animal-based and plant-based food over the study period. The mixed model for repeated measures ANOVA results reveal that, at the group level, there is no statistically significant difference in the consumption of either animal-based food consumption (F(2, 168.5) = 2.192, P = 0.115) or plant-based food consumption (F(2, 160.8) = 2.082, P = 0.128). However, statistically significant week effects can be observed for both animal-based (F(14, 1058.6)=2.029, P = 0.013) and plant-based foods (F(14, 1053.8) = 2.227, P = 0.006), indicating a significant difference across the weeks. No significant interactions in food consumption are detectable between group and week for both animal-based (F(28, 1058.5) = 0.840, P = 0.706) and plant-based foods (F(28, 1053.5) = 0.892, P = 0.628).

Further analyses with profiles reveal similar patterns of animal-based food consumption for all three groups. However, average animal-based food consumption (i.e. the portions) differ across weeks (F(14, 20) = 2.814, P = 0.017; see Table S6). This is graphically depicted in Fig. 4a and b: All three groups experienced fluctuations in their consumption of animal-based food (considering food log frequencies), with some peaking in the middle of the study period (around Christmas). The control group reported consistently higher amounts compared to the factual and social groups, ending at the level where it started (approx. 4.9 portions) after a stronger peak around the Christmas holidays. The two treatment groups, in turn, show a relatively flat pattern, though with a slight decline compared to the start of the study (social group: approx. 4.1 portions down to 3.7; factual group: approx. 3.9 portions down to 3.2).

Fig 4. Average food consumption portion divided by food log days per week (± standard error, SE). Graphs are shown for animal-based foods (a) and plant-based foods (b).

The consumption of plant-based food shows more volatility in all groups, including several peaks and dips. A profile analysis of plant-based food consumption validates this impression, showing comparable patterns across the three groups and an overall average that remained relatively consistent across weeks (F(14, 20) = 1.303, P = 0.287). However, the average plant-based food consumption differs at the group level (F(2,33) = 4.185, P = 0.024; see Supplementary Material, Table S7). Specifically, it was higher in the factual group compared to the other two groups and slightly decreased from the beginning to end of the study period, thus towards the end approaching the level of the other two groups. In summary, we find weekly fluctuation but no substantial longer-term changes within or across groups.

Lastly, we explore the frequencies of meat and plant-based food consumption in the pre- and post-questionnaires (not pre-established in the study protocol). The results are shown in Table 4. In the social group, the frequency of meat consumption decreased from ‘three to four times a week’ closer to ‘once or twice a week’ (4.2 and 3.8; P = 0.042). No other statistically significant changes occurred in the three groups, not even an increase in plant foods for the same group (social group) that reported lower follow-up numbers for meat, accounting for the decline in reporting over the weeks.

Table 4. Frequency of meat consumption and plant-based food consumption changes between pre- and post-questionnaires

Note: the P-value was calculated using a paired t-test. The figures represent means (SD). The answer scale in the questionnaire was: 1 = ‘Never’, 2 = ‘Less than once a week’, 3 = ‘Once or twice a week’, 4 = ‘Three or four times a week”, and 5 = ‘Five or more times a week’.

Discussion

To restore and sustain human and planetary health, a fundamental social change in diets is needed.(Reference Clark, Domingo and Colgan2,Reference Springmann, Clark and Mason-D’Croz36,Reference Herrero, Henderson and Havlík37) Public official(Reference Blomhoff, Andersen and Arnesen38) and unofficial dietary guidelines(Reference Willett, Rockström and Loken39) promote dietary transformation towards an increased proportion of (healthy) plant foods. The results of our four-month RCT with adults in Sweden suggest that both our treatment approaches — providing factual information or social cues in the form of the consumption of other group members — are unsuccessful in achieving this. There were indeed weekly fluctuations, indicating that some information was perhaps more effective than other. Yet, the first analyses did not reveal any statistically significant effects, and it is difficult to establish in this study whether observed fluctuations were attributable to a certain information message because participants could view previous messages accumulated as weekly tips in their app (thus, the effectiveness of a specific message could vary over time). Also, we can see a slight decrease in the consumption of animal-based food in the group that received social information, as found in the exploratory analysis. However, in general, the overarching picture is that the participants did not change their eating habits despite 16 weeks of regular external influence through the app.

Our results could be attributable to methodological problems such as the declining level of participation, or perhaps even due to an inadequate sample size from the outset. It could also be that messaging information on a weekly basis was not intense enough, that is, too infrequent or too mild, compared to, for example, giving the participants more alarming messages on a daily basis. However, we aimed to embed the factual messages into a similar context to what (Swedish) individuals are actually exposed to, and we wanted the social messages to be true. Another explanation could be the form of social influence. As Centola(Reference Centola10) has convincingly demonstrated, the complex contagion of behaviour is most effectively diffused through strong ties, but the interaction and influence of this study could only be considered weak. Also, the messages may have been too general, activating social norms but lacking the personalised impact necessary for (stronger) social effects. Incorporating injunctive norms, which signal approval of desirable behaviours, might have further strengthened the intervention by reinforcing positive actions among those participants who were already consuming less animal-based foods.(Reference Schultz, Nolan and Cialdini40)

The fact that the study collided with the holiday period around Christmas and New Year must also be acknowledged, as this time of year tends to centre on meat-intensive foods (popular Swedish holiday dishes include ham, meatballs, and fillet of beef). The data do suggest a minor holiday effect, partly in what was reported to be eaten, but even more by a lower level of activity. Nonetheless, we managed to re-activate the participants by sending them reminders, and the reported eating patterns once again regressed towards normality. This underlines the nature of eating as a strongly habituated and routinised practice, and it makes the results plausible.

With this in mind, the study makes a theoretical and methodological contribution. Great hopes of achieving large-scale dietary change and other forms of healthy or environmentally-friendly behaviours are often put in behaviour-focused ‘nudges’, which seem reasonably effective in the short term,(Reference Broers, De Breucker and Van Den Broucke41Reference Nørnberg, Houlby and Skov45) when embedding descriptive social norms in informational campaigns(Reference Nolan, Schultz and Cialdini16) or exposing people to social reinforcement diffusion through social networks.(Reference Centola9,Reference Centola10) Our results suggest that hope or belief in the power of social and informational mechanisms to induce lasting change should be toned down, at least as far as such ‘stubbornly’ persistent behaviours as everyday eating habits are concerned. As comparable developments in the control and treatment groups indicate, such influences can indeed make people choose differently at single moments, something that is supported by a wide array of (experimental) research.(Reference Higgs11Reference Zorell14,Reference Hackel, Coppin and Wohl46Reference Vartanian, Spanos and Herman48) However, the deeply habitual and routinised dietary patterns in everyday life outside the controlled laboratory setting appear to be substantially harder to influence by regular exposure to both factual and social information.

Having said that, our study reflects some common problems of longitudinal RCTs, facing decreasing activity among participants, and sample sizes that are both smaller and more demographically skewed than what would be optimal. However, the randomisation process seems to have worked very well, the hypothesis and the main outcome variables are clear, as are the treatments. The methodological approach and the behavioural mechanisms it can study are also not limited to questions about plant/animal foods or diets. The study design appears to be well replicable in our own validation studies and in studies of realms of behaviour, such as consumer goods purchasing, transportation, or exercising. Equally, our study may serve to validate previous studies with similar comparisons.(Reference Nolan, Schultz and Cialdini16Reference Bond, Fariss and Jones18,Reference Goldstein, Cialdini and Griskevicius49) Since this study is limited by the portion options available in the app — sufficient to test the behavioural hypothesis, but not to evaluate the nutritional or environmental metrics in detail — studies using more sophisticated measurement options can hopefully build on ours. It could also be extended socially, such as applying it to already existing networks with no anonymity, or groups that are anonymous but in which the networking opportunities are developed in ways that allow for more complex social interaction, thereby exploiting the power of strong ties.

Lastly, a unique methodological contribution is the use of the app as an instrument of both assessment and exposure. This type of technology has become very influential as a means of collecting data or offering behavioural ‘coaching’, for example, on physical activity and diets,(Reference Henderson and Gow50) but it is under-appreciated for its potential to also act as an experimental instrument for longitudinal behavioural treatment. This is what we have done and while greatly inspired by existing studies, we present a method that stands out in the literature. We have also been transparent about our hypotheses and planned outcomes of interest from our study protocol(Reference Zorell, Kim and Neuman19) in order to counter the serious issues of p-hacking and file drawer problems that are plaguing the sciences.(Reference Stefan and Schönbrodt51)

Conclusion

In conclusion, in our four-month RCT that tested the effects of factual information versus social cues on self-reported dietary behaviour we observe neither between- nor within-group differences over time. The pre- and post-study comparison hints at a possible effect of social information, but the explorative nature of this analysis warrants particular care in its interpretation. We believe that our results confirm the stubbornly persistent nature of eating behaviour as a strongly habituated, routinised, and symbolically meaningful practice. This provides insights into ongoing research that tries to discern ways of advancing social change to tackle current health and ecological crises. It suggests toning down hopes in the potential of ‘the informed social crowd’ to bring about substantial change. If this is true, socio-political energy will likely be more effectively placed in less ‘laissez-faire’ approaches. Furthermore, with our method of using a mobile phone app as an instrument of both behavioural assessment and exposure, we think we provide an important contribution to the broader field of health behaviour. We hope to inspire more social scientists to use the potential of RCTs to dive deeper into the causal mechanisms of individuals and societal behaviour change.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/jns.2024.82

Acknowledgements

The authors want to thank all the participants in the trial and the PAN Sweden consortium for their great support, as well as the developers of the mobile phone application which builds the heart of the trial, Metabite (formerly Wellness Foundry Oy) and Wellmo. The authors are also grateful for the valuable comments received on an earlier draft of our study idea from the participants in the panel ‘Policy diffusion for sustainability through local governments and institutions’ at the ECPR General Conference 2022. Last but not least, the authors are very thankful to our project colleagues Åsa Öström, Jun Niimi, and Stina Engelhart for their very valuable comments on the manuscript.

Financial support

The study is part of the research centre ‘PAN Sweden, plant-based proteins for health and wellbeing’ at Örebro University, Örebro, Sweden. The research centre is coordinated by Örebro University and financially supported by the Swedish research council FORMAS under grant no. 2020-02843. The authors furthermore received vouchers that were offered as gifts to participants from the Swedish agricultural cooperative Lantmännen, a non-academic partner of the research centre. Neither the centre managers, nor the funders, nor Lantmännen had in any way influence on the study design, material collection, data analysis, or publication of results.

Competing interests

The mobile phone application is a reconfigured version of the commercially available MealLogger PLUS app, produced by Metabite, formerly Wellness Foundry Oy. The company does not use the data for commercial purposes nor has it any commercial interests in the trial itself. They are, however, interested in the results to learn how their product is performing in scientific research. Moreover, we received vouchers from the cooperative Lantmännen for products of one of their brands, which were offered as symbolic gifts to the study participants who participated until the end of the study. This was not accompanied by any kind of advertising and the cooperative had no other kind of involvement or influence in the study.

Authorship

CVZ and NN developed the theory and designed the study. All three authors were involved in developing the methodology and experimental design. Data collection management, data curation, formal analysis, and visualisation were performed by AK. Project administrator is NN. In the original draft writing, all three authors were involved, while CVZ performed the final review and editing.

Ethics declaration

This study was conducted according to the guidelines laid down in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures involving research study participants were approved by the Swedish Ethical Review Authority, specifically, the authority deemed the study exempt from ethical review (dnr: 2022-02646-01). Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects.

References

Clark, MA, Springmann, M, Hill, J, et al. Multiple health and environmental impacts of foods. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2019;116:2335723362.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, MA, Domingo, NGG, Colgan, K, et al. Global food system emissions could preclude achieving the 1.5° and 2°C climate change targets. Science. 2020;370:705708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watts, N, Amann, M, Arnell, N, et al. The 2020 report of the lancet countdown on health and climate change: responding to converging crises. Lancet. 2020;397:129170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warde, A. The Practice of Eating. Cambridge: Polity Press; 2016.Google Scholar
Jönsson, H, Michaud, M, Neuman, N. What is commensality? A critical discussion of an expanding research field. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18:6235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, KP, Christakis, NA. Social networks and health. Annu Rev Sociol. 2008;34:405429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson Steeves, ET, Johnson, KA, Pollard, SL, et al. Social influences on eating and physical activity behaviours of urban, minority youths. Public Health Nutr. 2016;19:34063416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banjeree, AV, Chandrasekhar, AG, Duflo, E, et al. Using gossips to spread information: theory and evidence from two randomized controlled trials. Rev Econ Stud. 2019;86:24532490.Google Scholar
Centola, D. An experimental study of homophily in the adoption of health behavior. Science. 2011;334:12691272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centola, D. How Behaviour Spreads. The Science of Complex Contagions. Princeton and Oxford: Princeton University Press; 2018.Google Scholar
Higgs, S. Social norms and their influence on eating behaviours. Appetite. 2015;86:3844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, E, Thomas, J, Aveyard, P, et al. What everyone else is eating: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of informational eating norms on eating behavior. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2014;114:414429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruddock, HK, Brunstrom, JM, Vartanian, LR, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the social facilitation of eating. Am J Clin Nutr. 2019;110:842861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zorell, CV. Central persons in sustainable (food) consumption. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19:3139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salganik, MJ, Dodds, PS, Watts, DJ. Experimental study of inequality and unpredictability in an artificial cultural market. Sci (1979). 2006;311:854856.Google Scholar
Nolan, JM, Schultz, PW, Cialdini, RB, et al. Normative social influence is underdetected. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2008;34:913923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reese, G, Loew, K, Steffgen, G. A towel less: social norms enhance pro-environmental behavior in hotels. J Soc Psychol. 2014;154:97100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, RM, Fariss, CJ, Jones, JJ, et al. A 61-million-person experiment in social influence and political mobilization. Nature. 2012;489:295298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zorell, CV, Kim, A, Neuman, N. Diffusion of eating behaviour in different social networks: a study protocol and preliminary reflections from a randomized controlled trial. Res Square. Published February 7, 2023. doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-2541589/v1.Google Scholar
Centola, D. The spread of behavior in an online social network experiment. Sci (1979). 2010;329:11941197.Google Scholar
Harray, AJ, Boushey, CJ, Pollard, CM, et al. Perception v. actual intakes of junk food and sugar-sweetened beverages in australian young adults: assessed using the mobile food record. Public Health Nutr. 2017;20:23002307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurz, T, Gardner, B, Verplanken, B, et al. Habitual behaviors or patterns of practice? Explaining and changing repetitive climate-relevant actions. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Clim Change. 2015;6:113128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lally, P, Van Jaarsveld, CHM, Potts, HWW, et al. How are habits formed: modelling habit formation in the real world. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2010;40:9981009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shelton, RC, Lee, M, Brotzman, LE, et al. Use of social network analysis in the development, dissemination, implementation, and sustainability of health behavior interventions for adults. Soc Sci Med. 2019;220:81101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shoneye, CL, Dhaliwal, SS, Pollard, CM, et al. Image-based dietary assessment and tailored feedback using mobile technology: mediating behavior change in young adults. Nutrients. 2019;11:114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, W, Rünger, D. Psychology of habit. Annu Rev Psychol. 2016;67:289314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kristersson, U (2023) National statement at COP28, Dubai. Dubai.Google Scholar
Swedish Food Agency. Riksmaten vuxna – 2010–11: Livsmedels- och Näringsintag Bland Vuxna i Sverige. Uppsala: Swedish Food Agency; 2012.Google Scholar
Hallström, E, Davis, J, Håkansson, N, et al. Dietary environmental impacts relative to planetary boundaries for six environmental indicators – a population-based study. J Clean Prod. 2022;373:133949.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strid, A, Hallström, E, Lindroos, AK, et al. Adherence to the Swedish dietary guidelines and the impact on mortality and climate in a population-based cohort study. Public Health Nutr. 2023;26:23332342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swedish Food Agency. Mat och Dryck. Uppsala: Swedish Food Agency; 2022.Google Scholar
Röös, E. Mat-klimat-listan Version 1.1. Uppsala: Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences; 2014.Google Scholar
Andersson, S, Jörgensen, C, Sidemo-Holm, W, et al. Är ekologisk odling bättre för miljön? Fokus. 2021;5:119.Google Scholar
Bulut, O, Desjardins, CD. Profile analysis of multivariate data: a brief introduction to the profileR package. PsyArXiv. Published February 10, 2020. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/sgy8m.Google Scholar
Springmann, M, Clark, M, Mason-D’Croz, D, et al. Options for keeping the food system within environmental limits. Nature. 2018;562:519525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrero, M, Henderson, B, Havlík, P, et al. Greenhouse gas mitigation potentials in the livestock sector. Nat Clim Chang. 2016;6:452461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blomhoff, R, Andersen, R, Arnesen, EA, et al. Nordic Nutrition Recommendations 2023. Integrating Environmental Aspects. Copenhagen: Nordic Council of Ministers; 2023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willett, W, Rockström, J, Loken, B, et al. Food in the anthropocene: the EAT–lancet commission on healthy diets from sustainable food systems. Lancet. 2019;393:447492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, PW, Nolan, JM, Cialdini, RB, et al. The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power of social norms. Psychol Sci. 2007;18:429434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broers, VJV, De Breucker, C, Van Den Broucke, S, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effectiveness of nudging to increase fruit and vegetable choice. Eur J Public Health. 2017;27:912920.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cadario, R, Chandon, P. Which healthy eating nudges work best? A meta-analysis of field experiments. Mark Sci. 2020;39:465486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harbers, MC, Beulens, JWJ, Rutters, F, et al. The effects of nudges on purchases, food choice, and energy intake or content of purchases in real-life food purchasing environments: a systematic review and evidence synthesis. Nutr J. 2020;19:103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehner, M, Mont, O, Heiskanen, E. Nudging – a promising tool for sustainable consumption behaviour? J Clean Prod. 2016;134:166177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nørnberg, TR, Houlby, L, Skov, LR, et al. Choice architecture interventions for increased vegetable intake and behaviour change in a school setting: a systematic review. Perspect Public Health. 2016;136:132142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hackel, LM, Coppin, G, Wohl, MJA, et al. From groups to grits: social identity shapes evaluations of food pleasantness. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2018;74:270280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, N, Story, M. A review of environmental influences on food choices. Ann Beh Med. 2009;38:5673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vartanian, LR, Spanos, S, Herman, CP, et al. Modeling of food intake: a meta-analytic review. Soc Influ. 2015;10:119136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, NJ, Cialdini, RB, Griskevicius, V. A room with a viewpoint: using social norms to motivate environmental conservation in hotels. J Cons Res. 2008;35:472482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, MJ, Gow, ML. A scoping review of health promotion interventions delivered via social media to women of reproductive age. Publ Health Nutr. 2023;26:31733189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stefan, AM, Schönbrodt, FD. Big little lies: a compendium and simulation of p-hacking strategies. R Soc Open Sci. 2023;10:220346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Outline of the study design

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Social messages from weeks 1 (left), 2 (middle), and 3 (right) (translated from Swedish). Source: The sources of information for weeks 1 and 2 were the Swedish Food Agency (2012) and the Swedish Board of Agriculture (2022).

Figure 2

Table 2. Recruitment and participation in the study groups

Figure 3

Table 3. Characteristics of participants who reported a meal at least once

Figure 4

Fig. 2. The number of participants who reported meals per week over time.

Figure 5

Fig. 3. Proportion of animal-based foods (vs. plant-based foods) consumption over time (± standard error, SE).

Figure 6

Fig 4. Average food consumption portion divided by food log days per week (± standard error, SE). Graphs are shown for animal-based foods (a) and plant-based foods (b).

Figure 7

Table 4. Frequency of meat consumption and plant-based food consumption changes between pre- and post-questionnaires

Supplementary material: File

Zorell et al. supplementary material

Zorell et al. supplementary material
Download Zorell et al. supplementary material(File)
File 36.3 KB